SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 10
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
Designing for Collaborative Creative Problem Solving
Otmar Hilliges, Lucia Terrenghi, Sebastian Boring, David Kim, Hendrik Richter, Andreas Butz
Media informatics, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Munich
Amalienstrasse 17, 80333 Munich, Germany
otmar.hilliges|lucia.terrenghi|sebastian.boring|andreas.butz@ifi.lmu.de,
kimdav|richterh@cip.ifi.lmu.de
ABSTRACT
Collaborative creativity is traditionally supported by formal
techniques, such as brainstorming. These techniques im-
prove the idea-generation process by creating group syner-
gies, but also suffer from a number of negative effects [12].
Current electronic tools to support collaborative creativity
overcome some of these problems, but introduce new ones,
by either losing the benefits of face-to-face communication
or the immediacy of simultaneous contribution.
Using an interactive environment as a test bed, we are in-
vestigating how collaborative creativity can be supported
electronically while maintaining face-to-face communica-
tion. What are the design-factors influencing such a sys-
tem?
We have designed a brainstorming application that uses an
interactive table and a large wall display, and compared the
results of using it to traditional paper-based brainstorming
in a user study with 30 participants. From the considera-
tions that went into the design and the observations during
the study we derive a number of design guidelines for col-
laborative systems in interactive environments.
Author Keywords
Collaborative creative problem solving, brainstorming, in-
teractive environments, tabletop displays, wall displays,
large displays
ACM Classification Keywords
H5.m. Information interfaces and presentation (e.g., HCI):
Miscellaneous, H.5.3 Group and Organization Interfaces:
Organizational design
INTRODUCTION
Collaborative problem solving requires much more than
simply joining work forces. Knowledge and information
need to be exchanged, different skills have to be coordi-
nated, and the information communicated by others needs
interpretation so that new ideas can be created and new
solutions can be found. This process – with its core re-
quirements of communication, coordination and interpreta-
tion – is called collaborative creative problem solving [1].
In Fischer [16] creativity occurs in the relationship between
an individual and a society, and between an individual and
his or her technical environment. Appropriate socio-
technical settings can amplify the outcome of a group of
people by both augmenting individual creativity and multi-
plying rather than simply summing up individual output.
Physical, social and interaction contexts thus play an impor-
tant role in guiding cognitive processes.
In a ubiquitous computing scenario where technology
blends in with the environment, we observe a paradigm
shift from Human-Computer Interaction to computer-
mediated human-to-human interaction. With the introduc-
tion of large, interactive, high-resolution displays built into
walls and tables, we face for the first time the challenge,
and the opportunity, to design socio-technical systems
which not only support collaboration but also mediate and
foster human-to-human communication and interaction.
Current computer systems already offer a variety of com-
munication channels for distributed collaboration (e.g., in-
stant messaging, e-mail, online communities, groupware)
and support for collaborative work (CSCW). However, im-
portant parts of our professional and personal life still de-
pend on co-located collaboration and face-to-face commu-
nication, with all the nuances of facial expression and body
language, and the immediacy of verbal communication.
Visibility of action is a fundamental aspect of group aware-
ness [13]. Shared displays that support simultaneous input
afford novel communication patterns as well as social pro-
tocols, but the still predominantly used WIMP paradigm
has poorly supported such social contexts thus far.
In face-to-face collaborative creative problem-solving set-
ups, technology is very often absent or shut down because it
is considered disruptive to communication and the creative
flow [37]. Using single-user systems in a collaborative set-
ting leads, in most cases, to a communication breakdown
since the user’s concentration has to shift away from the
group and towards the computer in order to use it. Rather
than relying on technology, these meetings still rely on the
physical and social benefits of using surfaces such as tables
and walls to exchange and visualize different types of in-
formation (paper documents, presentations, pictures, etc.)
and the different collaboration behaviors implied therein.
Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for
personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are
not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies
bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. To copy otherwise, or
republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific
permission and/or a fee.
C&C’07, June 13–15, 2007, Washington, DC, USA.
Copyright 2007 ACM 978-1-59593-712-4/07/0006...$5.00.
137
Interactive surfaces offer new possibilities for the design of
socio-technical systems that can partly exploit the physical
and social affordances of a traditional face-to-face collabo-
rative environment and at the same time benefit from the
affordances of digital technology such as persistent data
storage, easy information access, and the possibility to re-
view previous processes or to undo certain actions.
Building on an analysis of the factors that influence the
brainstorming process in manual and electronic settings, we
postulate the design goals for socio-technical systems that
mediate creative group processes. We present a brainstorm-
ing system as an example of a socio-technical environment
that supports co-located collaborative creativity and our
results from the evaluation of this implemented system. In
designing and evaluating the system we studied how several
interactive displays, each having distinct roles and functions
in the process, influenced co-located collaboration. In con-
clusion and building on previous work in the field, we pro-
pose new design considerations to meet the requirements of
future collaborative ubiquitous computing systems.
COLLABORATIVE CREATIVE PROBLEM SOLVING
Brainstorming [30] is a technique for divergent thinking. It
can be individual, although the term more often refers to a
group process for generating as many ideas or options as
possible in response to an open question. Thus, it is fre-
quently used for collaborative creative problem solving and
it builds on a few main principles: quantity over quality of
ideas, elaboration on others’ ideas and absence of criticism.
The technique relies on the communication among group
members to stimulate idea generation, and on coordination
to maximize the individuals’ involvement and interpretation
of ideas in order to create new intellectual associations. In
this context the physical and social affordances of surfaces
play an important role (e.g. a table for idea generation, a
wall or whiteboard for idea discussion, paper for idea ex-
pression and recording). These properties of brainstorming
qualify it as a prime example for the creative problem solv-
ing group processes we want to investigate.
Osborn [30] anticipated positive synergy effects of such a
technique, which affect the productivity of ideas. Other
studies [12] show that these factors are apparently out-
weighed by several negative social implications of the tech-
nique (see Figure 1). They have shown that nominal brain-
storming groups (aggregating ideas from separate individu-
als) outperform face-to-face groups. The main reasons for
this are losses in productivity through production blocking,
social loafing and evaluation apprehension [2].
In addition to these findings later studies have shown that
groups using Electronic Brainstorming Systems (EBS) ap-
pear to outperform both manual and nominal brainstorming
groups [5, 10, 12]. The main reasons (see Figure 1) for in-
creased productivity are parallelism, to overcome produc-
tion blocking, and anonymity, to reduce evaluation appre-
hension. Even if anonymity bears the danger of social loaf-
ing [23], its benefits appear to outweigh the losses.
Figure 1: Factors influencing brainstorming productivity.
Although EBS produce more ideas and can track the proc-
ess better than manual brainstorming, the original technique
(usually paper-based) is still ubiquitous in professional life
and has not yet been replaced by EBS, as a recent study
shows [11]. Why doesn’t a superior technology replace the
inferior one? The answer to this has several aspects and not
all of them are grounded in the qualities of the technique
itself. The number and quality of ideas is not the only value
to be assessed.
Since group processes involve individuals, the individuals’
subjective perception of the process plays an important role.
The perceived quality of the outcome itself depends on the
degree to which personal interests are represented and val-
ued in the group’s output. Second, the face-to-face situation
of manual brainstorming has qualities which, in the long
run, might even outweigh pure productivity measurements,
namely the positive social aspects of team building, group
awareness and a shared sense of achievement.
If technology is either disruptive for an individual to ex-
press her/himself, or for a group to communicate in a face-
to-face situation, EBS won’t maintain those values that
people perceive in using the manual brainstorming tech-
nique, despite other quantifiable benefits such as productiv-
ity of ideas and storage provided by remote EBS.
Considering the tradeoff (see Table 1) between technique
performance and social implications of EBSs we wanted to
build a system that maintains the empirically proven advan-
tages of EBS while combining them with the social advan-
tages of manual face-to-face brainstorming. Hence we sac-
rificed the anonymity of group members (which is expected
to lessen evaluation apprehension) in favor of the social
implications of group awareness and personal communica-
tion. We deliberately accepted that this decision might de-
crease the number of ideas generated because we expect
that it improves the subjective perception of quality and
hence the acceptance of the whole process. In addition, we
are mostly interested in co-located collaboration since we
want to generalize our findings to other collaborative crea-
tivity scenarios more complex than this particular one.
138
PC-
based
face-to
face
Electronic
remote
Manual Electronic
face-to-
face
Synergy
Group
awareness -
Anonymity - - -
Parallelism - -
Reduced soc.
loafing - -
Reduced
production
blocking
- -
Reduced
evaluation
apprehension
- - -
Table 1: Properties of different technologies regarding brain-
storming productivity and group processes
DESIGN GOALS
We aimed to design a socio-technical environment which
positively affects collaborative creative problem solving.
For the support of such a group process we tried to enhance
both the divergent as well as the convergent thinking (i.e.,
generation and elaboration of ideas), which are typical and
essential activities in problem solving. Thus, we considered
the affordances of technologically enriched environments.
In order to facilitate both generation and selection of ideas,
we used large interactive surfaces in the environment. The
horizontal plane of a table affords writing, face to face
communication, territoriality [35], and group awareness,
while a wall display allows and supports shared visualiza-
tion, overview and context awareness. The combination of
these interactive surfaces opens interesting possibilities for
the design of a socio-technical environment.
The issue of more fluid interaction with large, high-
resolution displays that support creative group processes
has been treated in [19, 21, 26, 39]. A summary of recent
advances in the field of interaction techniques for large dis-
plays can be found in Czerwinski et al. [8]. Most of this
research has focused either on the properties and design
implications for vertical large displays [32] or on the influ-
ence of horizontal displays on co-located collaborative
work [34], thus mostly focusing on a specific type of dis-
play. We take a more ecological, holistic approach by look-
ing at the combination of different displays across the proc-
ess, both in time and space.
Immediacy of Communication and Interaction
Our main goal was to combine some of the benefits of pre-
vious EBS with the positive social implications of face-to-
face groups. First, we wanted to avoid production block by
guaranteeing true parallelism of input such that every mem-
ber of the group could contribute at any time. Additionally,
we wanted to store every idea and make it permanently
available such that it can be read and interpreted by every
member at all times.
We also wanted to minimize the costs of interaction and
communication so that even in small groups the synergy
effects can outweigh the losses. This can be achieved by
two complementing measures: first, by blending the com-
puter into the environment in which the collaborative crea-
tive processes take place, and second, by blending the vir-
tual interface into the task so that knowing the craft (or
technique) reduces the cost of learning and using the sys-
tem. Thus, essential elements of the interface should behave
just like their counterparts in the manual process.
In this way we hope to avoid some of the factors that seem
to hinder the communication process [2], e.g., the size of a
personal computer screen or the keyboard as a disruptive
interface in this context.
Enhance Phrasing
The difference between chunking and phrasing is explained
in more depth by Buxton [4]. Different representations of
the task and different levels of skills between novices and
experts imply different granularity of detail at which people
approach the solution of a problem. The more novice the
user, the lower the level at which s/he approaches the prob-
lem. This results in chunking the main goal into sub-goals,
to be achieved through sub-tasks. The acquisition of skills
enhances the automatic performance of some sub-tasks,
thus achieving the high-level goals faster and more easily
(phrasing). To enhance phrasing (i.e., the undisrupted flow
of action and creativity), the task needs to be represented in
a way that supports chunk maximization. Indeed, the bigger
the chunks are, the less cognitive resources are wasted. In
this sense we aim at an interface which allows a fluid and
immediate interaction with the task at hand. Furthermore,
the interface needs to support the externalization of the
chunks so as to allow reflection and association.
Minimize Cognitive Load
The human capability of keeping chunks of information in
short-term memory is very limited [28]. Thus, we need to
design the context (e.g., the representation of the task) so as
to minimize the use of cognitive resources for holding acti-
vated bundles in short term memory. Typical techniques for
freeing cognitive resources are externalization, e.g., through
the use of space, epistemic action [24, 25] and visual output
as well as spatial mapping [29, 22]. Thus more resources
remain available for creative associations.
139
Mediate Mutual Association Activation
When a person is exposed to stimuli from a variety of con-
texts s/he is more likely to have novel associations [33].
The activation of such associations can be automatic (with-
out intentional conscious awareness), or depend upon the
context of the stimuli (conscious capacity spreading activa-
tion, [3]). This suggests that the design of a context of in-
teraction can affect the association patterns. Furthermore, it
suggests that the context of interaction can stimulate activa-
tion patterns that would otherwise be unlikely in the auto-
matic spreading activation. Thus, with a socio-technical
environment which positively affects collaborative creativ-
ity, we hope to create a context that supports the explora-
tion of different areas of our knowledge network.
In order to enable such a process, it is important that people
can perceive each other’s ideas and communicate about
them. We think that providing different visual cues about
the generated ideas on the table and on the wall, as well as
giving users the possibility to explicitly exchange ideas
(and the visual representation thereof) can enable and foster
the generation of new unexpected associations.
Supporting Group Awareness and Overview
Visibility of action is a main design principle for embodied
interaction [14]. It provides awareness of what other col-
leagues are doing and how the actions of group members
affect the shared artifacts and relies on existing theories
from CSCW [13]. Group awareness (i.e. the condition
where members perceive the presence of other group mem-
bers and the possibility to communicate with them), seems
to provide a basis for informal communication. By giving
every group member at any time the possibility to under-
stand what other members are doing, the isolation of single
individuals is avoided. Mutual visibility of actions eases the
coordination and interpretation of the contributors’ actions.
DESIGN CHOICES
By our design choices we try to merge some of the advan-
tages of traditional techniques for face-to-face problem
solving, with some of the benefits of the technology em-
bedded in the interactive surfaces of the environment.
In order to do so, we designed a multi-user application for
supporting co-located collaborative problem solving, which
together with the users forms a socio-technical environment
resembling a face-to-face meeting situation. This means
that the system doesn’t have any standard computer moni-
tors or input devices but solely relies on interactive, touch
sensitive displays built into the meeting room’s table and
wall (see Figure 2). Presuming that display technology will
be cheap and distributed enough to pervade our working
environments in the near future, we explore the potential of
a socio-technical environment as ecosystem, by analyzing
how communication and spatial mapping can be supported.
Thus, we do not focus on the real estate and properties of
one specific display in isolation, but rather on the combina-
tion of displays and on the question how the relationships
between the different surfaces affect the group process.
Figure 2: Our instrumented room with wall and table displays
In this setup we developed an application, which meta-
phorically builds on the “idea card” method, i.e., the use of
Post-its for brainwriting. Geschka [17] and VanGundy [42]
developed the Interactive Brainwriting Pool Technique. In
this method group members write their ideas on a piece of
paper that is then placed in the center of the table for an-
other member to read prior to writing their next comment.
The use of Post-its during brainwriting has become a rather
common practice in collaborative problem solving. They
afford the recording of ideas in written rather than just ver-
bal form in the generative phase. Furthermore, they support
a certain territoriality and the creation of semantic regions.
When participants are given a stack of post-its and start
sticking them around their working area, they define their
personal region, which remains visible to others, thus creat-
ing a mutual awareness among participants.
Using Post-its on surfaces supports the convergent thinking
phase as well, when participants stick and move Post-its on
flip charts or white boards in order to recognize patterns
and create clusters. In this phase they structure the devel-
oped ideas into more meaningful concepts and try to iden-
tify relations between them. The affordances of paper have
been more deeply discussed in [36, 7, 18]. Studies of paper
in work practice show that paper continues to be widely
used for many reasons including its spatial flexibility (it can
be quickly arranged in physical space), sociability (it facili-
tates face-to-face communication), and tailorability (it is
easily annotated) [7]. Furthermore [18] explains that some
of the reasons why designers use paper rather than elec-
tronic communication is for its “friendliness”, immediacy
and affordances for face-to-face communication of ideas.
To this respect we opted for a pen based input to support
the fluid generation of ideas with handwriting. A limited
gesture vocabulary was designed and implemented. Users
can start generating ideas by drawing a square on the table
surface. This event triggers the appearance of a large yellow
square, resembling a Post-it, thus defining the area to write
in (see Figure 3). By tipping a designated area of the Post-
140
it, the latter shrinks to a smaller size and becomes moveable
(see Figure 4). The user can then create new Post-its/ideas
by drawing new squares in a blank region of the table and
writing within the yellow region. This choice was made in
order to create visual constraints for writing, so as to iden-
tify ideas as units, and to create visual cues for distinguish-
ing territories and patterns. When the Post-it is shrunk its
content is still readable.
Figure 3: Creating a Post-it and writing on it
The choice of using handwriting and gestures to enhance
fluid interaction and the group process was also made in
Guimbretière et al. [20]. Furthermore Buxton [4] examine
the effect of compound tasks on the users’ cognitive load.
These are tasks that usually can be expressed in one sen-
tence (e.g. write text onto a post-it) but have to be broken
down into multiple steps in standard desktop applications
(e.g. select target, choose text tool, type text). This may
result in additional cognitive burden on top of the actual
task (idea generation). Kinesthetic gestures can overcome
such problems by mapping to whole phrases rather than
functions of the underlying system. The desired one-to-one
correspondence between concept and gesture leads to inter-
faces which are more compatible with the users’ mental
model. Our design supports simultaneous, gesture-based
interaction, as well as the mutual visibility of action. Direct
manipulation in this context refers to a coincident spatial
mapping of input and output (there is no such device as a
pointer or a remote controller). This creates a transparent
causal relationship between gestures and output, and sup-
ports visibility of gestures. Furthermore, it allows the crea-
tion of temporal spatial structures, which are fundamentals
to epistemic actions [25]. Creative processes highly rely on
epistemic actions, as these allow the externalization and
visualization of different alternatives [24]. The size of the
table (see Figure 2) allows participants to see each other’s
actions, movements and gestures in real time. It also sup-
ports a sense of common ground by displaying the results of
both participants’ interactions on a single shared display.
Thus, both controls and resources (i.e., the different Post-
its) are continuously available to both participants.
Building on these considerations we designed the Post-its in
such a way that they can be edited, moved, deleted and cop-
ied by any participant after they have been created. Addi-
tionally we created a mechanism to encourage building on
each other’s ideas: With a quick movement of the pen, each
user can deliberately skid one idea to the other participant.
The Post-it slides quickly across the table and smoothly
reorients itself towards the other user. This supports the
explicit sharing of ideas and thus encourages the creation of
association chains.
Figure 4: Dragging a Post-it and skidding it
The immediate and visible change of the shared visual
landscape is supported by the system in additional ways. As
the participants create Post-its in their working area, thus
already creating a distinct territorial setup, the Post-its ap-
pear simultaneously on the vertical display, which is lo-
cated next to the table. On the vertical display the Post-its
are reoriented upright, i.e., readable for both readers, but
they maintain a spatial mapping to the territorial setup on
the table display. In this sense the perception of territoriality
and group awareness are supported. A participant will rec-
ognize his/her own “territory” on the wall, but at the same
time gain an overview of the ideas created by the group.
When users tip the Post-it in the middle, where a small grid
is displayed (Figure 3), they can enlarge it again to edit its
content. When they instead tip its periphery (Figure 4) and
drag the pen, they move the Post-it, both on the horizontal
and the vertical display. When users move from the table
(generative phase, divergent thinking) to the wall display
(structural phase, convergent thinking), they can spatially
organize the ideas by rearranging them on the wall. In addi-
tion they can create clusters by drawing a circle around
some Post-its (Figure 5). Clusters are merged by dragging
them close together. Drawing a cross on the border of a
cluster causes it to dissolve into single Post-its again. Clus-
ters can be connected to each other or to single Post-its by
drawing a line from the border of one cluster to the border
of another one (Figure 5) or to the center of a Post-it.
Finally, whole clusters can be moved across the display,
thus moving all the Post-its they contain. This set of cluster-
ing techniques clearly extends the functionality of a physi-
cal whiteboard or flip chart while it maintains the direct
manipulation characteristics thereof, facilitating the creation
of a structured knowledge representation, which is easily
editable by every participant through direct manipulation.
141
Figure 5: Creating and connecting clusters.
EVALUATION
We implemented our brainstorming system in order to ver-
ify whether its design meets the initial design goals and to
determine which effect our design choices had on the crea-
tive process, its objective outcome and the subjective feel-
ing of productivity of the participants. The evaluation of
subjective experiences is an open topic for the ubiquitous
computing community, especially when the aim is to assess
an experience involving different displays, people, and
temporal phases. For the assessment of a single users’ sub-
jective experience we opted for questionnaires. The col-
laborative creative process and its outcome are even harder
to measure quantitatively. Different approaches for measur-
ing collaborative creativity processes in novel interaction
settings have been proposed in the literature.
Conversational analysis has been previously used to assess
collaboration. Tatar et al. [40] apply techniques from Psy-
cholinguistics to evaluate the conversational patterns of a
system supporting co-located brainstorming. Damm et al.
[9] videotaped, observed and interpreted several sessions of
a collaborative software design process. The observations
allowed qualitative statements about the different designs
studied, but the process didn't produce any quantitative
data. Consolvo et al. [6] provide a good overview of rele-
vant known study and evaluation techniques and discuss
their applicability to ubiquitous computing settings. For
quantitative evaluations, they propose Lag Sequential
Analysis (LSA) and describe how it was applied to their
environment. LSA relies on the idea of logging and count-
ing relevant events in the environment and generates statis-
tical data about the observed process. In our evaluation we
logged the creation and exchange of ideas as the basic
events in the creativity process.
Pinelle et al. [31] propose the use of task analysis for the
evaluation of collaborative processes. They propose a hier-
archical task model and describe its application in a tech-
nique called collaboration usability analysis. The structure
of the brainstorming task is relatively simple and in our
study we only identified whether ideas were independently
generated, built on an own previous idea, or resulted from
seeing or discussing ideas of the brainstorming partner. Van
der Lugt [43] specifically recorded how people built on
each other's ideas in a brainstorming process and generated
link diagrams from this data. From the link diagrams he
then generated statistical data about the number and type of
connections between ideas. We found in the analysis of our
videotapes, that it was often not possible to identify all ear-
lier ideas which might have contributed to the creation of a
particular new one and that the attempt to do so would be
very speculative and error-prone. Therefore, we only identi-
fied chains of ideas, which directly built on each other (as-
sociation chains), and evaluated their size and number in
order to obtain statistical data.
Subjects
The study we conducted consisted of 30 participants in 15
teams of two subjects each. To avoid gender bias we had 5
pure male, 5 mixed and 5 pure female teams. Some of the
subjects knew each other while others had never met be-
fore. Among the participants a variety of professions was
present, such as computer science students, architects, de-
signers, civil engineers, musicologists and journalists. All in
all we had participants from 5 different nations.
Environment
The system was deployed in an instrumented environment
containing an interactive meeting table as well as displays
embedded into an interactive wall (see Figure 2). Both the
table and the wall display were included in our electronic
brainstorming system.
The interactive table consists of an LCD monitor embedded
into a wooden table and is equipped with a DViT overlay
panel [38] (a vision based tracking system providing multi-
ple simultaneous inputs) for interactivity. Hence, the par-
ticipants share an overall table space of 1.6 x 1.2 meters.
The complete wall is an interactive surface with a width of
5 meters and a height of 2.5 meters containing three back-
projected displays. The two side displays as well as the rest
of the wall are tracked by four cameras. The center display
additionally provides high precision input through another
DViT panel.
While applying the paper-based technique, the teams used
the same table covered with paper in the idea generation
phase and a large (1.8 x 1.5 meters) piece of paper,
mounted on the opposite wall, in the structural phase.
Tasks and Procedure
We conducted a within-group comparative study between
our system and the original paper-based manual brainstorm-
ing technique to assess the brainstorming productivity of
both techniques. Furthermore, we ran questionnaires before
and after the task as a qualitative user study to evaluate the
subjective perception of and judgments about our system.
We also assessed the subjective judgments of the brain-
storming results in these questionnaires. Each session took
approximately one hour and included the two question-
naires, introductions to the technique and the interface, as
well as one warm-up task. The participants received two
different but related tasks. Each of these tasks had to be
executed in a different technique. The order of the tech-
niques was inverted between each group to level fatigue
and related effects.
142
Considering the broad variance in professional education
we had to pick tasks that could be addressed without any
domain specific knowledge or education. Thus, we picked
rather simple tasks to which, we felt, everybody could re-
late, and thus contribute a significant number of ideas. In
the first task we asked the teams to take care of an Inuit
coming to a foreign country neither speaking the country’s
language nor having any useful equipment for the new en-
vironment. We considered this task a fruitful field for idea
generation, because necessary or useful items would in-
clude most, if not all, items in personal possession of the
participants. For ease of comparison we designed the sec-
ond task as similar as possible while still leaving plenty of
room for new unique ideas. In this second task the teams
had to discuss their own needs when they would leave their
home country for emigration into harsh, icy arctic territo-
ries. The subjects were asked to collect all material and
immaterial items they would consider necessary for sur-
vival under these conditions.
Results
In order to obtain quantitative data about the number and
types of ideas generated, we analyzed the system’s log files
and the videotapes to count new independent ideas (N),
ideas which built on own earlier ideas (O), ideas which re-
sulted from seeing somebody else write down an idea (S)
and ideas which resulted from talking about an idea (T). In
addition to this, we identified association chains and
counted their number (Nc) and length (Lc). Figure 6 shows
the overall numbers of ideas and their relative distribution.
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
Paper-based Electronic
N
O
S
T
Sum
Nc
Lc
Figure 6: Overall number of ideas (from 15 sessions) and rela-
tive distribution of types N, O, S and T, as well as number (Nc)
and the average length (Lc) of association chains
We found out, that the overall number of ideas generated
remained roughly equal (with a slight, but not statistically
significant decrease). This roughly confirmed our expecta-
tions, since we had not really changed any of the influenc-
ing parameters (Figure 1). It also suggests that the intro-
duced technology was not substantially disruptive for the
process of collaborative creative problem solving.
Figure 7: Subjective judgments about communication, num-
ber and quality of ideas
In the questionnaire before the evaluation, we had asked the
participants whether they had encountered problems with
insufficient communication, and with an insufficient num-
ber and quality of ideas in their previous experience with
manual brainstorming. In the questionnaire after the evalua-
tion, we asked them about these same problems when using
our electronic brainstorming system. Figure 7 shows a
comparison of the results.
Here, we found that the judgment had slightly changed after
using our electronic brainstorming system. From the par-
ticipants’ point of view, the communication turned out to
slightly decrease in the electronic version (70% before,
60% after) while the perceived number of ideas remained
the same (73% / 73%) and the quality (80% / 90%) of ideas
increased. The perceived decrease in communication is
valid within the participants but it is not strictly significant
(p ≈ 0.15). The increase in the perceived quality of ideas is
significant (p < 0.05).
Additionally, we asked the participants about the ease of
use of each interaction gesture we have implemented. Here
we received consistently good results regarding all interac-
tions. Only the possibility to write on the table received
rather low ratings. We think that this might be due to the
clumsy pen used, and the slightly unusual hand posture
which was required in order not to confuse the DViT track-
ing system. Figure 8 summarizes the results of users’ rat-
ings of the different interaction gestures.
To obtain an overall impression we finally asked the par-
ticipants whether they would use the paper-based version or
the electronic version of brainstorming in the future, assum-
ing that our system would be at product level. In our study,
80% of our participants would favor the electronic version
over the paper-based one.
0%
20%
40%
60%
80%
100%
More Communication More Ideas Better Ideas
Before Trial After Trial
143
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
90%
100%
Create
Post-It
Delete
Post-It
Open
Post-It
Close
Post-It
Copy
Post-It
Move
Post-It
Write
Post-It
Hard to learn Neutral Easy to learn
Figure 8: Subjective judgments about the ease of use of inter-
action gestures implemented in our system
INTERPRETATION
The data from our questionnaires and video transcripts
shows that the quantitative result of the brainstorming ses-
sions doesn’t differ significantly between electronic and
paper-based methods. In contrast to this, the perceived
judgments were consistently better for electronic brain-
storming. We attribute this to an improved communication,
which is supported by the free form comments participants
gave in the post-questionnaire, as well as two observations
we made during the sessions.
The first observation was that participants used the wall
display, showing ideas written on the desk immediately, as
an additional external reference. When they wanted to step
back mentally and obtain an overview, they looked at the
wall where all ideas generated so far were available in
much better overview. This common reference was a factor
which apparently increased group awareness and hence
improved communication.
The second observation is that this often led to resuming
discussion after a pause or a dead end. When a team got
stuck, it was much easier to review all the ideas generated
so far and start over by elaborating on earlier ones.
DESIGN CONSIDERATIONS
From our observations with the implemented system, both
from informal tests and our user study, we generalized the
following design considerations which we think can be ap-
plied to other socio-technical environments in order to sup-
port collaboration and creativity. They partly confirm exist-
ing guidelines for co-located collaboration on tabletop dis-
plays [34], but also apply to more general socio-technical
environments, comprising different classes of displays.
Pseudo-Physicality
Participants stated that they found the interface of the sys-
tem very easy to learn. We credit this on the close resem-
blance of each interface element to the real world. For ex-
ample, the interface contains visual elements that resemble
the real world equivalents (e.g. Post-it notes). These ele-
ments are also manipulated in the same way as they would
be in the real world (writing on paper with a pen). This al-
lows users to build on knowledge they gathered from a life-
long learning experience with the real world and the objects
in it. Touch sensitive interfaces and fluid gestures make
using the technology more continuous and analog. This
allows users to apply strategies they already use in the real
world to both implicitly and explicitly convey information
about the objects in the environment (e.g., territoriality).
Meta-Physicality
Even if the close resemblance of virtual items to real world
artifacts is beneficial to the ease of learning the interface, it
is worth exploiting the specific and different affordances of
digital media. These can augment physical actions, provid-
ing effects which are only possible in the digital realm (e.g.
the automatic re-orientation and appearance on the wall of
virtual Post-its). As long as objects have a clearly distinct
and explainable behavior, users seem to be willing to accept
and use a technique even if it is unrealistic in the strict
sense. For example, the participants reacted very positively
to the possibility to skid Post-its across the table, even if
this is not possible with real paper. Some even figured out
“that this is the behavior of a billiard ball”, which in fact is
true in terms of the physics we used for simulation.
Seamless Social Transitions
Transitions between concurrent and collaborative work
must be seamless in order to minimize obstacles for com-
munication. In co-located collaborative work it is natural
for humans to transition fluidly between collaborative and
concurrent individual activities [15]. For example, we ex-
perienced that participants in our study frequently switched
between developing their own ideas and re-joining the
group later to jointly develop an idea. This transition must
not be disrupted by the technology in order to prevent
communication breakdowns. To ensure this kind of seam-
less transitions several measurements can be taken:
All elements of the interface must be designed such that
there is no single or multi user mode. Every interaction
atom must be performable (in a meaningful way) so that it
can be carried out alone, in parallel with others and collabo-
ratively. As in a conversation, there should be no explicit
control token that has to be passed around in order for
someone to use the system. Everyone must be able to inter-
act with the system at any time. Thus it is important that all
data structures and controls (both virtual and physical input
devices) are replicated. This allows users to apply their
learned and familiar social protocols.
Finally, it is important that users can dynamically reconfig-
ure the spatial layout of items in the workspace so that they
can create distinct areas: private areas for interactions with
objects needed in personal work, and public areas to medi-
ate communication and interaction with the group [41].
144
Visibility of Social Interaction
Real time visibility of participants’ input actions and up-
dated output representation can foster group awareness. In
this sense, the system should afford communication through
body language and mediate communication through the
interface at the same time. The gesture vocabulary intro-
duced by the system merges with the one which is typical in
the face-to-face collaborative context. The possibility of
skidding Post-its to each other is an example of that. In this
context, the visible gesture of skidding a Post-it suggests to
the other group members the idea of “passing” something
over. Similarly, people move Post-its across the wall by
passing them among each other with an explicit gesture. In
contrast to a desktop or a mouse-based interaction para-
digm, the system affords body language and facial expres-
sion, rather than hindering those with the use of a keyboard
and of a small, vertical, personal screen.
The visibility of the produced Post-its in real time both on
the table and on the wall allows collaborators to immedi-
ately see, understand and react to the actions of others,
which can spark innovation and new ideas. The redundancy
of output representation on both displays affords different
perspectives: people can visually “step back” from their
focused view on the table and gain an overview of the
shared output on the wall: this can produce novel ideas and
communication.
SUMMARY
Based on an analysis of the factors influencing collabora-
tive creative problem solving, we have presented a number
of design goals for electronic systems to support this proc-
ess. Guided by these goals, we have designed and built an
electronic brainstorming system in an interactive environ-
ment using a tabletop and a large wall display, and dis-
cussed the design choices we made.
Our system was evaluated in a user study with 30 partici-
pants in order to verify the success of our design choices
and their influence on the brainstorming process. We found
that the quality and number of ideas generated with our
system was similar to classical paper-based brainstorming,
with the additional advantage of storing ideas and processes
which is afforded by our digital system. Furthermore the
perceived quality of the results was slightly higher in the
electronic brainstorming, possibly due to a design of the
system, which did not disrupt, but rather support social in-
teraction.
From the results of this study and a number of observations
we made in the process, we inferred a list of design consid-
erations, which can help others to design future collabora-
tive systems for this kind of socio-technical settings.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank all participants of the user study for their time
and patience.
REFERENCES
1. Amabile, T.M., The Social Psychology of Creativity.
Springer Verlag, New York, 1993.
2. Applegate, L., Konsynski, B., Nunamaker, J. A group
decision support system for idea generation and issue
analysis in organization planning. In Proceedings of
CSCW 1986, Texas, 16 – 34.
3. Barsalou, L. W. Context-independent and Context-
dependent Information in Concepts. Memory and Cogni-
tion (10), 1982, pp. 82-93.
4. Buxton, W. Chunking and Phrasing and the Design of
Human-Computer Dialogues. In Proceedings of the
IFIP World Computer Congress, Dublin, Ireland, 1986,
475-480.
5. Connolly, T., Jessup, L., Valacich, J. (1990), Effects of
anonymity and evaluative tone on idea generation in
computer-mediated groups. Management Science, 36,
689-703.
6. Consolvo, S., Arnstein, L., Franza, B., User Study
Techniques in the Design and Evaluation of a Ubicomp
Environment., Proceedings of the Fourth International
Conference on Ubiquitous Computing 2002, 73-90.
7. Cook, D. J. and Bailey, B. P. 2005. Designers' use of
paper and the implications for informal tools. In Pro-
ceedings of CHISIG ‘05 of Australia on Computer-
Human interaction: Citizens online: Considerations For
Today and the Future.
8. Czerwinski, M., Robertson, G.G., Meyers, B., Smith,
G., Robbins, D. & Tan, D. (2006). Large display re-
search overview. In Proceedings of CHI 2006, ACM
Press, 69-74.
9. Damm, C.H., Hansen, K.H., Thomsen, M., Tool Support
for Cooperative Object-Oriented Design: Gesture Based
Modeling on an Electronic Whiteboard. Proceedings of
CHI 2000, ACM Press, 518- 525.
10.Dennis, A., Heminger, A., Nunamaker, J., Vogel, D.
(1990), Bringing automated support to large groups: The
Burr-Brown experience. Information & Management,
18(3), 111-121.
11.Dennis, A.R. and Reinicke, B., Beta vs. VHS and the
Acceptance of Electronic Brainstorming Technology
MIS Quarterly, 28(1), 2004, 1-20.
12.Diehl, M., Stroebe, W., Productivity loss in brainstorm-
ing groups: Toward the solution of a riddle. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1987, 497–509.
13.Dourish, P. and Bellotti, V. Awareness and Coordina-
tion in Shared Workspaces. Proceedings of the Confer-
ence on Computer Supported Cooperative Work CSCW
´92, ACM Press, New York, 107-114.
14.Dourish, P.: Where the Action is. The Foundations of
Embodied Interaction. Bradford Books, 2001.
15.Elwart-Keys, M., Halonen, D., Horton, M., Kass, R.,
and Scott, P. (1990). User Interface Requirements for
145
Face to Face Groupware. In Proceedings of CHI ‘90,
ACM press, 295-301.
16.Fischer, G.: Social Creativity, Symmetry of Ignorance
and Meta-Design. In Proceedings of the Conference
Creativity & Cognition 1999, ACM Press, 116-123.
17.Geschka, H. (1978). Introduction and use of idea-
generation of methods. Research management. 21 (3).
18.Gladwell, M. (2002). The social life of paper. The New
Yorker, 92-96.
19.Guimbretière, F., Stone, M., Winograd, T. Fluid Interac-
tion with High-resolution Wall-size Displays. In Proc-
ceedings of UIST 2001.
20.Guimbretière, F., and Winograd, T. FlowMenu: Com-
bining Command, Text and Parameter Entry.
Procceedings of UIST 2000, 213 – 216.
21.Håkansson, M., Ljungblad, S. and Holmquist, L.E. Like
Solving a Giant Puzzle: Supporting Collaborative
Scheduling at a Film Festival. In Proceedings of
INTERACT 2003, IFIP, pp 773-776.
22.Hutchins, E. Cognition in the Wild. MIT Press, Cam-
bridge 1995.
23.Kerr, N.L, Bruun, S.E. (1983), Dispensability of mem-
ber effort and group motivation losses: Free-rider ef-
fects, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44,
78-94.
24.Kirsh, D. The Intelligent Use of Space. Artificial Intelli-
gence, 73, 31-68.
25.Kirsh, D., Maglio, P. (1994). On Distinguishing Epis-
temic from Pragmatic Action. In Journal on Cognitive
Science, 18, 513-549.
26.Klemmer, S.R., Newman, M.W., Farrell, R., Bilezikjian,
M., Landay, J.A. The Designers' Outpost: a Tangible In-
terface for Collaborative Web Site Design. In Proceed-
ings of UIST 2001, 1-10.
27.Leganchuk, A., Zhai, S., Buxton, W. Manual and Cogni-
tive Benefits of Two-Handed Input: An Experimental
Study. In ACM Transactions on Computer-Human In-
teraction, Vol5, No.4, 1998.
28.Miller, G.A. The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus
Two: Some Limits on our Capacity for Information
Processing. Psychological Review (63), 1956, 81-97.
29.Norman, D. A., and Bobrow, D. G. On Data-Limited
and Resource-Limited Processes. In Cognitive Psychol-
ogy (7), 1975, 44-64.
30.Osborn, A.F. Applied Imagination: Principles and Pro-
cedures of Creative Thinking. New York, Scribner, New
York, 1953.
31.Pinelle, D., Gutwin, C., Greenberg, S., Task Analysis
for Groupware Usability Evaluation: Modeling Shared-
Workspace Tasks with the Mechanics of Collaboration.
In ACM Tansactions on Computer-Human Interaction
(TOCHI), Vol.10 Issue 4, 2003, 281-311.
32.Robertson, G. Czerwinski, M. Baudisch, P. Meyers, B.
Robbins, D. Smith, G. Tan, D. Large Display User Ex-
perience. In IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications,
Vol. 25, Issue 4, 2005, 44-51,.
33.Santanen, E. L., Briggs, R. O., and de Vreede, G. 1999.
A cognitive network model of creativity: a renewed fo-
cus on brainstorming methodology. In Proceeding of the
20th International Conference on Information Systems
(1999). Association for Information Systems, Atlanta,
GA, 489-494.
34.Scott, S.D., Grant, K., D., & Mandryk, R.: System
Guidelines for Co-located, Collaborative Work on Ta-
bletop Display. System Guidelines for Co-located Col-
laborative Work on a Tabletop Display. In Proceedings
of ECSCW 2003, 159-178.
35.Scott, S.D., Carpendale, M.S.T, & Inkpen, K.M.: Terri-
toriality in Collaborative Tabletop Workspaces. In Pro-
ceedings of CSCW 2004.
36.Sellen, A. J., & Harper, R. (2001). The myth of the pa-
perless office.MIT Press.
37.Streitz, N.A., Geißler, J., Holmer, T., Konomi, S.,
Müller-Tomfelde, C., Reischl, W., Rexroth, P.,Seitz, P.,
and Steinmetz, R. , i-LAND: An interactive Landscape
for Creativitiy and Innovation. In Proceedings of CHI
1999. ACM Press, 120-127.
38.SmartTech DViT http://www.smarttech.com/DViT/
39. Terrenghi, L., Fritsche, T., Butz,A.: The EnLighTable: De-
sign ofAffordances to Support Collaborative Creativity. In
Proceedings of Smart Graphics Symposium 2006, 206-217.
40.Tatar, D.G., Foster, G., Bobrow, D.G. (1991). Design
for Conversation: Lessons from Cognoter. In Interna-
tional Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 34:185-209,
1991.
41.Tang, J.C., Findings from observational studies of col-
laborative work. In International Journal of Man-
Machine Studies (1991), 34, 143-160.
42.Van Gundy, A.B. (1981). Techniques of Structured
Problem Solving. NY. Van Nostrand, Reinhold.
43.Van Der Lugt, R., Functions of Sketching in Design
Idea Generation Meetings. In Proceedings of the 4th
Conference on Creativity & Cognition,2002, 72-79.
146

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

The challenges of remote scientific collaboration
The challenges of remote scientific collaborationThe challenges of remote scientific collaboration
The challenges of remote scientific collaborationProyecto CeVALE2
 
Managing Business Processes Communication and Performance
Managing Business Processes Communication and Performance Managing Business Processes Communication and Performance
Managing Business Processes Communication and Performance Yves Caseau
 
A Systems Literacy Manifesto / Hugh Dubberly
A Systems Literacy Manifesto / Hugh Dubberly A Systems Literacy Manifesto / Hugh Dubberly
A Systems Literacy Manifesto / Hugh Dubberly Robin Bahr
 
Resilience in Sociotechnical Systems: Studying Stakeholder Perspectives acros...
Resilience in Sociotechnical Systems: Studying Stakeholder Perspectives acros...Resilience in Sociotechnical Systems: Studying Stakeholder Perspectives acros...
Resilience in Sociotechnical Systems: Studying Stakeholder Perspectives acros...RSD Relating Systems Thinking and Design
 
Structured Dialogic Design
Structured  Dialogic  DesignStructured  Dialogic  Design
Structured Dialogic DesignSoCoDesign
 
What is Human Centred Design - The Design Journal
What is Human Centred Design - The Design JournalWhat is Human Centred Design - The Design Journal
What is Human Centred Design - The Design JournalJoseph Giacomin
 
Iqbal Developing Strategic Narratives: Designing Services as Systems
Iqbal  Developing Strategic Narratives: Designing Services as SystemsIqbal  Developing Strategic Narratives: Designing Services as Systems
Iqbal Developing Strategic Narratives: Designing Services as SystemsSystemic Design Association (SDA)
 
Patterns for collaborative creativity
Patterns for collaborative creativityPatterns for collaborative creativity
Patterns for collaborative creativityJohn Thomas
 
Supporting relationships with awareness systems
Supporting relationships with awareness systemsSupporting relationships with awareness systems
Supporting relationships with awareness systemsOnno Romijn
 
Social computing and knowledge creation
Social computing and knowledge creationSocial computing and knowledge creation
Social computing and knowledge creationMiia Kosonen
 
Abstract
AbstractAbstract
Abstractemaye
 
Personal dashboards for individual learning and project awareness in social s...
Personal dashboards for individual learning and project awareness in social s...Personal dashboards for individual learning and project awareness in social s...
Personal dashboards for individual learning and project awareness in social s...Wolfgang Reinhardt
 

Was ist angesagt? (18)

Collaborative E-Governance: Contours of a Meaningful Epistemology; David Pro...
Collaborative E-Governance:  Contours of a Meaningful Epistemology; David Pro...Collaborative E-Governance:  Contours of a Meaningful Epistemology; David Pro...
Collaborative E-Governance: Contours of a Meaningful Epistemology; David Pro...
 
Analyzing tools for facilitation through HEC Design Lens
Analyzing tools for facilitation through HEC Design LensAnalyzing tools for facilitation through HEC Design Lens
Analyzing tools for facilitation through HEC Design Lens
 
The challenges of remote scientific collaboration
The challenges of remote scientific collaborationThe challenges of remote scientific collaboration
The challenges of remote scientific collaboration
 
Lockton Tangible Thinking Workshop
Lockton Tangible Thinking WorkshopLockton Tangible Thinking Workshop
Lockton Tangible Thinking Workshop
 
Managing Business Processes Communication and Performance
Managing Business Processes Communication and Performance Managing Business Processes Communication and Performance
Managing Business Processes Communication and Performance
 
De Vries Probing the Future Workshop
De Vries Probing the Future WorkshopDe Vries Probing the Future Workshop
De Vries Probing the Future Workshop
 
A Systems Literacy Manifesto / Hugh Dubberly
A Systems Literacy Manifesto / Hugh Dubberly A Systems Literacy Manifesto / Hugh Dubberly
A Systems Literacy Manifesto / Hugh Dubberly
 
Lima Meninato Core Shifts for Emerging Desired Futures
Lima Meninato  Core Shifts for Emerging Desired FuturesLima Meninato  Core Shifts for Emerging Desired Futures
Lima Meninato Core Shifts for Emerging Desired Futures
 
Resilience in Sociotechnical Systems: Studying Stakeholder Perspectives acros...
Resilience in Sociotechnical Systems: Studying Stakeholder Perspectives acros...Resilience in Sociotechnical Systems: Studying Stakeholder Perspectives acros...
Resilience in Sociotechnical Systems: Studying Stakeholder Perspectives acros...
 
Structured Dialogic Design
Structured  Dialogic  DesignStructured  Dialogic  Design
Structured Dialogic Design
 
What is Human Centred Design - The Design Journal
What is Human Centred Design - The Design JournalWhat is Human Centred Design - The Design Journal
What is Human Centred Design - The Design Journal
 
Iqbal Developing Strategic Narratives: Designing Services as Systems
Iqbal  Developing Strategic Narratives: Designing Services as SystemsIqbal  Developing Strategic Narratives: Designing Services as Systems
Iqbal Developing Strategic Narratives: Designing Services as Systems
 
Connexions Roy 2013
Connexions Roy 2013Connexions Roy 2013
Connexions Roy 2013
 
Patterns for collaborative creativity
Patterns for collaborative creativityPatterns for collaborative creativity
Patterns for collaborative creativity
 
Supporting relationships with awareness systems
Supporting relationships with awareness systemsSupporting relationships with awareness systems
Supporting relationships with awareness systems
 
Social computing and knowledge creation
Social computing and knowledge creationSocial computing and knowledge creation
Social computing and knowledge creation
 
Abstract
AbstractAbstract
Abstract
 
Personal dashboards for individual learning and project awareness in social s...
Personal dashboards for individual learning and project awareness in social s...Personal dashboards for individual learning and project awareness in social s...
Personal dashboards for individual learning and project awareness in social s...
 

Andere mochten auch

Sw semantic web
Sw semantic webSw semantic web
Sw semantic webokeee
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc3
Dm uitwerkingen wc3Dm uitwerkingen wc3
Dm uitwerkingen wc3okeee
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc2
Dm uitwerkingen wc2Dm uitwerkingen wc2
Dm uitwerkingen wc2okeee
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc1
Dm uitwerkingen wc1Dm uitwerkingen wc1
Dm uitwerkingen wc1okeee
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc1
Dm uitwerkingen wc1Dm uitwerkingen wc1
Dm uitwerkingen wc1okeee
 
Dm part03 neural-networks-handout
Dm part03 neural-networks-handoutDm part03 neural-networks-handout
Dm part03 neural-networks-handoutokeee
 
Week02 answer
Week02 answerWeek02 answer
Week02 answerokeee
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc4
Dm uitwerkingen wc4Dm uitwerkingen wc4
Dm uitwerkingen wc4okeee
 
Web 2.0 Online Collaboration examples
Web 2.0 Online Collaboration examplesWeb 2.0 Online Collaboration examples
Web 2.0 Online Collaboration examplesR. Sosa
 

Andere mochten auch (9)

Sw semantic web
Sw semantic webSw semantic web
Sw semantic web
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc3
Dm uitwerkingen wc3Dm uitwerkingen wc3
Dm uitwerkingen wc3
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc2
Dm uitwerkingen wc2Dm uitwerkingen wc2
Dm uitwerkingen wc2
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc1
Dm uitwerkingen wc1Dm uitwerkingen wc1
Dm uitwerkingen wc1
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc1
Dm uitwerkingen wc1Dm uitwerkingen wc1
Dm uitwerkingen wc1
 
Dm part03 neural-networks-handout
Dm part03 neural-networks-handoutDm part03 neural-networks-handout
Dm part03 neural-networks-handout
 
Week02 answer
Week02 answerWeek02 answer
Week02 answer
 
Dm uitwerkingen wc4
Dm uitwerkingen wc4Dm uitwerkingen wc4
Dm uitwerkingen wc4
 
Web 2.0 Online Collaboration examples
Web 2.0 Online Collaboration examplesWeb 2.0 Online Collaboration examples
Web 2.0 Online Collaboration examples
 

Ähnlich wie Hcm p137 hilliges

A Systematic Literature Review For Human-Computer Interaction And Design Thin...
A Systematic Literature Review For Human-Computer Interaction And Design Thin...A Systematic Literature Review For Human-Computer Interaction And Design Thin...
A Systematic Literature Review For Human-Computer Interaction And Design Thin...Amy Roman
 
Designschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
DesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechangeDesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
DesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechangeAnoukPaepen
 
Designschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
DesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechangeDesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
DesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechangeAnoukPaepen
 
COLLABORA: A COLLABORATIVE ARCHITECTURE FOR EVALUATING INDIVIDUALS PARTICIPAT...
COLLABORA: A COLLABORATIVE ARCHITECTURE FOR EVALUATING INDIVIDUALS PARTICIPAT...COLLABORA: A COLLABORATIVE ARCHITECTURE FOR EVALUATING INDIVIDUALS PARTICIPAT...
COLLABORA: A COLLABORATIVE ARCHITECTURE FOR EVALUATING INDIVIDUALS PARTICIPAT...ijseajournal
 
Introduction to Interaction Design.pptx
Introduction to Interaction Design.pptxIntroduction to Interaction Design.pptx
Introduction to Interaction Design.pptxvaishalikhairnar4
 
MAB2014_DC_Final paper_Gonzalo_Reyero
MAB2014_DC_Final paper_Gonzalo_ReyeroMAB2014_DC_Final paper_Gonzalo_Reyero
MAB2014_DC_Final paper_Gonzalo_ReyeroGonzalo Reyero
 
I2126469
I2126469I2126469
I2126469aijbm
 
Collective and cumulative - some strategies of everyday design-in-use
Collective and cumulative - some strategies of everyday design-in-useCollective and cumulative - some strategies of everyday design-in-use
Collective and cumulative - some strategies of everyday design-in-useinuseproject
 
Gamify Your Team Design Thinking : Experimental Study on a Co-Evolution Theor...
Gamify Your Team Design Thinking : Experimental Study on a Co-Evolution Theor...Gamify Your Team Design Thinking : Experimental Study on a Co-Evolution Theor...
Gamify Your Team Design Thinking : Experimental Study on a Co-Evolution Theor...Junie Kwon
 
Towards Decision Support and Goal AchievementIdentifying Ac.docx
Towards Decision Support and Goal AchievementIdentifying Ac.docxTowards Decision Support and Goal AchievementIdentifying Ac.docx
Towards Decision Support and Goal AchievementIdentifying Ac.docxturveycharlyn
 
A case study analysis on digital convergent design: Skynet Platform
A case study analysis on digital convergent design: Skynet PlatformA case study analysis on digital convergent design: Skynet Platform
A case study analysis on digital convergent design: Skynet Platformdi8it
 
The Meta-Design of Systems, Menichinelli + Valsecchi 2016
The Meta-Design of Systems, Menichinelli + Valsecchi 2016The Meta-Design of Systems, Menichinelli + Valsecchi 2016
The Meta-Design of Systems, Menichinelli + Valsecchi 2016Massimo Menichinelli
 
Apply Funnel Model To Design Thinking Process
Apply Funnel Model To Design Thinking ProcessApply Funnel Model To Design Thinking Process
Apply Funnel Model To Design Thinking ProcessSara Alvarez
 
Usability guidelines for usable user interface
Usability guidelines for usable user interfaceUsability guidelines for usable user interface
Usability guidelines for usable user interfaceeSAT Publishing House
 
What is creative computing
What is creative computingWhat is creative computing
What is creative computingKamron Grant
 
Modelling the Media Logic of Software Systems
Modelling the Media Logic of Software SystemsModelling the Media Logic of Software Systems
Modelling the Media Logic of Software SystemsJan Schmidt
 
Paper On Infosuasive Appl
Paper On Infosuasive ApplPaper On Infosuasive Appl
Paper On Infosuasive Applguest5d624d
 

Ähnlich wie Hcm p137 hilliges (20)

A Systematic Literature Review For Human-Computer Interaction And Design Thin...
A Systematic Literature Review For Human-Computer Interaction And Design Thin...A Systematic Literature Review For Human-Computer Interaction And Design Thin...
A Systematic Literature Review For Human-Computer Interaction And Design Thin...
 
Designschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
DesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechangeDesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
Designschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
 
Designschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
DesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechangeDesignschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
Designschoolsasagentsofsustainablechange
 
Interface Design
Interface DesignInterface Design
Interface Design
 
COLLABORA: A COLLABORATIVE ARCHITECTURE FOR EVALUATING INDIVIDUALS PARTICIPAT...
COLLABORA: A COLLABORATIVE ARCHITECTURE FOR EVALUATING INDIVIDUALS PARTICIPAT...COLLABORA: A COLLABORATIVE ARCHITECTURE FOR EVALUATING INDIVIDUALS PARTICIPAT...
COLLABORA: A COLLABORATIVE ARCHITECTURE FOR EVALUATING INDIVIDUALS PARTICIPAT...
 
Introduction to Interaction Design.pptx
Introduction to Interaction Design.pptxIntroduction to Interaction Design.pptx
Introduction to Interaction Design.pptx
 
MAB2014_DC_Final paper_Gonzalo_Reyero
MAB2014_DC_Final paper_Gonzalo_ReyeroMAB2014_DC_Final paper_Gonzalo_Reyero
MAB2014_DC_Final paper_Gonzalo_Reyero
 
I2126469
I2126469I2126469
I2126469
 
Collective and cumulative - some strategies of everyday design-in-use
Collective and cumulative - some strategies of everyday design-in-useCollective and cumulative - some strategies of everyday design-in-use
Collective and cumulative - some strategies of everyday design-in-use
 
Gamify Your Team Design Thinking : Experimental Study on a Co-Evolution Theor...
Gamify Your Team Design Thinking : Experimental Study on a Co-Evolution Theor...Gamify Your Team Design Thinking : Experimental Study on a Co-Evolution Theor...
Gamify Your Team Design Thinking : Experimental Study on a Co-Evolution Theor...
 
Towards Decision Support and Goal AchievementIdentifying Ac.docx
Towards Decision Support and Goal AchievementIdentifying Ac.docxTowards Decision Support and Goal AchievementIdentifying Ac.docx
Towards Decision Support and Goal AchievementIdentifying Ac.docx
 
A case study analysis on digital convergent design: Skynet Platform
A case study analysis on digital convergent design: Skynet PlatformA case study analysis on digital convergent design: Skynet Platform
A case study analysis on digital convergent design: Skynet Platform
 
The Meta-Design of Systems, Menichinelli + Valsecchi 2016
The Meta-Design of Systems, Menichinelli + Valsecchi 2016The Meta-Design of Systems, Menichinelli + Valsecchi 2016
The Meta-Design of Systems, Menichinelli + Valsecchi 2016
 
Making ESSENCE Work
Making ESSENCE WorkMaking ESSENCE Work
Making ESSENCE Work
 
Apply Funnel Model To Design Thinking Process
Apply Funnel Model To Design Thinking ProcessApply Funnel Model To Design Thinking Process
Apply Funnel Model To Design Thinking Process
 
HCI CH-1.pptx
HCI CH-1.pptxHCI CH-1.pptx
HCI CH-1.pptx
 
Usability guidelines for usable user interface
Usability guidelines for usable user interfaceUsability guidelines for usable user interface
Usability guidelines for usable user interface
 
What is creative computing
What is creative computingWhat is creative computing
What is creative computing
 
Modelling the Media Logic of Software Systems
Modelling the Media Logic of Software SystemsModelling the Media Logic of Software Systems
Modelling the Media Logic of Software Systems
 
Paper On Infosuasive Appl
Paper On Infosuasive ApplPaper On Infosuasive Appl
Paper On Infosuasive Appl
 

Mehr von okeee

Dm part03 neural-networks-homework
Dm part03 neural-networks-homeworkDm part03 neural-networks-homework
Dm part03 neural-networks-homeworkokeee
 
10[1].1.1.115.9508
10[1].1.1.115.950810[1].1.1.115.9508
10[1].1.1.115.9508okeee
 
Prob18
Prob18Prob18
Prob18okeee
 
Overfit10
Overfit10Overfit10
Overfit10okeee
 
Decision tree.10.11
Decision tree.10.11Decision tree.10.11
Decision tree.10.11okeee
 
Dm week01 linreg.handout
Dm week01 linreg.handoutDm week01 linreg.handout
Dm week01 linreg.handoutokeee
 
Dm week02 decision-trees-handout
Dm week02 decision-trees-handoutDm week02 decision-trees-handout
Dm week02 decision-trees-handoutokeee
 
Dm week01 prob-refresher.handout
Dm week01 prob-refresher.handoutDm week01 prob-refresher.handout
Dm week01 prob-refresher.handoutokeee
 
Dm week01 intro.handout
Dm week01 intro.handoutDm week01 intro.handout
Dm week01 intro.handoutokeee
 
Dm week01 homework(1)
Dm week01 homework(1)Dm week01 homework(1)
Dm week01 homework(1)okeee
 
Chapter7 huizing
Chapter7 huizingChapter7 huizing
Chapter7 huizingokeee
 
Chapter8 choo
Chapter8 chooChapter8 choo
Chapter8 choookeee
 
Chapter6 huizing
Chapter6 huizingChapter6 huizing
Chapter6 huizingokeee
 
Kbms text-image
Kbms text-imageKbms text-image
Kbms text-imageokeee
 
Kbms audio
Kbms audioKbms audio
Kbms audiookeee
 
Kbms jan catin cont(1)
Kbms jan catin cont(1)Kbms jan catin cont(1)
Kbms jan catin cont(1)okeee
 
Kbms video-app
Kbms video-appKbms video-app
Kbms video-appokeee
 
Sw owl rules-proposal
Sw owl rules-proposalSw owl rules-proposal
Sw owl rules-proposalokeee
 
Sw practicumopdracht 4
Sw practicumopdracht 4Sw practicumopdracht 4
Sw practicumopdracht 4okeee
 
Sw cursusoverzicht
Sw cursusoverzichtSw cursusoverzicht
Sw cursusoverzichtokeee
 

Mehr von okeee (20)

Dm part03 neural-networks-homework
Dm part03 neural-networks-homeworkDm part03 neural-networks-homework
Dm part03 neural-networks-homework
 
10[1].1.1.115.9508
10[1].1.1.115.950810[1].1.1.115.9508
10[1].1.1.115.9508
 
Prob18
Prob18Prob18
Prob18
 
Overfit10
Overfit10Overfit10
Overfit10
 
Decision tree.10.11
Decision tree.10.11Decision tree.10.11
Decision tree.10.11
 
Dm week01 linreg.handout
Dm week01 linreg.handoutDm week01 linreg.handout
Dm week01 linreg.handout
 
Dm week02 decision-trees-handout
Dm week02 decision-trees-handoutDm week02 decision-trees-handout
Dm week02 decision-trees-handout
 
Dm week01 prob-refresher.handout
Dm week01 prob-refresher.handoutDm week01 prob-refresher.handout
Dm week01 prob-refresher.handout
 
Dm week01 intro.handout
Dm week01 intro.handoutDm week01 intro.handout
Dm week01 intro.handout
 
Dm week01 homework(1)
Dm week01 homework(1)Dm week01 homework(1)
Dm week01 homework(1)
 
Chapter7 huizing
Chapter7 huizingChapter7 huizing
Chapter7 huizing
 
Chapter8 choo
Chapter8 chooChapter8 choo
Chapter8 choo
 
Chapter6 huizing
Chapter6 huizingChapter6 huizing
Chapter6 huizing
 
Kbms text-image
Kbms text-imageKbms text-image
Kbms text-image
 
Kbms audio
Kbms audioKbms audio
Kbms audio
 
Kbms jan catin cont(1)
Kbms jan catin cont(1)Kbms jan catin cont(1)
Kbms jan catin cont(1)
 
Kbms video-app
Kbms video-appKbms video-app
Kbms video-app
 
Sw owl rules-proposal
Sw owl rules-proposalSw owl rules-proposal
Sw owl rules-proposal
 
Sw practicumopdracht 4
Sw practicumopdracht 4Sw practicumopdracht 4
Sw practicumopdracht 4
 
Sw cursusoverzicht
Sw cursusoverzichtSw cursusoverzicht
Sw cursusoverzicht
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 

Hcm p137 hilliges

  • 1. Designing for Collaborative Creative Problem Solving Otmar Hilliges, Lucia Terrenghi, Sebastian Boring, David Kim, Hendrik Richter, Andreas Butz Media informatics, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Munich Amalienstrasse 17, 80333 Munich, Germany otmar.hilliges|lucia.terrenghi|sebastian.boring|andreas.butz@ifi.lmu.de, kimdav|richterh@cip.ifi.lmu.de ABSTRACT Collaborative creativity is traditionally supported by formal techniques, such as brainstorming. These techniques im- prove the idea-generation process by creating group syner- gies, but also suffer from a number of negative effects [12]. Current electronic tools to support collaborative creativity overcome some of these problems, but introduce new ones, by either losing the benefits of face-to-face communication or the immediacy of simultaneous contribution. Using an interactive environment as a test bed, we are in- vestigating how collaborative creativity can be supported electronically while maintaining face-to-face communica- tion. What are the design-factors influencing such a sys- tem? We have designed a brainstorming application that uses an interactive table and a large wall display, and compared the results of using it to traditional paper-based brainstorming in a user study with 30 participants. From the considera- tions that went into the design and the observations during the study we derive a number of design guidelines for col- laborative systems in interactive environments. Author Keywords Collaborative creative problem solving, brainstorming, in- teractive environments, tabletop displays, wall displays, large displays ACM Classification Keywords H5.m. Information interfaces and presentation (e.g., HCI): Miscellaneous, H.5.3 Group and Organization Interfaces: Organizational design INTRODUCTION Collaborative problem solving requires much more than simply joining work forces. Knowledge and information need to be exchanged, different skills have to be coordi- nated, and the information communicated by others needs interpretation so that new ideas can be created and new solutions can be found. This process – with its core re- quirements of communication, coordination and interpreta- tion – is called collaborative creative problem solving [1]. In Fischer [16] creativity occurs in the relationship between an individual and a society, and between an individual and his or her technical environment. Appropriate socio- technical settings can amplify the outcome of a group of people by both augmenting individual creativity and multi- plying rather than simply summing up individual output. Physical, social and interaction contexts thus play an impor- tant role in guiding cognitive processes. In a ubiquitous computing scenario where technology blends in with the environment, we observe a paradigm shift from Human-Computer Interaction to computer- mediated human-to-human interaction. With the introduc- tion of large, interactive, high-resolution displays built into walls and tables, we face for the first time the challenge, and the opportunity, to design socio-technical systems which not only support collaboration but also mediate and foster human-to-human communication and interaction. Current computer systems already offer a variety of com- munication channels for distributed collaboration (e.g., in- stant messaging, e-mail, online communities, groupware) and support for collaborative work (CSCW). However, im- portant parts of our professional and personal life still de- pend on co-located collaboration and face-to-face commu- nication, with all the nuances of facial expression and body language, and the immediacy of verbal communication. Visibility of action is a fundamental aspect of group aware- ness [13]. Shared displays that support simultaneous input afford novel communication patterns as well as social pro- tocols, but the still predominantly used WIMP paradigm has poorly supported such social contexts thus far. In face-to-face collaborative creative problem-solving set- ups, technology is very often absent or shut down because it is considered disruptive to communication and the creative flow [37]. Using single-user systems in a collaborative set- ting leads, in most cases, to a communication breakdown since the user’s concentration has to shift away from the group and towards the computer in order to use it. Rather than relying on technology, these meetings still rely on the physical and social benefits of using surfaces such as tables and walls to exchange and visualize different types of in- formation (paper documents, presentations, pictures, etc.) and the different collaboration behaviors implied therein. Permission to make digital or hard copies of all or part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage and that copies bear this notice and the full citation on the first page. To copy otherwise, or republish, to post on servers or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. C&C’07, June 13–15, 2007, Washington, DC, USA. Copyright 2007 ACM 978-1-59593-712-4/07/0006...$5.00. 137
  • 2. Interactive surfaces offer new possibilities for the design of socio-technical systems that can partly exploit the physical and social affordances of a traditional face-to-face collabo- rative environment and at the same time benefit from the affordances of digital technology such as persistent data storage, easy information access, and the possibility to re- view previous processes or to undo certain actions. Building on an analysis of the factors that influence the brainstorming process in manual and electronic settings, we postulate the design goals for socio-technical systems that mediate creative group processes. We present a brainstorm- ing system as an example of a socio-technical environment that supports co-located collaborative creativity and our results from the evaluation of this implemented system. In designing and evaluating the system we studied how several interactive displays, each having distinct roles and functions in the process, influenced co-located collaboration. In con- clusion and building on previous work in the field, we pro- pose new design considerations to meet the requirements of future collaborative ubiquitous computing systems. COLLABORATIVE CREATIVE PROBLEM SOLVING Brainstorming [30] is a technique for divergent thinking. It can be individual, although the term more often refers to a group process for generating as many ideas or options as possible in response to an open question. Thus, it is fre- quently used for collaborative creative problem solving and it builds on a few main principles: quantity over quality of ideas, elaboration on others’ ideas and absence of criticism. The technique relies on the communication among group members to stimulate idea generation, and on coordination to maximize the individuals’ involvement and interpretation of ideas in order to create new intellectual associations. In this context the physical and social affordances of surfaces play an important role (e.g. a table for idea generation, a wall or whiteboard for idea discussion, paper for idea ex- pression and recording). These properties of brainstorming qualify it as a prime example for the creative problem solv- ing group processes we want to investigate. Osborn [30] anticipated positive synergy effects of such a technique, which affect the productivity of ideas. Other studies [12] show that these factors are apparently out- weighed by several negative social implications of the tech- nique (see Figure 1). They have shown that nominal brain- storming groups (aggregating ideas from separate individu- als) outperform face-to-face groups. The main reasons for this are losses in productivity through production blocking, social loafing and evaluation apprehension [2]. In addition to these findings later studies have shown that groups using Electronic Brainstorming Systems (EBS) ap- pear to outperform both manual and nominal brainstorming groups [5, 10, 12]. The main reasons (see Figure 1) for in- creased productivity are parallelism, to overcome produc- tion blocking, and anonymity, to reduce evaluation appre- hension. Even if anonymity bears the danger of social loaf- ing [23], its benefits appear to outweigh the losses. Figure 1: Factors influencing brainstorming productivity. Although EBS produce more ideas and can track the proc- ess better than manual brainstorming, the original technique (usually paper-based) is still ubiquitous in professional life and has not yet been replaced by EBS, as a recent study shows [11]. Why doesn’t a superior technology replace the inferior one? The answer to this has several aspects and not all of them are grounded in the qualities of the technique itself. The number and quality of ideas is not the only value to be assessed. Since group processes involve individuals, the individuals’ subjective perception of the process plays an important role. The perceived quality of the outcome itself depends on the degree to which personal interests are represented and val- ued in the group’s output. Second, the face-to-face situation of manual brainstorming has qualities which, in the long run, might even outweigh pure productivity measurements, namely the positive social aspects of team building, group awareness and a shared sense of achievement. If technology is either disruptive for an individual to ex- press her/himself, or for a group to communicate in a face- to-face situation, EBS won’t maintain those values that people perceive in using the manual brainstorming tech- nique, despite other quantifiable benefits such as productiv- ity of ideas and storage provided by remote EBS. Considering the tradeoff (see Table 1) between technique performance and social implications of EBSs we wanted to build a system that maintains the empirically proven advan- tages of EBS while combining them with the social advan- tages of manual face-to-face brainstorming. Hence we sac- rificed the anonymity of group members (which is expected to lessen evaluation apprehension) in favor of the social implications of group awareness and personal communica- tion. We deliberately accepted that this decision might de- crease the number of ideas generated because we expect that it improves the subjective perception of quality and hence the acceptance of the whole process. In addition, we are mostly interested in co-located collaboration since we want to generalize our findings to other collaborative crea- tivity scenarios more complex than this particular one. 138
  • 3. PC- based face-to face Electronic remote Manual Electronic face-to- face Synergy Group awareness - Anonymity - - - Parallelism - - Reduced soc. loafing - - Reduced production blocking - - Reduced evaluation apprehension - - - Table 1: Properties of different technologies regarding brain- storming productivity and group processes DESIGN GOALS We aimed to design a socio-technical environment which positively affects collaborative creative problem solving. For the support of such a group process we tried to enhance both the divergent as well as the convergent thinking (i.e., generation and elaboration of ideas), which are typical and essential activities in problem solving. Thus, we considered the affordances of technologically enriched environments. In order to facilitate both generation and selection of ideas, we used large interactive surfaces in the environment. The horizontal plane of a table affords writing, face to face communication, territoriality [35], and group awareness, while a wall display allows and supports shared visualiza- tion, overview and context awareness. The combination of these interactive surfaces opens interesting possibilities for the design of a socio-technical environment. The issue of more fluid interaction with large, high- resolution displays that support creative group processes has been treated in [19, 21, 26, 39]. A summary of recent advances in the field of interaction techniques for large dis- plays can be found in Czerwinski et al. [8]. Most of this research has focused either on the properties and design implications for vertical large displays [32] or on the influ- ence of horizontal displays on co-located collaborative work [34], thus mostly focusing on a specific type of dis- play. We take a more ecological, holistic approach by look- ing at the combination of different displays across the proc- ess, both in time and space. Immediacy of Communication and Interaction Our main goal was to combine some of the benefits of pre- vious EBS with the positive social implications of face-to- face groups. First, we wanted to avoid production block by guaranteeing true parallelism of input such that every mem- ber of the group could contribute at any time. Additionally, we wanted to store every idea and make it permanently available such that it can be read and interpreted by every member at all times. We also wanted to minimize the costs of interaction and communication so that even in small groups the synergy effects can outweigh the losses. This can be achieved by two complementing measures: first, by blending the com- puter into the environment in which the collaborative crea- tive processes take place, and second, by blending the vir- tual interface into the task so that knowing the craft (or technique) reduces the cost of learning and using the sys- tem. Thus, essential elements of the interface should behave just like their counterparts in the manual process. In this way we hope to avoid some of the factors that seem to hinder the communication process [2], e.g., the size of a personal computer screen or the keyboard as a disruptive interface in this context. Enhance Phrasing The difference between chunking and phrasing is explained in more depth by Buxton [4]. Different representations of the task and different levels of skills between novices and experts imply different granularity of detail at which people approach the solution of a problem. The more novice the user, the lower the level at which s/he approaches the prob- lem. This results in chunking the main goal into sub-goals, to be achieved through sub-tasks. The acquisition of skills enhances the automatic performance of some sub-tasks, thus achieving the high-level goals faster and more easily (phrasing). To enhance phrasing (i.e., the undisrupted flow of action and creativity), the task needs to be represented in a way that supports chunk maximization. Indeed, the bigger the chunks are, the less cognitive resources are wasted. In this sense we aim at an interface which allows a fluid and immediate interaction with the task at hand. Furthermore, the interface needs to support the externalization of the chunks so as to allow reflection and association. Minimize Cognitive Load The human capability of keeping chunks of information in short-term memory is very limited [28]. Thus, we need to design the context (e.g., the representation of the task) so as to minimize the use of cognitive resources for holding acti- vated bundles in short term memory. Typical techniques for freeing cognitive resources are externalization, e.g., through the use of space, epistemic action [24, 25] and visual output as well as spatial mapping [29, 22]. Thus more resources remain available for creative associations. 139
  • 4. Mediate Mutual Association Activation When a person is exposed to stimuli from a variety of con- texts s/he is more likely to have novel associations [33]. The activation of such associations can be automatic (with- out intentional conscious awareness), or depend upon the context of the stimuli (conscious capacity spreading activa- tion, [3]). This suggests that the design of a context of in- teraction can affect the association patterns. Furthermore, it suggests that the context of interaction can stimulate activa- tion patterns that would otherwise be unlikely in the auto- matic spreading activation. Thus, with a socio-technical environment which positively affects collaborative creativ- ity, we hope to create a context that supports the explora- tion of different areas of our knowledge network. In order to enable such a process, it is important that people can perceive each other’s ideas and communicate about them. We think that providing different visual cues about the generated ideas on the table and on the wall, as well as giving users the possibility to explicitly exchange ideas (and the visual representation thereof) can enable and foster the generation of new unexpected associations. Supporting Group Awareness and Overview Visibility of action is a main design principle for embodied interaction [14]. It provides awareness of what other col- leagues are doing and how the actions of group members affect the shared artifacts and relies on existing theories from CSCW [13]. Group awareness (i.e. the condition where members perceive the presence of other group mem- bers and the possibility to communicate with them), seems to provide a basis for informal communication. By giving every group member at any time the possibility to under- stand what other members are doing, the isolation of single individuals is avoided. Mutual visibility of actions eases the coordination and interpretation of the contributors’ actions. DESIGN CHOICES By our design choices we try to merge some of the advan- tages of traditional techniques for face-to-face problem solving, with some of the benefits of the technology em- bedded in the interactive surfaces of the environment. In order to do so, we designed a multi-user application for supporting co-located collaborative problem solving, which together with the users forms a socio-technical environment resembling a face-to-face meeting situation. This means that the system doesn’t have any standard computer moni- tors or input devices but solely relies on interactive, touch sensitive displays built into the meeting room’s table and wall (see Figure 2). Presuming that display technology will be cheap and distributed enough to pervade our working environments in the near future, we explore the potential of a socio-technical environment as ecosystem, by analyzing how communication and spatial mapping can be supported. Thus, we do not focus on the real estate and properties of one specific display in isolation, but rather on the combina- tion of displays and on the question how the relationships between the different surfaces affect the group process. Figure 2: Our instrumented room with wall and table displays In this setup we developed an application, which meta- phorically builds on the “idea card” method, i.e., the use of Post-its for brainwriting. Geschka [17] and VanGundy [42] developed the Interactive Brainwriting Pool Technique. In this method group members write their ideas on a piece of paper that is then placed in the center of the table for an- other member to read prior to writing their next comment. The use of Post-its during brainwriting has become a rather common practice in collaborative problem solving. They afford the recording of ideas in written rather than just ver- bal form in the generative phase. Furthermore, they support a certain territoriality and the creation of semantic regions. When participants are given a stack of post-its and start sticking them around their working area, they define their personal region, which remains visible to others, thus creat- ing a mutual awareness among participants. Using Post-its on surfaces supports the convergent thinking phase as well, when participants stick and move Post-its on flip charts or white boards in order to recognize patterns and create clusters. In this phase they structure the devel- oped ideas into more meaningful concepts and try to iden- tify relations between them. The affordances of paper have been more deeply discussed in [36, 7, 18]. Studies of paper in work practice show that paper continues to be widely used for many reasons including its spatial flexibility (it can be quickly arranged in physical space), sociability (it facili- tates face-to-face communication), and tailorability (it is easily annotated) [7]. Furthermore [18] explains that some of the reasons why designers use paper rather than elec- tronic communication is for its “friendliness”, immediacy and affordances for face-to-face communication of ideas. To this respect we opted for a pen based input to support the fluid generation of ideas with handwriting. A limited gesture vocabulary was designed and implemented. Users can start generating ideas by drawing a square on the table surface. This event triggers the appearance of a large yellow square, resembling a Post-it, thus defining the area to write in (see Figure 3). By tipping a designated area of the Post- 140
  • 5. it, the latter shrinks to a smaller size and becomes moveable (see Figure 4). The user can then create new Post-its/ideas by drawing new squares in a blank region of the table and writing within the yellow region. This choice was made in order to create visual constraints for writing, so as to iden- tify ideas as units, and to create visual cues for distinguish- ing territories and patterns. When the Post-it is shrunk its content is still readable. Figure 3: Creating a Post-it and writing on it The choice of using handwriting and gestures to enhance fluid interaction and the group process was also made in Guimbretière et al. [20]. Furthermore Buxton [4] examine the effect of compound tasks on the users’ cognitive load. These are tasks that usually can be expressed in one sen- tence (e.g. write text onto a post-it) but have to be broken down into multiple steps in standard desktop applications (e.g. select target, choose text tool, type text). This may result in additional cognitive burden on top of the actual task (idea generation). Kinesthetic gestures can overcome such problems by mapping to whole phrases rather than functions of the underlying system. The desired one-to-one correspondence between concept and gesture leads to inter- faces which are more compatible with the users’ mental model. Our design supports simultaneous, gesture-based interaction, as well as the mutual visibility of action. Direct manipulation in this context refers to a coincident spatial mapping of input and output (there is no such device as a pointer or a remote controller). This creates a transparent causal relationship between gestures and output, and sup- ports visibility of gestures. Furthermore, it allows the crea- tion of temporal spatial structures, which are fundamentals to epistemic actions [25]. Creative processes highly rely on epistemic actions, as these allow the externalization and visualization of different alternatives [24]. The size of the table (see Figure 2) allows participants to see each other’s actions, movements and gestures in real time. It also sup- ports a sense of common ground by displaying the results of both participants’ interactions on a single shared display. Thus, both controls and resources (i.e., the different Post- its) are continuously available to both participants. Building on these considerations we designed the Post-its in such a way that they can be edited, moved, deleted and cop- ied by any participant after they have been created. Addi- tionally we created a mechanism to encourage building on each other’s ideas: With a quick movement of the pen, each user can deliberately skid one idea to the other participant. The Post-it slides quickly across the table and smoothly reorients itself towards the other user. This supports the explicit sharing of ideas and thus encourages the creation of association chains. Figure 4: Dragging a Post-it and skidding it The immediate and visible change of the shared visual landscape is supported by the system in additional ways. As the participants create Post-its in their working area, thus already creating a distinct territorial setup, the Post-its ap- pear simultaneously on the vertical display, which is lo- cated next to the table. On the vertical display the Post-its are reoriented upright, i.e., readable for both readers, but they maintain a spatial mapping to the territorial setup on the table display. In this sense the perception of territoriality and group awareness are supported. A participant will rec- ognize his/her own “territory” on the wall, but at the same time gain an overview of the ideas created by the group. When users tip the Post-it in the middle, where a small grid is displayed (Figure 3), they can enlarge it again to edit its content. When they instead tip its periphery (Figure 4) and drag the pen, they move the Post-it, both on the horizontal and the vertical display. When users move from the table (generative phase, divergent thinking) to the wall display (structural phase, convergent thinking), they can spatially organize the ideas by rearranging them on the wall. In addi- tion they can create clusters by drawing a circle around some Post-its (Figure 5). Clusters are merged by dragging them close together. Drawing a cross on the border of a cluster causes it to dissolve into single Post-its again. Clus- ters can be connected to each other or to single Post-its by drawing a line from the border of one cluster to the border of another one (Figure 5) or to the center of a Post-it. Finally, whole clusters can be moved across the display, thus moving all the Post-its they contain. This set of cluster- ing techniques clearly extends the functionality of a physi- cal whiteboard or flip chart while it maintains the direct manipulation characteristics thereof, facilitating the creation of a structured knowledge representation, which is easily editable by every participant through direct manipulation. 141
  • 6. Figure 5: Creating and connecting clusters. EVALUATION We implemented our brainstorming system in order to ver- ify whether its design meets the initial design goals and to determine which effect our design choices had on the crea- tive process, its objective outcome and the subjective feel- ing of productivity of the participants. The evaluation of subjective experiences is an open topic for the ubiquitous computing community, especially when the aim is to assess an experience involving different displays, people, and temporal phases. For the assessment of a single users’ sub- jective experience we opted for questionnaires. The col- laborative creative process and its outcome are even harder to measure quantitatively. Different approaches for measur- ing collaborative creativity processes in novel interaction settings have been proposed in the literature. Conversational analysis has been previously used to assess collaboration. Tatar et al. [40] apply techniques from Psy- cholinguistics to evaluate the conversational patterns of a system supporting co-located brainstorming. Damm et al. [9] videotaped, observed and interpreted several sessions of a collaborative software design process. The observations allowed qualitative statements about the different designs studied, but the process didn't produce any quantitative data. Consolvo et al. [6] provide a good overview of rele- vant known study and evaluation techniques and discuss their applicability to ubiquitous computing settings. For quantitative evaluations, they propose Lag Sequential Analysis (LSA) and describe how it was applied to their environment. LSA relies on the idea of logging and count- ing relevant events in the environment and generates statis- tical data about the observed process. In our evaluation we logged the creation and exchange of ideas as the basic events in the creativity process. Pinelle et al. [31] propose the use of task analysis for the evaluation of collaborative processes. They propose a hier- archical task model and describe its application in a tech- nique called collaboration usability analysis. The structure of the brainstorming task is relatively simple and in our study we only identified whether ideas were independently generated, built on an own previous idea, or resulted from seeing or discussing ideas of the brainstorming partner. Van der Lugt [43] specifically recorded how people built on each other's ideas in a brainstorming process and generated link diagrams from this data. From the link diagrams he then generated statistical data about the number and type of connections between ideas. We found in the analysis of our videotapes, that it was often not possible to identify all ear- lier ideas which might have contributed to the creation of a particular new one and that the attempt to do so would be very speculative and error-prone. Therefore, we only identi- fied chains of ideas, which directly built on each other (as- sociation chains), and evaluated their size and number in order to obtain statistical data. Subjects The study we conducted consisted of 30 participants in 15 teams of two subjects each. To avoid gender bias we had 5 pure male, 5 mixed and 5 pure female teams. Some of the subjects knew each other while others had never met be- fore. Among the participants a variety of professions was present, such as computer science students, architects, de- signers, civil engineers, musicologists and journalists. All in all we had participants from 5 different nations. Environment The system was deployed in an instrumented environment containing an interactive meeting table as well as displays embedded into an interactive wall (see Figure 2). Both the table and the wall display were included in our electronic brainstorming system. The interactive table consists of an LCD monitor embedded into a wooden table and is equipped with a DViT overlay panel [38] (a vision based tracking system providing multi- ple simultaneous inputs) for interactivity. Hence, the par- ticipants share an overall table space of 1.6 x 1.2 meters. The complete wall is an interactive surface with a width of 5 meters and a height of 2.5 meters containing three back- projected displays. The two side displays as well as the rest of the wall are tracked by four cameras. The center display additionally provides high precision input through another DViT panel. While applying the paper-based technique, the teams used the same table covered with paper in the idea generation phase and a large (1.8 x 1.5 meters) piece of paper, mounted on the opposite wall, in the structural phase. Tasks and Procedure We conducted a within-group comparative study between our system and the original paper-based manual brainstorm- ing technique to assess the brainstorming productivity of both techniques. Furthermore, we ran questionnaires before and after the task as a qualitative user study to evaluate the subjective perception of and judgments about our system. We also assessed the subjective judgments of the brain- storming results in these questionnaires. Each session took approximately one hour and included the two question- naires, introductions to the technique and the interface, as well as one warm-up task. The participants received two different but related tasks. Each of these tasks had to be executed in a different technique. The order of the tech- niques was inverted between each group to level fatigue and related effects. 142
  • 7. Considering the broad variance in professional education we had to pick tasks that could be addressed without any domain specific knowledge or education. Thus, we picked rather simple tasks to which, we felt, everybody could re- late, and thus contribute a significant number of ideas. In the first task we asked the teams to take care of an Inuit coming to a foreign country neither speaking the country’s language nor having any useful equipment for the new en- vironment. We considered this task a fruitful field for idea generation, because necessary or useful items would in- clude most, if not all, items in personal possession of the participants. For ease of comparison we designed the sec- ond task as similar as possible while still leaving plenty of room for new unique ideas. In this second task the teams had to discuss their own needs when they would leave their home country for emigration into harsh, icy arctic territo- ries. The subjects were asked to collect all material and immaterial items they would consider necessary for sur- vival under these conditions. Results In order to obtain quantitative data about the number and types of ideas generated, we analyzed the system’s log files and the videotapes to count new independent ideas (N), ideas which built on own earlier ideas (O), ideas which re- sulted from seeing somebody else write down an idea (S) and ideas which resulted from talking about an idea (T). In addition to this, we identified association chains and counted their number (Nc) and length (Lc). Figure 6 shows the overall numbers of ideas and their relative distribution. 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 Paper-based Electronic N O S T Sum Nc Lc Figure 6: Overall number of ideas (from 15 sessions) and rela- tive distribution of types N, O, S and T, as well as number (Nc) and the average length (Lc) of association chains We found out, that the overall number of ideas generated remained roughly equal (with a slight, but not statistically significant decrease). This roughly confirmed our expecta- tions, since we had not really changed any of the influenc- ing parameters (Figure 1). It also suggests that the intro- duced technology was not substantially disruptive for the process of collaborative creative problem solving. Figure 7: Subjective judgments about communication, num- ber and quality of ideas In the questionnaire before the evaluation, we had asked the participants whether they had encountered problems with insufficient communication, and with an insufficient num- ber and quality of ideas in their previous experience with manual brainstorming. In the questionnaire after the evalua- tion, we asked them about these same problems when using our electronic brainstorming system. Figure 7 shows a comparison of the results. Here, we found that the judgment had slightly changed after using our electronic brainstorming system. From the par- ticipants’ point of view, the communication turned out to slightly decrease in the electronic version (70% before, 60% after) while the perceived number of ideas remained the same (73% / 73%) and the quality (80% / 90%) of ideas increased. The perceived decrease in communication is valid within the participants but it is not strictly significant (p ≈ 0.15). The increase in the perceived quality of ideas is significant (p < 0.05). Additionally, we asked the participants about the ease of use of each interaction gesture we have implemented. Here we received consistently good results regarding all interac- tions. Only the possibility to write on the table received rather low ratings. We think that this might be due to the clumsy pen used, and the slightly unusual hand posture which was required in order not to confuse the DViT track- ing system. Figure 8 summarizes the results of users’ rat- ings of the different interaction gestures. To obtain an overall impression we finally asked the par- ticipants whether they would use the paper-based version or the electronic version of brainstorming in the future, assum- ing that our system would be at product level. In our study, 80% of our participants would favor the electronic version over the paper-based one. 0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100% More Communication More Ideas Better Ideas Before Trial After Trial 143
  • 8. 0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% 100% Create Post-It Delete Post-It Open Post-It Close Post-It Copy Post-It Move Post-It Write Post-It Hard to learn Neutral Easy to learn Figure 8: Subjective judgments about the ease of use of inter- action gestures implemented in our system INTERPRETATION The data from our questionnaires and video transcripts shows that the quantitative result of the brainstorming ses- sions doesn’t differ significantly between electronic and paper-based methods. In contrast to this, the perceived judgments were consistently better for electronic brain- storming. We attribute this to an improved communication, which is supported by the free form comments participants gave in the post-questionnaire, as well as two observations we made during the sessions. The first observation was that participants used the wall display, showing ideas written on the desk immediately, as an additional external reference. When they wanted to step back mentally and obtain an overview, they looked at the wall where all ideas generated so far were available in much better overview. This common reference was a factor which apparently increased group awareness and hence improved communication. The second observation is that this often led to resuming discussion after a pause or a dead end. When a team got stuck, it was much easier to review all the ideas generated so far and start over by elaborating on earlier ones. DESIGN CONSIDERATIONS From our observations with the implemented system, both from informal tests and our user study, we generalized the following design considerations which we think can be ap- plied to other socio-technical environments in order to sup- port collaboration and creativity. They partly confirm exist- ing guidelines for co-located collaboration on tabletop dis- plays [34], but also apply to more general socio-technical environments, comprising different classes of displays. Pseudo-Physicality Participants stated that they found the interface of the sys- tem very easy to learn. We credit this on the close resem- blance of each interface element to the real world. For ex- ample, the interface contains visual elements that resemble the real world equivalents (e.g. Post-it notes). These ele- ments are also manipulated in the same way as they would be in the real world (writing on paper with a pen). This al- lows users to build on knowledge they gathered from a life- long learning experience with the real world and the objects in it. Touch sensitive interfaces and fluid gestures make using the technology more continuous and analog. This allows users to apply strategies they already use in the real world to both implicitly and explicitly convey information about the objects in the environment (e.g., territoriality). Meta-Physicality Even if the close resemblance of virtual items to real world artifacts is beneficial to the ease of learning the interface, it is worth exploiting the specific and different affordances of digital media. These can augment physical actions, provid- ing effects which are only possible in the digital realm (e.g. the automatic re-orientation and appearance on the wall of virtual Post-its). As long as objects have a clearly distinct and explainable behavior, users seem to be willing to accept and use a technique even if it is unrealistic in the strict sense. For example, the participants reacted very positively to the possibility to skid Post-its across the table, even if this is not possible with real paper. Some even figured out “that this is the behavior of a billiard ball”, which in fact is true in terms of the physics we used for simulation. Seamless Social Transitions Transitions between concurrent and collaborative work must be seamless in order to minimize obstacles for com- munication. In co-located collaborative work it is natural for humans to transition fluidly between collaborative and concurrent individual activities [15]. For example, we ex- perienced that participants in our study frequently switched between developing their own ideas and re-joining the group later to jointly develop an idea. This transition must not be disrupted by the technology in order to prevent communication breakdowns. To ensure this kind of seam- less transitions several measurements can be taken: All elements of the interface must be designed such that there is no single or multi user mode. Every interaction atom must be performable (in a meaningful way) so that it can be carried out alone, in parallel with others and collabo- ratively. As in a conversation, there should be no explicit control token that has to be passed around in order for someone to use the system. Everyone must be able to inter- act with the system at any time. Thus it is important that all data structures and controls (both virtual and physical input devices) are replicated. This allows users to apply their learned and familiar social protocols. Finally, it is important that users can dynamically reconfig- ure the spatial layout of items in the workspace so that they can create distinct areas: private areas for interactions with objects needed in personal work, and public areas to medi- ate communication and interaction with the group [41]. 144
  • 9. Visibility of Social Interaction Real time visibility of participants’ input actions and up- dated output representation can foster group awareness. In this sense, the system should afford communication through body language and mediate communication through the interface at the same time. The gesture vocabulary intro- duced by the system merges with the one which is typical in the face-to-face collaborative context. The possibility of skidding Post-its to each other is an example of that. In this context, the visible gesture of skidding a Post-it suggests to the other group members the idea of “passing” something over. Similarly, people move Post-its across the wall by passing them among each other with an explicit gesture. In contrast to a desktop or a mouse-based interaction para- digm, the system affords body language and facial expres- sion, rather than hindering those with the use of a keyboard and of a small, vertical, personal screen. The visibility of the produced Post-its in real time both on the table and on the wall allows collaborators to immedi- ately see, understand and react to the actions of others, which can spark innovation and new ideas. The redundancy of output representation on both displays affords different perspectives: people can visually “step back” from their focused view on the table and gain an overview of the shared output on the wall: this can produce novel ideas and communication. SUMMARY Based on an analysis of the factors influencing collabora- tive creative problem solving, we have presented a number of design goals for electronic systems to support this proc- ess. Guided by these goals, we have designed and built an electronic brainstorming system in an interactive environ- ment using a tabletop and a large wall display, and dis- cussed the design choices we made. Our system was evaluated in a user study with 30 partici- pants in order to verify the success of our design choices and their influence on the brainstorming process. We found that the quality and number of ideas generated with our system was similar to classical paper-based brainstorming, with the additional advantage of storing ideas and processes which is afforded by our digital system. Furthermore the perceived quality of the results was slightly higher in the electronic brainstorming, possibly due to a design of the system, which did not disrupt, but rather support social in- teraction. From the results of this study and a number of observations we made in the process, we inferred a list of design consid- erations, which can help others to design future collabora- tive systems for this kind of socio-technical settings. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank all participants of the user study for their time and patience. REFERENCES 1. Amabile, T.M., The Social Psychology of Creativity. Springer Verlag, New York, 1993. 2. Applegate, L., Konsynski, B., Nunamaker, J. A group decision support system for idea generation and issue analysis in organization planning. In Proceedings of CSCW 1986, Texas, 16 – 34. 3. Barsalou, L. W. Context-independent and Context- dependent Information in Concepts. Memory and Cogni- tion (10), 1982, pp. 82-93. 4. Buxton, W. Chunking and Phrasing and the Design of Human-Computer Dialogues. In Proceedings of the IFIP World Computer Congress, Dublin, Ireland, 1986, 475-480. 5. Connolly, T., Jessup, L., Valacich, J. (1990), Effects of anonymity and evaluative tone on idea generation in computer-mediated groups. Management Science, 36, 689-703. 6. Consolvo, S., Arnstein, L., Franza, B., User Study Techniques in the Design and Evaluation of a Ubicomp Environment., Proceedings of the Fourth International Conference on Ubiquitous Computing 2002, 73-90. 7. Cook, D. J. and Bailey, B. P. 2005. Designers' use of paper and the implications for informal tools. In Pro- ceedings of CHISIG ‘05 of Australia on Computer- Human interaction: Citizens online: Considerations For Today and the Future. 8. Czerwinski, M., Robertson, G.G., Meyers, B., Smith, G., Robbins, D. & Tan, D. (2006). Large display re- search overview. In Proceedings of CHI 2006, ACM Press, 69-74. 9. Damm, C.H., Hansen, K.H., Thomsen, M., Tool Support for Cooperative Object-Oriented Design: Gesture Based Modeling on an Electronic Whiteboard. Proceedings of CHI 2000, ACM Press, 518- 525. 10.Dennis, A., Heminger, A., Nunamaker, J., Vogel, D. (1990), Bringing automated support to large groups: The Burr-Brown experience. Information & Management, 18(3), 111-121. 11.Dennis, A.R. and Reinicke, B., Beta vs. VHS and the Acceptance of Electronic Brainstorming Technology MIS Quarterly, 28(1), 2004, 1-20. 12.Diehl, M., Stroebe, W., Productivity loss in brainstorm- ing groups: Toward the solution of a riddle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1987, 497–509. 13.Dourish, P. and Bellotti, V. Awareness and Coordina- tion in Shared Workspaces. Proceedings of the Confer- ence on Computer Supported Cooperative Work CSCW ´92, ACM Press, New York, 107-114. 14.Dourish, P.: Where the Action is. The Foundations of Embodied Interaction. Bradford Books, 2001. 15.Elwart-Keys, M., Halonen, D., Horton, M., Kass, R., and Scott, P. (1990). User Interface Requirements for 145
  • 10. Face to Face Groupware. In Proceedings of CHI ‘90, ACM press, 295-301. 16.Fischer, G.: Social Creativity, Symmetry of Ignorance and Meta-Design. In Proceedings of the Conference Creativity & Cognition 1999, ACM Press, 116-123. 17.Geschka, H. (1978). Introduction and use of idea- generation of methods. Research management. 21 (3). 18.Gladwell, M. (2002). The social life of paper. The New Yorker, 92-96. 19.Guimbretière, F., Stone, M., Winograd, T. Fluid Interac- tion with High-resolution Wall-size Displays. In Proc- ceedings of UIST 2001. 20.Guimbretière, F., and Winograd, T. FlowMenu: Com- bining Command, Text and Parameter Entry. Procceedings of UIST 2000, 213 – 216. 21.Håkansson, M., Ljungblad, S. and Holmquist, L.E. Like Solving a Giant Puzzle: Supporting Collaborative Scheduling at a Film Festival. In Proceedings of INTERACT 2003, IFIP, pp 773-776. 22.Hutchins, E. Cognition in the Wild. MIT Press, Cam- bridge 1995. 23.Kerr, N.L, Bruun, S.E. (1983), Dispensability of mem- ber effort and group motivation losses: Free-rider ef- fects, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 78-94. 24.Kirsh, D. The Intelligent Use of Space. Artificial Intelli- gence, 73, 31-68. 25.Kirsh, D., Maglio, P. (1994). On Distinguishing Epis- temic from Pragmatic Action. In Journal on Cognitive Science, 18, 513-549. 26.Klemmer, S.R., Newman, M.W., Farrell, R., Bilezikjian, M., Landay, J.A. The Designers' Outpost: a Tangible In- terface for Collaborative Web Site Design. In Proceed- ings of UIST 2001, 1-10. 27.Leganchuk, A., Zhai, S., Buxton, W. Manual and Cogni- tive Benefits of Two-Handed Input: An Experimental Study. In ACM Transactions on Computer-Human In- teraction, Vol5, No.4, 1998. 28.Miller, G.A. The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on our Capacity for Information Processing. Psychological Review (63), 1956, 81-97. 29.Norman, D. A., and Bobrow, D. G. On Data-Limited and Resource-Limited Processes. In Cognitive Psychol- ogy (7), 1975, 44-64. 30.Osborn, A.F. Applied Imagination: Principles and Pro- cedures of Creative Thinking. New York, Scribner, New York, 1953. 31.Pinelle, D., Gutwin, C., Greenberg, S., Task Analysis for Groupware Usability Evaluation: Modeling Shared- Workspace Tasks with the Mechanics of Collaboration. In ACM Tansactions on Computer-Human Interaction (TOCHI), Vol.10 Issue 4, 2003, 281-311. 32.Robertson, G. Czerwinski, M. Baudisch, P. Meyers, B. Robbins, D. Smith, G. Tan, D. Large Display User Ex- perience. In IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications, Vol. 25, Issue 4, 2005, 44-51,. 33.Santanen, E. L., Briggs, R. O., and de Vreede, G. 1999. A cognitive network model of creativity: a renewed fo- cus on brainstorming methodology. In Proceeding of the 20th International Conference on Information Systems (1999). Association for Information Systems, Atlanta, GA, 489-494. 34.Scott, S.D., Grant, K., D., & Mandryk, R.: System Guidelines for Co-located, Collaborative Work on Ta- bletop Display. System Guidelines for Co-located Col- laborative Work on a Tabletop Display. In Proceedings of ECSCW 2003, 159-178. 35.Scott, S.D., Carpendale, M.S.T, & Inkpen, K.M.: Terri- toriality in Collaborative Tabletop Workspaces. In Pro- ceedings of CSCW 2004. 36.Sellen, A. J., & Harper, R. (2001). The myth of the pa- perless office.MIT Press. 37.Streitz, N.A., Geißler, J., Holmer, T., Konomi, S., Müller-Tomfelde, C., Reischl, W., Rexroth, P.,Seitz, P., and Steinmetz, R. , i-LAND: An interactive Landscape for Creativitiy and Innovation. In Proceedings of CHI 1999. ACM Press, 120-127. 38.SmartTech DViT http://www.smarttech.com/DViT/ 39. Terrenghi, L., Fritsche, T., Butz,A.: The EnLighTable: De- sign ofAffordances to Support Collaborative Creativity. In Proceedings of Smart Graphics Symposium 2006, 206-217. 40.Tatar, D.G., Foster, G., Bobrow, D.G. (1991). Design for Conversation: Lessons from Cognoter. In Interna- tional Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 34:185-209, 1991. 41.Tang, J.C., Findings from observational studies of col- laborative work. In International Journal of Man- Machine Studies (1991), 34, 143-160. 42.Van Gundy, A.B. (1981). Techniques of Structured Problem Solving. NY. Van Nostrand, Reinhold. 43.Van Der Lugt, R., Functions of Sketching in Design Idea Generation Meetings. In Proceedings of the 4th Conference on Creativity & Cognition,2002, 72-79. 146