SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 22
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
College Students and Community Service:
Who, With Whom, and Why?
KIMBERLEEJ. TRUDEAUAND ANN SLOAN DEVLINl
Connecticut College
The community service volunteer vs. nonvolunteer status of 124 college students
was examined in relation to motivation, gender, extraversion-introversion,and social
anxiety. Volunteers and female students were hypothesized to have higher levels of
motivation to participate than were nonvolunteers and males, and to have higher
levels of interest in volunteering. The data for volunteer status and gender were
significant only with respect to interest in volunteering, with volunteers and females
significantly more interested in volunteering than were nonvolunteers and males.
Gender differences were also found for volunteering stylepreference and participation
within particular volunteer groups. Volunteers were also hypothesized to have lower
levels of extraversion and higher levels of social anxiety than nonvolunteers because
socially anxious introverts were thought to seek the anxiety-reducing structure of a
volunteer activity. This hypothesis was not supported.
The importance of community serviceto students today has implications for
the future of the nation (Fitch, 1987).The 1989U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics
(1993) reported that 28.1% of the college student population were volunteers.
Students volunteer with children, with the elderly, with each other. Who are
these students? Why do they volunteer? What demographics and personality
characteristics describe them? With which groups and individuals do they
volunteer?
Volunteering, like many prosocial behaviors, is influenced by both egoistic
and altruistic motivations. For example, personal growth was selected as their
central life meaning by 34 university student volunteers in a service organiza-
tion much more frequently than in a comparable group of 106 university
student nonvolunteers (D’Braunstein & Ebersole, 1992). Intrinsic-altruistic
and egoistic motives together constitute what the volunteer considers a reward-
ing experience (Cnaan & Goldberg-Glen, 1991). Serow (1991) viewed this
combination in a positive light, “It is noteworthy that so many students are
capable of deriving satisfaction from helping those in need” (p. 556).
‘Correspondence to concerning this article should be addressed to Ann Sloan Devlin, Box
5448, Department of Psychology, Connecticut College, 270 Mohegan Avenue, New London,
CT 06320-4196.
1867
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1996, 26, 21, pp. 1867-1888.
Copyright 0 1996 by V. H. Winston & Son, Inc. All rights reserved.
1868 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
Studies examining the length of commitment to a volunteer project provide
information about what motivates volunteers. Rubien and Thorelli (1985)
found that volunteers are less likely to leave the organization if their ego needs
are satisfied (e.g., believing that what they are doing is helpful). Other studies
(Miller, 1985; Wiehe & Isenhour, 1977) have discussed the importance of
personal satisfaction to length of participation; volunteers want to learn new
skills and to use these to benefit the organization.
Sensitivity to the interests and motivations of volunteers may well affect
agency recruiting efforts. Wiehe and Isenhour (1977) suggested that volunteer
coordinators consider the interests and opinions of their volunteers when
assigning tasks to be accomplished, as volunteers who felt well-utilized and
appreciated were more likely to continue volunteering in the organization. This
sensitivity to the needs of volunteers was also encouraged by Gillespie and
King (1985) in order to ensure future volunteer participation, as well as
program effectiveness; “training and skills” was cited as the single most
important item by the majority of participants in their study. Phillips (1982)
added that recruiters must describe not only the gains, but also the purpose of
the program so that the volunteers would believe they were benefiting from, as
well as participating in, something worthwhile. For example, year-long volun-
teers in an ombudservice program cited advocating for the elderly as their
motivation for participation (Nathanson & Eggleton, 1993).
Among the motives for volunteerism that emerge with some frequency in
the literature are those of a spiritual, altruistic, and affiliative nature. Religious
motives for volunteer participation have emerged in a number of studies
(Batson, Oleson, Weeks, & Healy, 1989; Fitch, 1991; Serow, 1989). Serow
(1989) reported that public university students provided less service than
students at institutions with a strong religious emphasis. Within the student
population studied by Serow and Dreyden (1990), spiritual and religious values
were positively associated with community service. Knapp and Holzberg
(1964) also found that volunteers were slightly more religious, as well as more
morally concerned and more compassionate.
Altruism also frequently appears as a motivational force in volunteerism
(Gelineau & Kantor, 1969;Gillespie & King, 1985;Wiehe & Isenhour, 1977).
Cnaan and Goldberg-Glen (1991) created a 28-motive questionnaire from an
extensive survey of the literatureon motivation and volunteerism. They discov-
ered that an altruistic motive, “the opportunity to do something worthwhile,”
was the most popular response. This altruistic response was followed by an
egoistic one: “Volunteering for others makes me feel better about myself.” In
a study of student volunteers, Fitch (1987) found that the highest rated motive
was egoistic: “It gives me a good feelingor senseof satisfactionto help others.”
The second highest motive was altruistic: “I am concerned about those less
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1869
fortunate than me.” The third motive was affiliative; people were involved in
community service because of “the people [they] meet and friendships [they]
make with other volunteers.”
The presence of affiliation motivation in community service participants
also has been observed in several studies. Volunteers have been found to desire
close relationships more than nonvolunteers Koopman (as cited in Abdennur,
1987). Interviewed college students often said that they would volunteer to
compensate for a lost, lacking, or distant relationship in their lives (Serow,
1991).A study of divorced elderly women who volunteered revealed that these
women “felt that their involvement in social programs allowed them to fill
otherwise empty, lonely hours and to once again recapture a sense of pride and
purpose” (Cain, 1988, p. 568). A study of volunteer recruiting (Abrami &
Perry, 1976) showed that students were positively disposed toward programs
that used the persuasions of “personal growth’’and opportunities for “meaning-
ful relationships.” Inquiry into the significance of affiliation motivation in a
student’s decision to volunteer could have value in future recruiting attempts.
Beyond the motivations of altruism and affiliation, personality charac-
teristics provide another salient factor in the willingness to volunteer. A num-
ber of characteristics have been examined, including extraversion-introversion.
However, the results of these studies invite further exploration, as both extrav-
ersion and introversion have predicted volunteering. Spitz and MacKinnon
(1993) describe successful Big Brother/Big Sister volunteers (those who follow
through on their commitment) as “persons suited to making new acquaintances
and maintaining a positive, comfortable atmosphere within the relationships”
(p. 816).Other research done within the framework of volunteering for psycho-
logical research indicates that extraversion predicted volunteering (Dollinger
& Leong, 1993).
At the same time, students who volunteered with mental patients were
found to be more introverted than the general student population (Knapp &
Holzberg, 1964), and Hobfoll (1980) reports that volunteers could not be
clearly distinguished from nonvolunteers in regard to personality charac-
teristics associated with the “helping personality.” Further, Fox (1984) states
that while there are particular social situations that distinguish the affiliative
tendencies of extroverts and introverts, “extroverts do not have a stronger
general need for social contact which finds expression in all types of situations
and in relation to all categories of companions” (p. 9). Perhaps Snyder’s (1993)
functional approach best describes the relationship between personality char-
acteristics such as extraversion-introversionand volunteering in that “different
people have different motivations for engaging in the same acts of volunteer-
ism” (p. 256); and that different personality characteristics accompany the
different motivations.
1870 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
Theoretically, extroverts are sensitive to the feedback they receive from
others and are interested in the social contact a group would provide (Cooper
& Scalise, 1974); extroverts could thus be expected to have lower social
anxiety in a group situation while introverts could be expected to experience
more social anxiety. A desire to belong to a group has been shown to be
inversely related to anxiety (Anant, 1967).
Several studies have found a relationship between extraversion-introver-
sion and anxiety (DeMan & Efraim, 1988;Teichman, 1974). Extraverts in one
studytended to report high self-esteem and low trait anxiety (DeMan& Efraim,
1988).Teichman’s (1974) data indicated that when highly aroused, participants
with a high predisposition for anxiety did not desire company, whereas subjects
with a low predisposition for anxiety preferred to affiliate with others. Intro-
verts might be likely to seek affiliation in the moderately structured setting of
a volunteer organization. Perhaps introverts also seek social stimulation in the
structured relationship a volunteer situation provides (Knapp & Holzberg,
1964). In particular, introverts may be attracted to a volunteer setting because
it provides a safe environment in which to satisfy needs for affiliation. Thus,
socially anxious introverts may find the organized activity (e.g.. tutoring, child
care, meal basket preparation for AIDS patients) of a volunteer placement
fulfills their affiliative needs in a manner that is tolerable in terms of social
anxiety.
Beyond personality characteristics, factors such as gender have been exam-
ined in relationship to volunteerism. Female students were more likely to be
volunteers than were male students (Fitch, 1991; Serow, 1990). Furthermore,
male and female volunteers may have different volunteer group preferences.
Signorella and Vegega (1984) confirmed the hypothesis that the degree of
research involvement by male and female students is topic-dependent. There
may well be prototypical male and female volunteer domains and volunteering
style preferences.
Building on the wide-ranging research examining volunteerism, the present
studyexaminescollegestudentmotivation,gender,and extraversion-introversion
in relation to community service. The researchers anticipate that more female
students than male students will be (a) volunteers, (b) more motivated to
volunteer, and (c) more interested in volunteering. It is also hypothesized that
volunteers will have lower levels of extraversion and higher levels of social
anxiety and social avoidance than nonvolunteers because a volunteer setting
provides a safe environment for introverts to satisfy needs for affiliation.
Introverts want to affiliate but fear social groups, leading to higher social
anxiety; they should have higher social avoidance scores. In addition, it is
predicted that volunteer style preferences and actual participation in the volun-
teer groups will be related to gender.
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1871
Method
Subjects
Subjectswere 124studentsfrom a small liberal arts college. Of these, 66.1%
(n= 82)were women, 33.1 % (n= 41) were men, and one subjectdid not specify
his or her gender. Students who had volunteered on at least one occasion while
in college comprised 62.1% (n = 77) of the sample; the remaining 37.9% (n =
47) of the students were nonvolunteers. Of the female students, 68.3% (n= 56)
were volunteers and 31.7%(n = 26) were nonvolunteers. Of the male students,
48.8% (n = 20) were volunteers and 51.2% (n = 21) were nonvolunteers.
Materials
Four questionnaires were used in this study. The first scale, the Eysenck
Personality Inventory (EPI; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968), is a 54-item test that
measures introversion-extraversion using a true-false format. Eysenck and
Eysenck (1968) established that their scale had good test-retest (.80to .97) and
split-half reliability (Spearman-Brown = .74 to .91), as well as construct and
concurrent validity. Vingoe (1968) also found that the EPI had criterion and
construct validity using self-ratings and the California Psychological Inventory
(Gough, 1957), respectively. Any items that showed large gender differences
were eliminated during the construction of the scale. Farley (1971) confirmed
this when his study demonstrated that there were no significant gender differ-
ences in the scores of men and women.
The second instrument, containing a list of 23 motives to volunteer, is the
Motives to Volunteer Scale that was composed by Cnaan and Goldberg-Glen
(1991) after a thorough survey of the literature on motivation and community
service.Of the original 28 motives, 22 of them loaded on one factor (Cronbach’s
a = .86). “Gaining practical experience toward paid employment” was in-
cluded as the 23rd item because the authors thought that this motive would be
important to college students, although it did not load on Cnaan and Goldberg-
Glen’s unnamed major factor.
Students rated the importance of various motives to participate in commu-
nity service on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1(not important) to 5 (very
important). Cnaan and Goldberg-Glen (1991) asserted that their scale pos-
sessed “pragmatic validity” because volunteers received higher scores than
nonvolunteers within their sample. Using factor analysis, they found that this
“unidimensional phenomenon,” as they described it, did not distinguish altru-
istic volunteers from egoistic volunteers; instead, they concluded that volun-
teers were motivated by both altruistic and egoistic intentions.
1872 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
The third instrument is a list of 25 organizations and programs with which
students might hypothetically volunteer (e.g., tutoring students, assisting hos-
pital patients, helping the homeless) that was developed by the authors. Stu-
dents rated (a) the extent of their interest in volunteering with each of these
programs on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not very interested) to
5 (very interested); (b) when applicable, the age group(s) with whom they
would prefer working within each program (i.e., children, adolescents, adults);
and (c) whether or not they had previous volunteer experience with each
program. At the end of this list were eight semantic differential items repre-
senting the different styles of volunteering. This list, also developed by the
authors, contained the polar anchors of academicfocus versus social/personal
focus, short-term (day/week) versus long term (semester or more), with train-
ing versus without training, with other college students versus by oneself,
one-on-one versus group situations, planned projects versus flexibility, with
supervision versus independently, and morning versus evening. Using a 5-point
differential, students marked the number indicating their preference. For
example, students who preferred an academic focus to a personal one would
circle 1; with no particular preference, they circled 3; and with a personal
focus, they circled 5. On this same page, biographical information was re-
quested, including gender, present class year, volunteer experience in high
school, and volunteer experience in college (class year and organization).
The fourth instrument is the Social Avoidance and Distress Scale (SAD),
developedby Watson and Friend (1969)to measure social anxiety. This bipolar
scale was reported by Watson and Friend to have (a) test-retest reliability of .68
after a month, (b) convergent validity (i.e., subjects who scored high on the
SAD avoided and/or were anxious in social situations), and (c) discriminant
validity (i.e., the SAD correlates -.25 with the Crowne-Marlowe Social Desir-
ability Scale; Crowne & Marlowe, 1964).Leary (1991) stated that the original
true-false format had been abandoned for the more reliable 5-point Likert scale
(the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient is close to .90),therefore the Likert scale was
employed in this study. We were unable to locate any studies that had actually
used the Likert scale, and therefore were unable to acquire further documenta-
tion regarding the validity of the Likert format of the SAD.
Procedure
The solicitation of the 150 potential subjects of this convenience sample
occurred at the college’s spring semester Volunteer Fair as they left the fair, as
well as in the college library foyer as students entered. All students were
informed that the study would take about 15min to complete and were thanked
for their participation. Prospective subjects read a consent form, then signed a
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1873
separate sheet with their name. The majority of the questionnaires were filled
out at the location where the subjects received them (i.e., the fair or the library).
One of the authors remained at that location to collect the completed question-
naires from the subjects as they were leaving. Some completed questionnaires
were mailed to the authors. A total of 150 questionnaires were distributed; 124
of these (82.7%)were returned. Of the 33 surveys distributed at the Volunteer
Fair, 32 were returned-a return rate of 97.0%.Of the 117surveys distributed
at the library, 92 were returned-a return rate of 78.6%.Psychology students
who desired research credit wrote their campus box numbers on an indicated
line at the end of the questionnaire and anonymous credit slips were put directly
into their boxes by the authors. Those who wanted a summaryof the results also
gave their box numbers to the authors.
Results
VolunteerMotivation and Interest Levels
To evaluate the hypotheses that volunteers have both higher levels of
motivation to participate in community service and higher levels of interest in
volunteering, and that female students have significantly higher levels of these
two dependent variables than do ma!sstpd,ents, a 2 x 2 (Volunteer vs. Nonvol-
unteer x Gender) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was per-
formed. A Pearson correlation indicated that there was a positive linear
relationship between the levels of motivation and interest (r = .20, p < .05).
There were main effects for volunteer status, Wilks’s h = .892,F(2,91) = 5.49,
p < .01, and for gender, Wilks’s h =.771,F(2,91) = 1 3 . 4 8 , ~<: .001, as well as
an interactionbetween volunteer status and gender, Wilks’s h = .933,F(2,91) =
3.27,p < .05, on the total interest score.
Univariate analyses revealed that there was a main effect for volunteer
status on the total interest score, F( 1,92) = 11.09,p < .005,but not on the total
motivation score, F( 1, 92) = 0.35,p > .05. As hypothesized, volunteers were
significantly more interested in volunteering (M= 77.92) than were nonvolun-
teers (M = 68.43). Contrary to the hypothesis, there was no main effect of
volunteer status on the total motivation scores for volunteers (M= 67.59) and
nonvolunteers (M = 65.00). Examination of the data via scatterplots revealed
no indication of curvilinearity.
Univariate analyses of the total interest scores by gender were statistically
significant, F(1,92) = 25.69, p < .001. The hypothesis that female students
(M= 79.25) were significantly more interested in participating in the volunteer
programs than were male students (M = 64.42) was supported. Although the
motivation scores of the female students (M = 68.37) were not significantly
1874 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
Table 1
Summary of Results of Simple Effects Test of Level of Interestfor Gender
Volunteer Nonvolunteer F(1,92)
Male 72.18 55.00 12.71**
Female 80.14 77.38 ns
F(1992) 4.43* 23.36**
*p< .05. **p < .01.
higher than the motivation scores of the males (M = 62.97), as had been
predicted, the analysis approached significance, F(1, 92) = 3.68,p < .06.
In addition to these main effects, there was also an interaction effect
between volunteer status and gender. Univariate analyses revealed that the
interaction involved the dependent variable total interest, F( 1, 92) = 5.80,p <
.05. The results of the simple effects follow-up tests are shown in Table 1.
Specific MotivationaI Factors
To further investigate the motivations of students and the volunteer groups
of interest to them, two separate orthogonal factor analyses were executed on
these dependent variables. The 23 items of the Motives to Volunteer scale
(Cnaan & Goldberg-Glen, 1991) were divided into factors using a principal
components analysis. Although seven individual factors with eigenvalues
greater than 1.OO were identified, an examination of those eigenvalues with a
scree test suggested five major factors, and it was thus decided to rotate five
factors. When the principal components analysis was rerun to produce five
factors, the fifth factor was found to have only two items on it and, as both of
these items had factor loading scores less than 0.30, this factor was also
dropped and the analysis was rerun to produce four factors. These remaining
four factors had eigenvalues of above 1SO.The chosen cut off point for factor
loadings was 0.45 because it created a more coherent structure for each factor.
The three items that did not load 0.45 on any factor (“opportunity to work with
different age groups”; “opportunity for relationships”; and “people in my
community volunteer”) were dropped from the respective factors. Two more
items were dropped because they loaded highly (0.45 or better) on two factors
(“excellent educational experience” and “provides challenging activities”).
The first factor, labeled “social obligation,” included five items that sug-
gested an institutional commitment to volunteerism: “It is God’s expectation”;
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1875
“agency can provide more for less”; “continuing a family tradition”; “agency
is prestigious”; and “previous contact with professionals in this agency”
(Cronbach’s a = .73). The second factor, labeled “experience-seeking,’’ in-
cluded four items that suggested the desire to gain and share experience: “able
to relate to clients due to my own similar experience”; “experience in providing
service”; “my employerlschool expect it”; and “gaining practical experience
toward paid employment”(Cronbach’sa = .70).The third factor, labeled‘taltru-
istic intention,” included four items: “opportunity to do something worth-
while”; “it creates a better society”; “opportunity to return my good fortune”;
and “opportunity to change injustices” (Cronbach’s a =.66). The fourth factor,
labeled “personal motivation,” included five items that expressed reasons
that people help others while helping themselves: “makes me feel better
about myself’; “it improves my attitude on my own life situation”; “opportu-
nity to vary my activities”; “broadening my horizons”; and “nothing else to do
with my time” (Cronbach’s a =.70; see Table 2 for factor loadings).
To examine the relationship between volunteer status and gender and the
importance of the individual factor motives to volunteer, a 2 x 2 (Volunteer
Status x Gender) MANOVA was conducted on the unitized factor scores of
each of these factors. Although there were no interaction effects, Wilks’s h =
.940, F(4, 107)= 1 . 7 0 , ~> .05, or main effects for volunteer status, Wilks’s h =
.938, F(4, 107)= 1 . 7 6 , ~< .05, there was a main effect for gender, Wilks’s h =
.913, F(4, 107) = 2 . 5 7 , ~< .05. Univariate analyses demonstrated that female
students (M= 5.94) were significantly more motivated by the altruistic inten-
tion factor than were male students (M= 14.08),F(1, 110)= 8 . 5 0 , ~< .005.
Volunteer Group Types
An orthogonal principal cornponents analysis was executed on the 25
volunteer groups. Eight factors with eigenvalues of above 1.OO were identified.
An examination of those eigenvalues with the scree test suggested the analysis
be rerun with five factors. The remaining factors had eigenvalues of above
1.45. The 0.45 item factor loading score was chosen as an inclusion criterion,
as before, to create a more coherent factor structure. One item that did not load
0.45 on any factor was dropped from further analyses (“preschool children
[daycare]”). One item loaded highly on two factors and was also dropped
(“battered women”). The first factor, labeled the “socially discriminated
against” group included six items: “substance abusers”; “survivors of sexual
abuselrape”;“teen mothers”; “juveniledelinquents”; “bilgay teens”; and “prison
inmates” (Cronbach’sa = .77). The second factor, labeledthe “medical-disaster
relief,” group includedfour items: “cancerpatients”; “AIDS patients”; “hospital
1876 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
Table 2
VarimaxRotated Factor Loadings of Motives to Volunteer and Percentage of
Variance
Motive
% of
Loading variance
Social obligation
Agency is prestigious.
Agency can provide more for less.
It is God’s expectation.
Continuing a family tradition.
Previous contact with professionals in this agency
Gaining practical experience toward paid
My employer/school expects it.
Experience in providing service.
Able to relate to clients due to my own similar
Experience seeking
employment.
experience.
Altruistic intention
Opportunity to do something worthwhile.
It creates a better society.
Opportunity to change injustices.
Opportunity to return my good fortune.
Makes me feel better about myself.
It improves my attitude on my own life situation.
Opportunity to vary my activities.
Broadening my horizons.
Personal motivation
.69
.68
.61
.59
.57
.83
.67
.57
.49
.69
.69
.61
.50
.67
.66
.52
.47
24.7
10.3
7.6
6.9
patients”; and “disaster relief’ (Cronbach’s a = .74). The third factor, labeled
the “teaching-mentoring” group, included five items: “the illiterate”; “teach
for America”; “tutoring students”; “bilingual students”; and “Big BrotherdSis-
ters” (Cronbach’s a = .74). The fourth factor, labeled the “special needs”
group, included five items: “mentally retarded”; “elderly”; “homeless (soup
kitchens)”; “mentally ill”; and “Habitat for Humanity” (Cronbach’s a = .74).
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1877
The fifth factor, labeled the “social causes-sports” group, included three items:
“political groups”; “environmental groups”; and “Sports teams (coaching)”
(Cronbach’s a =.44; see Table 3 for factor loadings).
The multivariate analyses conducted with the unitized scores of these five
interest factors by volunteer status and gender produced no significant interac-
tion, Wilks’s h = .938,F(5,95)= 1 . 2 6 , ~> .05, nor significant main effects for
volunteer status,Wilks’s h = .918,F(5,95) = 1 . 7 1 , ~> .05. However, there was
a main effect for gender on the interest factors, Wilks’s h = .651, F(5, 95) =
1 0 . 1 6 , ~< .001.
Univariate analyses of variance reiterated that female students were more
interested in volunteering with the different groups than were male students in
four of the five factor types (see Table 4 for detailed statistics).
Additional GenderDifferences
The hypothesis that gender differences exist in the types of activities in
which students actually volunteered was tested using a series of chi-squares,
one for each of the 25 provided programs, with two additions. Women’s groups
and youth groups were also included because several students had listed these
among the groups with which they volunteered. To guard against the possibility
of Type I error, the Bonferroni test was used to set the significance level at .002
(.05/27). One significant difference emerged. Analyses revealed that female
students had volunteered with preschool children, ~ ~ ( 1 ,N = 123) = 12.66,p <
.002, significantly more than had male students.
Gender differences also were anticipated with respect to the styles of
volunteering preferred by the participants. The possibility of gender differ-
ences was tested with a MANOVA using the eight volunteering styles as the
dependent variables. There was a main effect for gender, Wilks’s h = ,805,
F(8, 110) = 3.33, p < .005. Univariate analyses indicated that males and
females differed with respect to two of the eight volunteering styles: how long
subjects preferred to volunteer (short term, consisting of a day or week com-
mitment, or long term, lasting for a semester of more) and whether or not they
preferred training within the organization. Female students preferred long-term
commitments ( M = 3.70) more than did male students (M= 2.90), F(1, 117)=
10.62,~< .005, and male students preferred to volunteer without training (M=
3.18) more than did female students (M = 2.26), F( 1, 117) = 13.40,p < .OO 1
(see Table 5 for means and standard deviations of styles).
Extraversionand Social Anxiety
Preliminary analyses revealed that the mean and standard deviation of the
extraversion-introversion scores of the current study were 12.61 and 4.01,
1878 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
Table 3
VarirnaxRotated Factor Loadings of Interest in VolunteerPrograms
Factor Loading % of variance
Socially discriminated against 25.9
Substance abusers .73
Survivors of sexual abuse .70
Teen mothers .64
Juvenile delinquents .58
Prison inmates .52
Bi/gay teens .57
Medical-disaster relief
Cancer patients
AIDS patients
Hospital patients
Disaster relief
Teaching-mentoring
The illiterate
Teach for America
Tutoring students
Bilingual students
Big BrotherdBig Sisters
Special needs
Mentally retarded
Elderly
Homeless (soup kitchens)
Mentally ill
Habitat for Humanity
.76
.65
.59
.57
8.7
6.9
.76
.74
.73
.62
.49
6.6
.78
.72
.59
.53
.49
Social causes-sports 6.0
Political groups .73
Environmental groups .55
Sports teams .53
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1879
Table 4
Means and F Statistics of Interest Factorsfor Gender
Gender
Factor Females Males F(L99)
Socially discriminated against 18.68 13.52 24.81****
Medical-disaster relief 13.41 9.86 25.81****
Teaching-mentoring 20.35 17.46 8.22**
Special needs 15.40 13.11 ns
Social causes-sports 7.26 9.26 ns
**p< .01. ****p< .001.
respectively.The mean and standard deviation of Eysenck and Eysenck's (1968)
original sample of college students (N = 1,003) were 13.10 and 4.10, respec-
tively. The mean of the SAD scores was 38.55 with a standard deviation of
20.13. A Pearson correlation revealed that there was a negative linear correla-
tion between the levels of extraversion and social anxiety (r = -.55,p < .Ol).
To evaluate the hypothesis that volunteers have lower levels of extraversion
and higherlevelsof socialanxietythannonvolunteers,a MANOVA wasperformed
on these two dependent variables. The hypothesis was not supported, Wilks's
h = .976, F(2, 121)= 1 . 5 0 , ~> .05; there were no significant main effects for
extraversion,F(l, 122)= 0 . 2 0 , ~>.05,or socialanxiety,F(1,122)= 2 . 7 2 , ~> .05.
A standard regression was performed to identify whether extraversion
and/or social anxiety in part predict total level of volunteer participation
(operationalized as the number of different groups with whom one had volun-
teered). Extraversion and social anxiety were regressed on the total number of
volunteer programs in which the students participated. The Pearson correla-
tions between extraversion and the number of volunteer programs was not
significant (r = .O 1,p > .05) while the correlation between social anxiety and
the number of volunteer programs was significant (r = -.27,p < .01). Consistent
with the correlational analysis, the regression analysis showed that the level of
extraversion did not contribute significantly to the model (p = -.21, p > .05),
but that social anxiety explained 10%of the variance in level of participation
in volunteer programs (R2= .lo), F(2, 101)= 5 . 7 0 , ~< .01; p = -.39, p < .01.
The b-weight score (-.06) indicates that as social anxiety increases, the level of
volunteer participation decreases.
1880 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
Table 5
Means and Standard Deviations of Semantic Differential Styles of
Volunteeringfor Gender
Gender
-
Style of volunteering Males Females
Academic focus vs. social/personal focus
M
SD
Short-term vs. long-term
M
SD
With training vs. without training
A4
SD
With other college students vs. by oneself
M
SD
One on one vs. group situations
M
SD
Planned projects vs. flexibility
A4
SD
M
SD
With supervision vs. independently
Morning vs. evening
M
SD
3.59
0.88
2.90
1.09
3.18
1.40
2.95
1.30
2.64
1.31
3.33
1.40
3.56
1.29
3.26
1.31
3.50
1.09
3.70***
1.40
2.26****
1.23
3.30
1.22
2.38
1.16
3.44
1.17
3.41
1.16
3.00
1.26
Note. The above means correspond to a semantic differential scale on which 1 indicates
a strong preference for the first item through 5 which indicates a strong preference for
the item opposite to it.
***p< .005. ****p< .001.
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1881
Table 6
Frequency of Participation in VolunteerPrograms in Descending Order
No. of
students
Program (N= 124)
No. of
students
Program (N= 124)
Preschool children
Tutoring students
Homeless (soup kitchens)
Elderly
Sports teams (coaching)
Environmental groups
Hospital patients
Bilingual students
Political groups
Mentally retarded
Mentally ill
Habitat for Humanity
Big Brothers/Big Sisters
Juvenile delinquents
72
70
46
44
38
38
31
30
29
28
27
24
24
15
Survivors of sexual abuse
Substance abusers
Youth groups
The illiterate
Cancer patients
Battered women
AIDS patients
Bi/gay teens
Prison inmates
Disaster relief
Teach for America
Teen mothers
Women’s groups
14
13
12
12
10
9
8
7
6
6
5
5
4
Participant Characteristics
Chi-square analyses provided information about the participant pool. Sig-
nificantly more female students (n = 56) than male students (n = 20) were
volunteers, x2(1,N = 123)=4 . 4 1 , ~< .05.Significantly more students who had
volunteered in high school also volunteered in college, x2(1, N = 124) = 8.61,
p < .005.Finally, significantly more juniors and seniors volunteered than did
not, x2(3,N= 123)= 1 8 . 3 5 , ~< .0005.
Frequency analyses or means provided information about students’ volun-
teer program participation in high school and college, their motivation to
volunteer with particular programs, and the extent to which the students,
overall, were motivated by each of the items on the Motives to Volunteer Scale
(Cnaan & Goldberg-Glen, 1991). As illustrated in Table 6, these analyses
revealed that of the 124 subjects, 58.1% (n = 72) had volunteered with pre-
school children, 56.5% (n= 70) of the subjects had tutored students, 37.1% (n =
46) of the subjects had volunteered to help the homeless, and 35.5% (n = 44)
1882 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
Table 7
Means and Standard Deviations of Interest in VolunteerPrograms in
Descending Order
Program M SD
Tutoring students
Big Brothers/Sisters
Homeless
Preschool children
AIDS patients
Teach for America
Battered women
Habitat for Humanity
Survivors of abuse
The illiterate
Cancer patients
Environmental groups
Hospital patients
Disaster relief
Elderly
Bilingual students
Juvenile delinquents
Teen mothers
Mentally ill
Substance abusers
Sports teams (coaching)
Prison inmates
Mentally retarded
Bi/gay teens
Political groups
3.65
3.63
3.45
3.37
3.26
3.21
3.18
3.16
3.15
3.13
3.12
3.05
3.03
3.03
2.94
2.93
2.90
2.84
2.76
2.75
2.71
2.68
2.67
2.65
2.53
1.46
1.14
1.24
1.46
1.32
1.33
1.37
1.32
1.44
1.23
1.16
1.34
1.21
1.26
1.21
1.44
1.21
1.30
1.43
1.30
1.58
1.28
1.26
1.24
1.45
Note. The above means indicate level of interest, 1 = not interested to 5 = very
interested.
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1883
Table 8
Means and Standard Deviations of Motives to Volunteer
Motive M SD
Opportunity to do something worthwhile.
It creates a better society.
Makes me feel better about myself.
Broadening my horizons.
Excellent educational experience.
Opportunity to change injustices.
Opportunity to return my good fortune.
Provides challenging activities.
It improves my attitude on my own life situation.
Experience in providing service.
Opportunity to vary my activities.
Opportunity for relationships.
Opportunity to work with different age groups.
Gaining practical experience toward paid employment.
Able to relate to clients due to my own similar experience.
Agency can provide more for less.
Previous contact with professional in this agency.
It is God’s expectation.
My employerlschool expects it.
Nothing else to do with my time.
4.29
4.02
3.92
3.86
3.81
3.57
3.50
3.49
3.49
3.44
3.21
3.21
3.10
3.06
2.80
2.47
1.94
1.73
1.71
1.58
0.88
1.04
1.02
0.98
1.01
1.16
1.11
1.05
1.13
1.16
1.09
1.16
1.23
1.34
1.29
1.14
1.12
1.15
1.01
0.98
Note. The above means indicate how important each motive would be when deciding
whether or not to participate in community service, 1 = not important to 5 = very
important.
of the subjects had volunteered with the elderly (see Table 6 for frequencies of
participation in the volunteer programs).
With respect to interest in participating in the volunteer programs, student
tutors (M= 3.65), Big BrothedSisters (M= 3.63), the homeless (M= 3.45),
and preschool children (M=3.37) received the highest means of interest out of
a possible 5.00 signifying greatest interest. The elderly category (M= 2.94),
which was fourth in actual participation, ranked 15th in interest on the list of
25 (see Table 7 for means and standard deviations of interest in the volunteer
programs). Of the possible motives, the participants in this sample were most
1884 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
motivated by “opportunity to do something worthwhile” ( M = 4.29), “it creates
a better society” ( M = 4.02), “makes me feel better about myself’ ( M = 3.92),
and “broadening my horizons” (A4= 3.86; see Table 8 for means and standard
deviations of motives to volunteer).
Discussion
Human service experience is a valuable commodity in this economy because
the economy is “undergoing a shift from goods producingto service producing”
(Nathanson& Eggleton, 1993,p. 108).The majority of the studentsin the present
study are active in this social change; they volunteer. Students who volunteered
in high school also volunteer in college, suggestingthat volunteerism is affected
by psychological and social variables, not simply situational circumstances.
Contrary to expectation, levels of extraversion and social anxiety did not
predict volunteerism in this study. These results contradict those by Carlson
and Levy (1973), who found that volunteers were predominantly extraverted;
as well as the results of Knapp and Holzberg (1964), who found that volunteers
were predominantly introverted. The results of the present study suggest that
volunteer populations are diverse, including individuals with varying levels of
extraversion and social anxiety. Snyder’s (1993) functional analysis of volun-
teerism may best explain the varying levels of extraversion and social anxiety
and avoidance found in this study. The similarity between the statistics of the
original sample tested with the EPI (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968)and the present
sample further suggests that personality characteristics themselves are rela-
tively unaffected by generational pressures; students today appear to be no
more extraverted or introverted than they were in 1968.
It is important to note that the levelsof extraversionand social anxietyare not
independent.The significantnegative linear relationshipbetween these variables
demonstrates that extraverted subjects reported less social anxiety, and intro-
verted subjects reported more social anxiety. Though these dimensions had no
effect on volunteerstatusin the present study,this significantnegativecorrelation
indicates that these constructs have a conceptual relationshipthat may be further
explored.A problemwiththepresent studyisthat it didnot measurethe affiliation
motivation through a theoretical mixture of extraversion and social anxiety. If
one tested the need for affiliation and the number of hours one devotes to com-
munity service, this might also provide insight into the extent that affiliation
motivationaffectsthe activecommunityserviceparticipationof a collegestudent.
The positive linear correlationbetweenmotivation to volunteerand interest in
volunteeringwith the various programs also merits further study, as multivariate
analyses of these variables did not reveal consistent group differences. While
volunteers in the present study were more interested in volunteering than were
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1885
nonvolunteers, consistent with Hobfoll’s (1980) discussion of social interest
motivation for volunteer participation, volunteers in the present study were
not significantly more motivated than were nonvolunteers, which contradicts
Cnaan and Goldberg-Glen’s (1991) results. Either students have learned what
the socially accepted motives to volunteer are and, therefore, simply reproduced
them as responses on the questionnaire, or nonvolunteers are equally motivated
but have other obligations that precludetheir ability to participate in community
service.What this studyshowsis that volunteersare distinguishablefromnonvol-
unteersin that they are more interestedin participating in the availableprograms.
The results also revealed anticipated gender differences. Just as Signorella
and Vegega (1984) reported gender differences in volunteerism for particular
research topics, gender differences appear with regard to volunteering for a
particular program. Notably, significantly more female students have volun-
teered with preschool children than have male students. MANOVA indicated
that female students are also more motivated by the altruistic factor than are
male students. This evidence conflicts with an earlier review of research that
females are not more altruistic (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). In the authors’
view, it is not surprising that women had nearly significantly higher levels of
motivation to volunteer and that women are more interested in volunteering.
Female students may be more affiliative than male students, and thus more
interested in participating with the variety of populations represented by the
many volunteer programs. The programs on the survey may also appeal more
to women than to men, as female students are more interested in volunteering
with the programs represented in four of the five factors, whereas men are more
interested in volunteering only with the factor containing “political groups,”
“environmental groups,” and “sports teams.” While there are volunteer oppor-
tunities in which male students are more interested than are females, these
opportunities tend to mirror stereotypical interpretations of male interest.
The students at this small liberal arts college volunteer. They volunteer
predominantly with preschool children, with students who require tutoring,
with the homeless, and with the elderly. Perhaps these are the more popular
volunteer programs because youth, education, homelessness, and old age are
concerns of contemporary society.These programs might also attract the largest
number of volunteers because they provide students with more opportunities,
both in number and convenience. As women’s centers, AIDS programs, and
environmental projects receive more funding and, consequently, expand their
services, more opportunities to volunteer with these groups should develop.
The 10 volunteer programs in which students are most interested (but not
necessarilywherethey actuallyvolunteered)involvedopportunitiestoactastutors
or mentors; as counselors for AIDS patients, battered women, and survivors of
sexual abuse; and as carpenters and food providers for the homeless. Students
1886 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
are least interested in volunteering with political groups. This pattern might
indicate a growing concern for humanity, above policy, among students.
Of the 23 motives of the Motives to Volunteer Scale (Cnaan & Goldberg-
Glen, 1991), the 7 that received the highest ratings from the students were as
follows: the first two and two others loaded on the factor labeled altruistic, two
loaded on the personal motivation factor, and one loaded on the experience-
seeking factor. These results show that students recognize and appreciate
community service more as a way of giving back to the community than as a
way of gaining the benefits of personal satisfaction and practical experience.
These results reveal that altruism is a popular motivation for participation in
community service, which corroborates findings reported by Gelineau and
Kantor (1969), Gillespie and King (1985), and Wiehe and Isenhour (1977).
College students and community service: who, with whom, and why? The
findings of this study are significant. More women than men are volunteers.
Women are more interested in volunteering than are men. Women volunteer
more with preschool children than do men. Women are more motivated to
volunteer by altruistic motives than are men. Future research could explore
which characteristicsofwomen and men seem to predispose them to participate
in community service. This might be accomplished, in part, by comparing
gender role orientation to service participation.
References
Abdennur, A. (Ed.). (1987). The conflict resolution syndrome: Volunteerism,
violence, and beyond. Ottawa, Canada: University of Ottawa Press.
Abrami, P. C., & Perry, R. P. (1976). Recruitment of college volunteers for
community service organizations using the lecture method. Journal of
Community Psychology, 4,369-377.
Anant, S . S. (1967). Belongingness, anxiety, and self-sufficiency: Pilot study.
Psychological Reports, 20, 1137-1138.
Batson, C. D., Oleson, K. C., Weeks, J. L., & Healy, S. P. (1989). Religious
prosocial motivation: Is it altruistic or egoistic? Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 57,873-884.
Cain, B. S. (1988). Divorce among elderly women: A growing social phe-
nomenon. Special issue: Life transitions in the elderly. Social Casework,
Carlson, R., & Levy, N. (1973). Studies of Jungian typology: I. Memory,
social perception, and social action. Journal of Personality, 41, 559-576.
Cnaan,R. A., & Goldberg-Glen,R. S. (1991).Measuringmotivationto volunteer
in human services. Special issue: Methods for research and intervention
with organizations. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 27,269-284.
69, 563-568.
COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1887
Cooper, J., & Scalise, C. J. (1974). Dissonance produced by deviations from
life styles: The interaction of Jungian typology and conformity. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 4,566-571.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive: Studies in evalu-
ative dependence. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
D’Braunstein, S., & Ebersole, P. (1992). Categories of life meaning for service
organization volunteers. Psychological Reports, 70,281-282.
DeMan, A. F., & Efraim, D. P. (1988). Selected personality correlates of social
participation in university students. Journal of Social Psychology, 128,
Dollinger, S. J., & Leong, F. T. L. (1993). Volunteer bias and the five-factor
model. TheJournal of Psychology, 127,29-36.
Eysenck, H. I., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968). Eysenck Personality Inventory
manual. San Diego, CA: Educational 8c Industrial Testing Series.
Farley, F. H. (1971). Some EPI reliability estimates. Journal of Personality
Assessment, 35,364-366.
Fitch, R. T. (1987). Characteristics and motivations of college students volun-
teering for community service. Journal of College Student Personnel, 28,
Fitch, R. T. (1991). Differences among community service volunteers, extra-
curricular volunteers, and nonvolunteers on the college campus. Journal
of College Student Development, 32, 534-540.
Fox, S. (1984). The sociability aspect of extraversion as a situation-specific
dimension. Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 7-10.
Gelineau, V. A., & Kantor, D. (1969). Pro-social commitment among college
students. Journal of Social Issues, 20, 112-130.
Gillespie, D. F., & King, A. E. (1985). Demographic understanding of volun-
teerism. Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare,12,798-816.
Gough, H. G. (1957). California Psychological Inventory. Palo Alto, CA:
Consulting Psychologists Press.
Hobfoll, S. E. (1980). Personal characteristics of the college volunteer. Ameri-
can Journal of CommunityPsychology, 8,503-506.
Knapp, R. H., & Holzberg, J. D. (1964). Characteristics of college students
volunteering for service to mental patients. Journal of ConsultingPsychol-
Leary, M. R. (1991). Social anxiety, shyness, and related constructs. In J. P.
Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.),Measures ofperson-
ality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 161-194). San Diego, CA:
Academic.
Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
265-267.
424-431.
ogy, 28, 82-85.
1888 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN
Miller, L. E. (1985). Understanding the motivation of volunteers: An exami-
nation of personality differences and characteristics of volunteers’ paid
employment. Journal of VoluntaryAction Research, 14, 112-122.
Nathanson, I. I., & Eggleton, E. (1993). Motivation versus program effect on
length of service: A study of four cohorts of ombudservice volunteers.
Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 19,95-114.
Phillips, M. (1982). Motivation and expectation in successful volunteerism.
Journal of VoluntaryAction Research, 11, 118-125.
Rubien, A., & Thorelli, I. M. (1985). Egoistic motives and longevity of partici-
pation by social service volunteers. Journal of Applied Behavioral Sci-
ence, 20,223-235.
Serow, R. C. (1989). Community service, religious commitment, and campus
climate. Youth & Society, 21, 105-120.
Serow, R. C. (1990). Volunteering and values: An analysis of students’ par-
ticipation in community service. Journal of Research and Development,
Serow, R. C. (1991). Students and voluntarism: Looking into the motive of
community service participants. American Educational Research Journal,
Serow, R. C., & Dreyden, J. I. (1990). Community service among college and
university students: Individual and institutional relationships. Adoles-
cence, 25,553-566.
Signorella, M. L., & Vegega, M. E. (1984). A note on gender stereotyping of
research topics. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 107-109.
Snyder, M. (1993). Basic research and practical problems: The promise of a
“functional” personality and social psychology. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 19, 251-264.
Spitz, R. T., & MacKinnon, J. R. (1993). Predicting success in volunteer
community service. Psychological Reports, 73, 815-818.
Teichman, Y. (1974). Predisposition for anxiety and affiliation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 29,405-410.
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (1993). No. 614. Percent of adult population
doing volunteer work: 1989: Statistical Abstract of The United States
(p. 386). Washington, DC: U. S. Department of Commerce.
Vingoe, F. J. (1968). Validity of the Eysenck Extraversion Scale: Replication
and extension. Psychological Reports, 22, 706-708.
Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33,448-457.
Wiehe, V. R., & Isenhour, L. (1977). Motivation of volunteers. Journal of
Social Welfare,473-79.
23, 198-203.
28, 543-556.

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

research methods lit review
research methods lit reviewresearch methods lit review
research methods lit reviewDanielle Monahan
 
Minding a Healthy Body: Clarifying Media Roles as Primers in the Rating of Bo...
Minding a Healthy Body: Clarifying Media Roles as Primers in the Rating of Bo...Minding a Healthy Body: Clarifying Media Roles as Primers in the Rating of Bo...
Minding a Healthy Body: Clarifying Media Roles as Primers in the Rating of Bo...CrimsonpublishersPPrs
 
gjfmv1n2_2
gjfmv1n2_2gjfmv1n2_2
gjfmv1n2_2Jen W
 
National Trends in Prayer Use as a Coping Mechanism for Health Concerns: Chan...
National Trends in Prayer Use as a Coping Mechanism for Health Concerns: Chan...National Trends in Prayer Use as a Coping Mechanism for Health Concerns: Chan...
National Trends in Prayer Use as a Coping Mechanism for Health Concerns: Chan...Jonathan Dunnemann
 
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior pPrepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior pamit657720
 
Anonymity Versus Publicity of Answers and Reported Self-Esteem
Anonymity Versus Publicity of Answers and Reported Self-EsteemAnonymity Versus Publicity of Answers and Reported Self-Esteem
Anonymity Versus Publicity of Answers and Reported Self-EsteemDanielle Hoyt
 
Millennials in the Workplace copy
Millennials in the Workplace copyMillennials in the Workplace copy
Millennials in the Workplace copyD'Shai Hendricks
 
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955Hayley Gibson
 
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15Kyle Erwin
 
Teens Volunteering: Implications for mental health
Teens Volunteering: Implications for mental healthTeens Volunteering: Implications for mental health
Teens Volunteering: Implications for mental healthMichael Torres
 
Qualitative Research Final Paper
Qualitative Research Final PaperQualitative Research Final Paper
Qualitative Research Final PaperCherie Azzopardi
 

Was ist angesagt? (19)

Kuhtreiber Final Draft
Kuhtreiber Final DraftKuhtreiber Final Draft
Kuhtreiber Final Draft
 
research methods lit review
research methods lit reviewresearch methods lit review
research methods lit review
 
Ashleigh Vogle REU
Ashleigh Vogle REUAshleigh Vogle REU
Ashleigh Vogle REU
 
Final Draft ARP Paper
Final Draft ARP PaperFinal Draft ARP Paper
Final Draft ARP Paper
 
Minding a Healthy Body: Clarifying Media Roles as Primers in the Rating of Bo...
Minding a Healthy Body: Clarifying Media Roles as Primers in the Rating of Bo...Minding a Healthy Body: Clarifying Media Roles as Primers in the Rating of Bo...
Minding a Healthy Body: Clarifying Media Roles as Primers in the Rating of Bo...
 
gjfmv1n2_2
gjfmv1n2_2gjfmv1n2_2
gjfmv1n2_2
 
National Trends in Prayer Use as a Coping Mechanism for Health Concerns: Chan...
National Trends in Prayer Use as a Coping Mechanism for Health Concerns: Chan...National Trends in Prayer Use as a Coping Mechanism for Health Concerns: Chan...
National Trends in Prayer Use as a Coping Mechanism for Health Concerns: Chan...
 
Eyes of a Serial Killer
Eyes of a Serial KillerEyes of a Serial Killer
Eyes of a Serial Killer
 
Social psychology
Social psychologySocial psychology
Social psychology
 
Tomas final-brl-chapter
Tomas final-brl-chapterTomas final-brl-chapter
Tomas final-brl-chapter
 
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior pPrepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
Prepared by louise kaplan, ph d, arnp, fnp bc, faanp senior p
 
Anonymity Versus Publicity of Answers and Reported Self-Esteem
Anonymity Versus Publicity of Answers and Reported Self-EsteemAnonymity Versus Publicity of Answers and Reported Self-Esteem
Anonymity Versus Publicity of Answers and Reported Self-Esteem
 
Millennials in the Workplace copy
Millennials in the Workplace copyMillennials in the Workplace copy
Millennials in the Workplace copy
 
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
PSYC3002 Independent Study - 12006955
 
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
Erwin_Strain_APA_Poster_15
 
Teens Volunteering: Implications for mental health
Teens Volunteering: Implications for mental healthTeens Volunteering: Implications for mental health
Teens Volunteering: Implications for mental health
 
Owning the personal past
Owning the personal pastOwning the personal past
Owning the personal past
 
Qualitative Research Final Paper
Qualitative Research Final PaperQualitative Research Final Paper
Qualitative Research Final Paper
 
Group Pressure & Obedience to Authority
Group Pressure & Obedience to AuthorityGroup Pressure & Obedience to Authority
Group Pressure & Obedience to Authority
 

Andere mochten auch

Positive Social Action Conference 2010: Photo Essay
Positive Social Action Conference 2010: Photo EssayPositive Social Action Conference 2010: Photo Essay
Positive Social Action Conference 2010: Photo EssayJordan Thompson
 
Case study presentation
Case study presentationCase study presentation
Case study presentationlukabear
 
SVC 2009-2010 Survey Totals
SVC 2009-2010 Survey TotalsSVC 2009-2010 Survey Totals
SVC 2009-2010 Survey TotalsJordan Thompson
 
Do So Much Weekend Resource Guide
Do So Much Weekend Resource GuideDo So Much Weekend Resource Guide
Do So Much Weekend Resource GuideJordan Thompson
 
SB4 2.24 Screening Day B - Analytical Statement w/ Commentary
SB4 2.24 Screening Day B - Analytical Statement w/ CommentarySB4 2.24 Screening Day B - Analytical Statement w/ Commentary
SB4 2.24 Screening Day B - Analytical Statement w/ CommentaryKindled Class Consulting
 
Social Media For Social Good: a guide to
Social Media For Social Good: a guide toSocial Media For Social Good: a guide to
Social Media For Social Good: a guide toJordan Thompson
 
Photocomposition
PhotocompositionPhotocomposition
Photocompositionkyle1993
 

Andere mochten auch (10)

Positive Social Action Conference 2010: Photo Essay
Positive Social Action Conference 2010: Photo EssayPositive Social Action Conference 2010: Photo Essay
Positive Social Action Conference 2010: Photo Essay
 
PSAC 2010 photo essay
PSAC 2010 photo essayPSAC 2010 photo essay
PSAC 2010 photo essay
 
Case study presentation
Case study presentationCase study presentation
Case study presentation
 
SVC 2009-2010 Survey Totals
SVC 2009-2010 Survey TotalsSVC 2009-2010 Survey Totals
SVC 2009-2010 Survey Totals
 
Do So Much Weekend Resource Guide
Do So Much Weekend Resource GuideDo So Much Weekend Resource Guide
Do So Much Weekend Resource Guide
 
SB4 2.24 Screening Day B - Analytical Statement w/ Commentary
SB4 2.24 Screening Day B - Analytical Statement w/ CommentarySB4 2.24 Screening Day B - Analytical Statement w/ Commentary
SB4 2.24 Screening Day B - Analytical Statement w/ Commentary
 
Social Media For Social Good: a guide to
Social Media For Social Good: a guide toSocial Media For Social Good: a guide to
Social Media For Social Good: a guide to
 
Photocomposition
PhotocompositionPhotocomposition
Photocomposition
 
Focus on point of view
Focus on point of viewFocus on point of view
Focus on point of view
 
Blog
BlogBlog
Blog
 

Ähnlich wie College Students And Community

Affiliation and Attraction Social psychologists have studied aff.docx
Affiliation and Attraction Social psychologists have studied aff.docxAffiliation and Attraction Social psychologists have studied aff.docx
Affiliation and Attraction Social psychologists have studied aff.docxnettletondevon
 
MARY REVIEW1.Chan, G. & Yanos, P. T. (2018). Media depictions .docx
MARY REVIEW1.Chan, G. & Yanos, P. T. (2018). Media depictions .docxMARY REVIEW1.Chan, G. & Yanos, P. T. (2018). Media depictions .docx
MARY REVIEW1.Chan, G. & Yanos, P. T. (2018). Media depictions .docxalfredacavx97
 
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdfBrown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdftytrete
 
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...Alexander Decker
 
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...Alexander Decker
 
Topic area grief and students in middle school introduct
Topic area  grief and students in middle school introductTopic area  grief and students in middle school introduct
Topic area grief and students in middle school introductJASS44
 
Journal of Diversity in Higher EducationTrans Activism and A.docx
Journal of Diversity in Higher EducationTrans Activism and A.docxJournal of Diversity in Higher EducationTrans Activism and A.docx
Journal of Diversity in Higher EducationTrans Activism and A.docxcroysierkathey
 
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...Ashley Fisher
 
The Impact Of Social Identity On Education
The Impact Of Social Identity On EducationThe Impact Of Social Identity On Education
The Impact Of Social Identity On EducationLindsey Campbell
 
1 GENEROSITY AND PHILANTHROPY A LITERATURE REV.docx
1 GENEROSITY AND PHILANTHROPY A LITERATURE REV.docx1 GENEROSITY AND PHILANTHROPY A LITERATURE REV.docx
1 GENEROSITY AND PHILANTHROPY A LITERATURE REV.docxtarifarmarie
 
Psych Paper Final
Psych Paper FinalPsych Paper Final
Psych Paper FinalSarah Jones
 
Educational achievement_level of political conviction
Educational achievement_level of political convictionEducational achievement_level of political conviction
Educational achievement_level of political convictionAndrew Taylor
 
Ethicalness is next to openness? An exploration of the ethics of information ...
Ethicalness is next to openness? An exploration of the ethics of information ...Ethicalness is next to openness? An exploration of the ethics of information ...
Ethicalness is next to openness? An exploration of the ethics of information ...Jess Elmore
 
J Child Fam Stud (2007) 16331–346DOI 10.1007s10826-006-908.docx
J Child Fam Stud (2007) 16331–346DOI 10.1007s10826-006-908.docxJ Child Fam Stud (2007) 16331–346DOI 10.1007s10826-006-908.docx
J Child Fam Stud (2007) 16331–346DOI 10.1007s10826-006-908.docxchristiandean12115
 
Feeling Hopeful Inspires Support for Social ChangeKatharin.docx
Feeling Hopeful Inspires Support for Social ChangeKatharin.docxFeeling Hopeful Inspires Support for Social ChangeKatharin.docx
Feeling Hopeful Inspires Support for Social ChangeKatharin.docxmglenn3
 
When intentions go public - Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.
When intentions go public -  Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.When intentions go public -  Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.
When intentions go public - Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.Marcela Fernandez
 
Research on Donor Characteristics and Motivations-Misawa & Walker
Research on Donor Characteristics and Motivations-Misawa & WalkerResearch on Donor Characteristics and Motivations-Misawa & Walker
Research on Donor Characteristics and Motivations-Misawa & WalkerDr. Adam G. Walker ✔
 
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults Spontaneous Self-Representations
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults  Spontaneous Self-RepresentationsAgency And Communion Attributes In Adults  Spontaneous Self-Representations
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults Spontaneous Self-RepresentationsAudrey Britton
 

Ähnlich wie College Students And Community (20)

Kissick Thesis 2015
Kissick Thesis 2015Kissick Thesis 2015
Kissick Thesis 2015
 
Affiliation and Attraction Social psychologists have studied aff.docx
Affiliation and Attraction Social psychologists have studied aff.docxAffiliation and Attraction Social psychologists have studied aff.docx
Affiliation and Attraction Social psychologists have studied aff.docx
 
MARY REVIEW1.Chan, G. & Yanos, P. T. (2018). Media depictions .docx
MARY REVIEW1.Chan, G. & Yanos, P. T. (2018). Media depictions .docxMARY REVIEW1.Chan, G. & Yanos, P. T. (2018). Media depictions .docx
MARY REVIEW1.Chan, G. & Yanos, P. T. (2018). Media depictions .docx
 
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdfBrown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
Brown-Giving-PsychSci-2003.pdf
 
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
 
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
An evaluation of the motives behind volunteering and existing motivational st...
 
Topic area grief and students in middle school introduct
Topic area  grief and students in middle school introductTopic area  grief and students in middle school introduct
Topic area grief and students in middle school introduct
 
Journal of Diversity in Higher EducationTrans Activism and A.docx
Journal of Diversity in Higher EducationTrans Activism and A.docxJournal of Diversity in Higher EducationTrans Activism and A.docx
Journal of Diversity in Higher EducationTrans Activism and A.docx
 
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
Comparing And Contrasting Qualitative And Quantitative...
 
The Impact Of Social Identity On Education
The Impact Of Social Identity On EducationThe Impact Of Social Identity On Education
The Impact Of Social Identity On Education
 
1 GENEROSITY AND PHILANTHROPY A LITERATURE REV.docx
1 GENEROSITY AND PHILANTHROPY A LITERATURE REV.docx1 GENEROSITY AND PHILANTHROPY A LITERATURE REV.docx
1 GENEROSITY AND PHILANTHROPY A LITERATURE REV.docx
 
Psych Paper Final
Psych Paper FinalPsych Paper Final
Psych Paper Final
 
Educational achievement_level of political conviction
Educational achievement_level of political convictionEducational achievement_level of political conviction
Educational achievement_level of political conviction
 
Dependency
DependencyDependency
Dependency
 
Ethicalness is next to openness? An exploration of the ethics of information ...
Ethicalness is next to openness? An exploration of the ethics of information ...Ethicalness is next to openness? An exploration of the ethics of information ...
Ethicalness is next to openness? An exploration of the ethics of information ...
 
J Child Fam Stud (2007) 16331–346DOI 10.1007s10826-006-908.docx
J Child Fam Stud (2007) 16331–346DOI 10.1007s10826-006-908.docxJ Child Fam Stud (2007) 16331–346DOI 10.1007s10826-006-908.docx
J Child Fam Stud (2007) 16331–346DOI 10.1007s10826-006-908.docx
 
Feeling Hopeful Inspires Support for Social ChangeKatharin.docx
Feeling Hopeful Inspires Support for Social ChangeKatharin.docxFeeling Hopeful Inspires Support for Social ChangeKatharin.docx
Feeling Hopeful Inspires Support for Social ChangeKatharin.docx
 
When intentions go public - Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.
When intentions go public -  Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.When intentions go public -  Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.
When intentions go public - Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P, Michalski V, Seifert AE.
 
Research on Donor Characteristics and Motivations-Misawa & Walker
Research on Donor Characteristics and Motivations-Misawa & WalkerResearch on Donor Characteristics and Motivations-Misawa & Walker
Research on Donor Characteristics and Motivations-Misawa & Walker
 
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults Spontaneous Self-Representations
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults  Spontaneous Self-RepresentationsAgency And Communion Attributes In Adults  Spontaneous Self-Representations
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults Spontaneous Self-Representations
 

Mehr von Jordan Thompson

WrapAround Hamilton - Shalem Network - Brochure 2013
WrapAround Hamilton - Shalem Network - Brochure 2013WrapAround Hamilton - Shalem Network - Brochure 2013
WrapAround Hamilton - Shalem Network - Brochure 2013Jordan Thompson
 
Academia Display Poster for SVC
Academia Display Poster for SVCAcademia Display Poster for SVC
Academia Display Poster for SVCJordan Thompson
 
Poster for Positive Social Action Conference 2010
Poster for Positive Social Action Conference 2010Poster for Positive Social Action Conference 2010
Poster for Positive Social Action Conference 2010Jordan Thompson
 
Poster for Do So Much Weekend 2011
Poster for Do So Much Weekend 2011Poster for Do So Much Weekend 2011
Poster for Do So Much Weekend 2011Jordan Thompson
 
Newspaper ad for Student Volunteer Connections
Newspaper ad for Student Volunteer ConnectionsNewspaper ad for Student Volunteer Connections
Newspaper ad for Student Volunteer ConnectionsJordan Thompson
 
Poster for Social Action Series
Poster for Social Action SeriesPoster for Social Action Series
Poster for Social Action SeriesJordan Thompson
 
Poster for Student Volunteer Connections of Guelph
Poster for Student Volunteer Connections of GuelphPoster for Student Volunteer Connections of Guelph
Poster for Student Volunteer Connections of GuelphJordan Thompson
 
Poster for Tin Roof Global (2)
Poster for Tin Roof Global (2)Poster for Tin Roof Global (2)
Poster for Tin Roof Global (2)Jordan Thompson
 
Tin Roof Fundraiser Poster
Tin Roof Fundraiser PosterTin Roof Fundraiser Poster
Tin Roof Fundraiser PosterJordan Thompson
 
Tin Roof Fundraiser Tickets
Tin Roof Fundraiser TicketsTin Roof Fundraiser Tickets
Tin Roof Fundraiser TicketsJordan Thompson
 
Poster for Tin Roof Global
Poster for Tin Roof GlobalPoster for Tin Roof Global
Poster for Tin Roof GlobalJordan Thompson
 
Managing volunteers, successfully
Managing volunteers, successfullyManaging volunteers, successfully
Managing volunteers, successfullyJordan Thompson
 
Research on Student Volunteers
Research on Student VolunteersResearch on Student Volunteers
Research on Student VolunteersJordan Thompson
 

Mehr von Jordan Thompson (16)

WrapAround Hamilton - Shalem Network - Brochure 2013
WrapAround Hamilton - Shalem Network - Brochure 2013WrapAround Hamilton - Shalem Network - Brochure 2013
WrapAround Hamilton - Shalem Network - Brochure 2013
 
Academia Display Poster for SVC
Academia Display Poster for SVCAcademia Display Poster for SVC
Academia Display Poster for SVC
 
Poster for Positive Social Action Conference 2010
Poster for Positive Social Action Conference 2010Poster for Positive Social Action Conference 2010
Poster for Positive Social Action Conference 2010
 
Poster for Do So Much Weekend 2011
Poster for Do So Much Weekend 2011Poster for Do So Much Weekend 2011
Poster for Do So Much Weekend 2011
 
Newspaper ad for Student Volunteer Connections
Newspaper ad for Student Volunteer ConnectionsNewspaper ad for Student Volunteer Connections
Newspaper ad for Student Volunteer Connections
 
Poster for Social Action Series
Poster for Social Action SeriesPoster for Social Action Series
Poster for Social Action Series
 
Poster for Student Volunteer Connections of Guelph
Poster for Student Volunteer Connections of GuelphPoster for Student Volunteer Connections of Guelph
Poster for Student Volunteer Connections of Guelph
 
Move Out Madness Poster
Move Out Madness PosterMove Out Madness Poster
Move Out Madness Poster
 
Poster for Tin Roof Global (2)
Poster for Tin Roof Global (2)Poster for Tin Roof Global (2)
Poster for Tin Roof Global (2)
 
Tin Roof Fundraiser Poster
Tin Roof Fundraiser PosterTin Roof Fundraiser Poster
Tin Roof Fundraiser Poster
 
Tin Roof Fundraiser Tickets
Tin Roof Fundraiser TicketsTin Roof Fundraiser Tickets
Tin Roof Fundraiser Tickets
 
Poster for Tin Roof Global
Poster for Tin Roof GlobalPoster for Tin Roof Global
Poster for Tin Roof Global
 
Managing volunteers, successfully
Managing volunteers, successfullyManaging volunteers, successfully
Managing volunteers, successfully
 
The Volunteer
The VolunteerThe Volunteer
The Volunteer
 
Types of Volunteerism
Types of VolunteerismTypes of Volunteerism
Types of Volunteerism
 
Research on Student Volunteers
Research on Student VolunteersResearch on Student Volunteers
Research on Student Volunteers
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppCeline George
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...RKavithamani
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
 

College Students And Community

  • 1. College Students and Community Service: Who, With Whom, and Why? KIMBERLEEJ. TRUDEAUAND ANN SLOAN DEVLINl Connecticut College The community service volunteer vs. nonvolunteer status of 124 college students was examined in relation to motivation, gender, extraversion-introversion,and social anxiety. Volunteers and female students were hypothesized to have higher levels of motivation to participate than were nonvolunteers and males, and to have higher levels of interest in volunteering. The data for volunteer status and gender were significant only with respect to interest in volunteering, with volunteers and females significantly more interested in volunteering than were nonvolunteers and males. Gender differences were also found for volunteering stylepreference and participation within particular volunteer groups. Volunteers were also hypothesized to have lower levels of extraversion and higher levels of social anxiety than nonvolunteers because socially anxious introverts were thought to seek the anxiety-reducing structure of a volunteer activity. This hypothesis was not supported. The importance of community serviceto students today has implications for the future of the nation (Fitch, 1987).The 1989U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (1993) reported that 28.1% of the college student population were volunteers. Students volunteer with children, with the elderly, with each other. Who are these students? Why do they volunteer? What demographics and personality characteristics describe them? With which groups and individuals do they volunteer? Volunteering, like many prosocial behaviors, is influenced by both egoistic and altruistic motivations. For example, personal growth was selected as their central life meaning by 34 university student volunteers in a service organiza- tion much more frequently than in a comparable group of 106 university student nonvolunteers (D’Braunstein & Ebersole, 1992). Intrinsic-altruistic and egoistic motives together constitute what the volunteer considers a reward- ing experience (Cnaan & Goldberg-Glen, 1991). Serow (1991) viewed this combination in a positive light, “It is noteworthy that so many students are capable of deriving satisfaction from helping those in need” (p. 556). ‘Correspondence to concerning this article should be addressed to Ann Sloan Devlin, Box 5448, Department of Psychology, Connecticut College, 270 Mohegan Avenue, New London, CT 06320-4196. 1867 Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 1996, 26, 21, pp. 1867-1888. Copyright 0 1996 by V. H. Winston & Son, Inc. All rights reserved.
  • 2. 1868 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN Studies examining the length of commitment to a volunteer project provide information about what motivates volunteers. Rubien and Thorelli (1985) found that volunteers are less likely to leave the organization if their ego needs are satisfied (e.g., believing that what they are doing is helpful). Other studies (Miller, 1985; Wiehe & Isenhour, 1977) have discussed the importance of personal satisfaction to length of participation; volunteers want to learn new skills and to use these to benefit the organization. Sensitivity to the interests and motivations of volunteers may well affect agency recruiting efforts. Wiehe and Isenhour (1977) suggested that volunteer coordinators consider the interests and opinions of their volunteers when assigning tasks to be accomplished, as volunteers who felt well-utilized and appreciated were more likely to continue volunteering in the organization. This sensitivity to the needs of volunteers was also encouraged by Gillespie and King (1985) in order to ensure future volunteer participation, as well as program effectiveness; “training and skills” was cited as the single most important item by the majority of participants in their study. Phillips (1982) added that recruiters must describe not only the gains, but also the purpose of the program so that the volunteers would believe they were benefiting from, as well as participating in, something worthwhile. For example, year-long volun- teers in an ombudservice program cited advocating for the elderly as their motivation for participation (Nathanson & Eggleton, 1993). Among the motives for volunteerism that emerge with some frequency in the literature are those of a spiritual, altruistic, and affiliative nature. Religious motives for volunteer participation have emerged in a number of studies (Batson, Oleson, Weeks, & Healy, 1989; Fitch, 1991; Serow, 1989). Serow (1989) reported that public university students provided less service than students at institutions with a strong religious emphasis. Within the student population studied by Serow and Dreyden (1990), spiritual and religious values were positively associated with community service. Knapp and Holzberg (1964) also found that volunteers were slightly more religious, as well as more morally concerned and more compassionate. Altruism also frequently appears as a motivational force in volunteerism (Gelineau & Kantor, 1969;Gillespie & King, 1985;Wiehe & Isenhour, 1977). Cnaan and Goldberg-Glen (1991) created a 28-motive questionnaire from an extensive survey of the literatureon motivation and volunteerism. They discov- ered that an altruistic motive, “the opportunity to do something worthwhile,” was the most popular response. This altruistic response was followed by an egoistic one: “Volunteering for others makes me feel better about myself.” In a study of student volunteers, Fitch (1987) found that the highest rated motive was egoistic: “It gives me a good feelingor senseof satisfactionto help others.” The second highest motive was altruistic: “I am concerned about those less
  • 3. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1869 fortunate than me.” The third motive was affiliative; people were involved in community service because of “the people [they] meet and friendships [they] make with other volunteers.” The presence of affiliation motivation in community service participants also has been observed in several studies. Volunteers have been found to desire close relationships more than nonvolunteers Koopman (as cited in Abdennur, 1987). Interviewed college students often said that they would volunteer to compensate for a lost, lacking, or distant relationship in their lives (Serow, 1991).A study of divorced elderly women who volunteered revealed that these women “felt that their involvement in social programs allowed them to fill otherwise empty, lonely hours and to once again recapture a sense of pride and purpose” (Cain, 1988, p. 568). A study of volunteer recruiting (Abrami & Perry, 1976) showed that students were positively disposed toward programs that used the persuasions of “personal growth’’and opportunities for “meaning- ful relationships.” Inquiry into the significance of affiliation motivation in a student’s decision to volunteer could have value in future recruiting attempts. Beyond the motivations of altruism and affiliation, personality charac- teristics provide another salient factor in the willingness to volunteer. A num- ber of characteristics have been examined, including extraversion-introversion. However, the results of these studies invite further exploration, as both extrav- ersion and introversion have predicted volunteering. Spitz and MacKinnon (1993) describe successful Big Brother/Big Sister volunteers (those who follow through on their commitment) as “persons suited to making new acquaintances and maintaining a positive, comfortable atmosphere within the relationships” (p. 816).Other research done within the framework of volunteering for psycho- logical research indicates that extraversion predicted volunteering (Dollinger & Leong, 1993). At the same time, students who volunteered with mental patients were found to be more introverted than the general student population (Knapp & Holzberg, 1964), and Hobfoll (1980) reports that volunteers could not be clearly distinguished from nonvolunteers in regard to personality charac- teristics associated with the “helping personality.” Further, Fox (1984) states that while there are particular social situations that distinguish the affiliative tendencies of extroverts and introverts, “extroverts do not have a stronger general need for social contact which finds expression in all types of situations and in relation to all categories of companions” (p. 9). Perhaps Snyder’s (1993) functional approach best describes the relationship between personality char- acteristics such as extraversion-introversionand volunteering in that “different people have different motivations for engaging in the same acts of volunteer- ism” (p. 256); and that different personality characteristics accompany the different motivations.
  • 4. 1870 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN Theoretically, extroverts are sensitive to the feedback they receive from others and are interested in the social contact a group would provide (Cooper & Scalise, 1974); extroverts could thus be expected to have lower social anxiety in a group situation while introverts could be expected to experience more social anxiety. A desire to belong to a group has been shown to be inversely related to anxiety (Anant, 1967). Several studies have found a relationship between extraversion-introver- sion and anxiety (DeMan & Efraim, 1988;Teichman, 1974). Extraverts in one studytended to report high self-esteem and low trait anxiety (DeMan& Efraim, 1988).Teichman’s (1974) data indicated that when highly aroused, participants with a high predisposition for anxiety did not desire company, whereas subjects with a low predisposition for anxiety preferred to affiliate with others. Intro- verts might be likely to seek affiliation in the moderately structured setting of a volunteer organization. Perhaps introverts also seek social stimulation in the structured relationship a volunteer situation provides (Knapp & Holzberg, 1964). In particular, introverts may be attracted to a volunteer setting because it provides a safe environment in which to satisfy needs for affiliation. Thus, socially anxious introverts may find the organized activity (e.g.. tutoring, child care, meal basket preparation for AIDS patients) of a volunteer placement fulfills their affiliative needs in a manner that is tolerable in terms of social anxiety. Beyond personality characteristics, factors such as gender have been exam- ined in relationship to volunteerism. Female students were more likely to be volunteers than were male students (Fitch, 1991; Serow, 1990). Furthermore, male and female volunteers may have different volunteer group preferences. Signorella and Vegega (1984) confirmed the hypothesis that the degree of research involvement by male and female students is topic-dependent. There may well be prototypical male and female volunteer domains and volunteering style preferences. Building on the wide-ranging research examining volunteerism, the present studyexaminescollegestudentmotivation,gender,and extraversion-introversion in relation to community service. The researchers anticipate that more female students than male students will be (a) volunteers, (b) more motivated to volunteer, and (c) more interested in volunteering. It is also hypothesized that volunteers will have lower levels of extraversion and higher levels of social anxiety and social avoidance than nonvolunteers because a volunteer setting provides a safe environment for introverts to satisfy needs for affiliation. Introverts want to affiliate but fear social groups, leading to higher social anxiety; they should have higher social avoidance scores. In addition, it is predicted that volunteer style preferences and actual participation in the volun- teer groups will be related to gender.
  • 5. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1871 Method Subjects Subjectswere 124studentsfrom a small liberal arts college. Of these, 66.1% (n= 82)were women, 33.1 % (n= 41) were men, and one subjectdid not specify his or her gender. Students who had volunteered on at least one occasion while in college comprised 62.1% (n = 77) of the sample; the remaining 37.9% (n = 47) of the students were nonvolunteers. Of the female students, 68.3% (n= 56) were volunteers and 31.7%(n = 26) were nonvolunteers. Of the male students, 48.8% (n = 20) were volunteers and 51.2% (n = 21) were nonvolunteers. Materials Four questionnaires were used in this study. The first scale, the Eysenck Personality Inventory (EPI; Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968), is a 54-item test that measures introversion-extraversion using a true-false format. Eysenck and Eysenck (1968) established that their scale had good test-retest (.80to .97) and split-half reliability (Spearman-Brown = .74 to .91), as well as construct and concurrent validity. Vingoe (1968) also found that the EPI had criterion and construct validity using self-ratings and the California Psychological Inventory (Gough, 1957), respectively. Any items that showed large gender differences were eliminated during the construction of the scale. Farley (1971) confirmed this when his study demonstrated that there were no significant gender differ- ences in the scores of men and women. The second instrument, containing a list of 23 motives to volunteer, is the Motives to Volunteer Scale that was composed by Cnaan and Goldberg-Glen (1991) after a thorough survey of the literature on motivation and community service.Of the original 28 motives, 22 of them loaded on one factor (Cronbach’s a = .86). “Gaining practical experience toward paid employment” was in- cluded as the 23rd item because the authors thought that this motive would be important to college students, although it did not load on Cnaan and Goldberg- Glen’s unnamed major factor. Students rated the importance of various motives to participate in commu- nity service on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1(not important) to 5 (very important). Cnaan and Goldberg-Glen (1991) asserted that their scale pos- sessed “pragmatic validity” because volunteers received higher scores than nonvolunteers within their sample. Using factor analysis, they found that this “unidimensional phenomenon,” as they described it, did not distinguish altru- istic volunteers from egoistic volunteers; instead, they concluded that volun- teers were motivated by both altruistic and egoistic intentions.
  • 6. 1872 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN The third instrument is a list of 25 organizations and programs with which students might hypothetically volunteer (e.g., tutoring students, assisting hos- pital patients, helping the homeless) that was developed by the authors. Stu- dents rated (a) the extent of their interest in volunteering with each of these programs on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not very interested) to 5 (very interested); (b) when applicable, the age group(s) with whom they would prefer working within each program (i.e., children, adolescents, adults); and (c) whether or not they had previous volunteer experience with each program. At the end of this list were eight semantic differential items repre- senting the different styles of volunteering. This list, also developed by the authors, contained the polar anchors of academicfocus versus social/personal focus, short-term (day/week) versus long term (semester or more), with train- ing versus without training, with other college students versus by oneself, one-on-one versus group situations, planned projects versus flexibility, with supervision versus independently, and morning versus evening. Using a 5-point differential, students marked the number indicating their preference. For example, students who preferred an academic focus to a personal one would circle 1; with no particular preference, they circled 3; and with a personal focus, they circled 5. On this same page, biographical information was re- quested, including gender, present class year, volunteer experience in high school, and volunteer experience in college (class year and organization). The fourth instrument is the Social Avoidance and Distress Scale (SAD), developedby Watson and Friend (1969)to measure social anxiety. This bipolar scale was reported by Watson and Friend to have (a) test-retest reliability of .68 after a month, (b) convergent validity (i.e., subjects who scored high on the SAD avoided and/or were anxious in social situations), and (c) discriminant validity (i.e., the SAD correlates -.25 with the Crowne-Marlowe Social Desir- ability Scale; Crowne & Marlowe, 1964).Leary (1991) stated that the original true-false format had been abandoned for the more reliable 5-point Likert scale (the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient is close to .90),therefore the Likert scale was employed in this study. We were unable to locate any studies that had actually used the Likert scale, and therefore were unable to acquire further documenta- tion regarding the validity of the Likert format of the SAD. Procedure The solicitation of the 150 potential subjects of this convenience sample occurred at the college’s spring semester Volunteer Fair as they left the fair, as well as in the college library foyer as students entered. All students were informed that the study would take about 15min to complete and were thanked for their participation. Prospective subjects read a consent form, then signed a
  • 7. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1873 separate sheet with their name. The majority of the questionnaires were filled out at the location where the subjects received them (i.e., the fair or the library). One of the authors remained at that location to collect the completed question- naires from the subjects as they were leaving. Some completed questionnaires were mailed to the authors. A total of 150 questionnaires were distributed; 124 of these (82.7%)were returned. Of the 33 surveys distributed at the Volunteer Fair, 32 were returned-a return rate of 97.0%.Of the 117surveys distributed at the library, 92 were returned-a return rate of 78.6%.Psychology students who desired research credit wrote their campus box numbers on an indicated line at the end of the questionnaire and anonymous credit slips were put directly into their boxes by the authors. Those who wanted a summaryof the results also gave their box numbers to the authors. Results VolunteerMotivation and Interest Levels To evaluate the hypotheses that volunteers have both higher levels of motivation to participate in community service and higher levels of interest in volunteering, and that female students have significantly higher levels of these two dependent variables than do ma!sstpd,ents, a 2 x 2 (Volunteer vs. Nonvol- unteer x Gender) multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was per- formed. A Pearson correlation indicated that there was a positive linear relationship between the levels of motivation and interest (r = .20, p < .05). There were main effects for volunteer status, Wilks’s h = .892,F(2,91) = 5.49, p < .01, and for gender, Wilks’s h =.771,F(2,91) = 1 3 . 4 8 , ~<: .001, as well as an interactionbetween volunteer status and gender, Wilks’s h = .933,F(2,91) = 3.27,p < .05, on the total interest score. Univariate analyses revealed that there was a main effect for volunteer status on the total interest score, F( 1,92) = 11.09,p < .005,but not on the total motivation score, F( 1, 92) = 0.35,p > .05. As hypothesized, volunteers were significantly more interested in volunteering (M= 77.92) than were nonvolun- teers (M = 68.43). Contrary to the hypothesis, there was no main effect of volunteer status on the total motivation scores for volunteers (M= 67.59) and nonvolunteers (M = 65.00). Examination of the data via scatterplots revealed no indication of curvilinearity. Univariate analyses of the total interest scores by gender were statistically significant, F(1,92) = 25.69, p < .001. The hypothesis that female students (M= 79.25) were significantly more interested in participating in the volunteer programs than were male students (M = 64.42) was supported. Although the motivation scores of the female students (M = 68.37) were not significantly
  • 8. 1874 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN Table 1 Summary of Results of Simple Effects Test of Level of Interestfor Gender Volunteer Nonvolunteer F(1,92) Male 72.18 55.00 12.71** Female 80.14 77.38 ns F(1992) 4.43* 23.36** *p< .05. **p < .01. higher than the motivation scores of the males (M = 62.97), as had been predicted, the analysis approached significance, F(1, 92) = 3.68,p < .06. In addition to these main effects, there was also an interaction effect between volunteer status and gender. Univariate analyses revealed that the interaction involved the dependent variable total interest, F( 1, 92) = 5.80,p < .05. The results of the simple effects follow-up tests are shown in Table 1. Specific MotivationaI Factors To further investigate the motivations of students and the volunteer groups of interest to them, two separate orthogonal factor analyses were executed on these dependent variables. The 23 items of the Motives to Volunteer scale (Cnaan & Goldberg-Glen, 1991) were divided into factors using a principal components analysis. Although seven individual factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.OO were identified, an examination of those eigenvalues with a scree test suggested five major factors, and it was thus decided to rotate five factors. When the principal components analysis was rerun to produce five factors, the fifth factor was found to have only two items on it and, as both of these items had factor loading scores less than 0.30, this factor was also dropped and the analysis was rerun to produce four factors. These remaining four factors had eigenvalues of above 1SO.The chosen cut off point for factor loadings was 0.45 because it created a more coherent structure for each factor. The three items that did not load 0.45 on any factor (“opportunity to work with different age groups”; “opportunity for relationships”; and “people in my community volunteer”) were dropped from the respective factors. Two more items were dropped because they loaded highly (0.45 or better) on two factors (“excellent educational experience” and “provides challenging activities”). The first factor, labeled “social obligation,” included five items that sug- gested an institutional commitment to volunteerism: “It is God’s expectation”;
  • 9. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1875 “agency can provide more for less”; “continuing a family tradition”; “agency is prestigious”; and “previous contact with professionals in this agency” (Cronbach’s a = .73). The second factor, labeled “experience-seeking,’’ in- cluded four items that suggested the desire to gain and share experience: “able to relate to clients due to my own similar experience”; “experience in providing service”; “my employerlschool expect it”; and “gaining practical experience toward paid employment”(Cronbach’sa = .70).The third factor, labeled‘taltru- istic intention,” included four items: “opportunity to do something worth- while”; “it creates a better society”; “opportunity to return my good fortune”; and “opportunity to change injustices” (Cronbach’s a =.66). The fourth factor, labeled “personal motivation,” included five items that expressed reasons that people help others while helping themselves: “makes me feel better about myself’; “it improves my attitude on my own life situation”; “opportu- nity to vary my activities”; “broadening my horizons”; and “nothing else to do with my time” (Cronbach’s a =.70; see Table 2 for factor loadings). To examine the relationship between volunteer status and gender and the importance of the individual factor motives to volunteer, a 2 x 2 (Volunteer Status x Gender) MANOVA was conducted on the unitized factor scores of each of these factors. Although there were no interaction effects, Wilks’s h = .940, F(4, 107)= 1 . 7 0 , ~> .05, or main effects for volunteer status, Wilks’s h = .938, F(4, 107)= 1 . 7 6 , ~< .05, there was a main effect for gender, Wilks’s h = .913, F(4, 107) = 2 . 5 7 , ~< .05. Univariate analyses demonstrated that female students (M= 5.94) were significantly more motivated by the altruistic inten- tion factor than were male students (M= 14.08),F(1, 110)= 8 . 5 0 , ~< .005. Volunteer Group Types An orthogonal principal cornponents analysis was executed on the 25 volunteer groups. Eight factors with eigenvalues of above 1.OO were identified. An examination of those eigenvalues with the scree test suggested the analysis be rerun with five factors. The remaining factors had eigenvalues of above 1.45. The 0.45 item factor loading score was chosen as an inclusion criterion, as before, to create a more coherent factor structure. One item that did not load 0.45 on any factor was dropped from further analyses (“preschool children [daycare]”). One item loaded highly on two factors and was also dropped (“battered women”). The first factor, labeled the “socially discriminated against” group included six items: “substance abusers”; “survivors of sexual abuselrape”;“teen mothers”; “juveniledelinquents”; “bilgay teens”; and “prison inmates” (Cronbach’sa = .77). The second factor, labeledthe “medical-disaster relief,” group includedfour items: “cancerpatients”; “AIDS patients”; “hospital
  • 10. 1876 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN Table 2 VarimaxRotated Factor Loadings of Motives to Volunteer and Percentage of Variance Motive % of Loading variance Social obligation Agency is prestigious. Agency can provide more for less. It is God’s expectation. Continuing a family tradition. Previous contact with professionals in this agency Gaining practical experience toward paid My employer/school expects it. Experience in providing service. Able to relate to clients due to my own similar Experience seeking employment. experience. Altruistic intention Opportunity to do something worthwhile. It creates a better society. Opportunity to change injustices. Opportunity to return my good fortune. Makes me feel better about myself. It improves my attitude on my own life situation. Opportunity to vary my activities. Broadening my horizons. Personal motivation .69 .68 .61 .59 .57 .83 .67 .57 .49 .69 .69 .61 .50 .67 .66 .52 .47 24.7 10.3 7.6 6.9 patients”; and “disaster relief’ (Cronbach’s a = .74). The third factor, labeled the “teaching-mentoring” group, included five items: “the illiterate”; “teach for America”; “tutoring students”; “bilingual students”; and “Big BrotherdSis- ters” (Cronbach’s a = .74). The fourth factor, labeled the “special needs” group, included five items: “mentally retarded”; “elderly”; “homeless (soup kitchens)”; “mentally ill”; and “Habitat for Humanity” (Cronbach’s a = .74).
  • 11. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1877 The fifth factor, labeled the “social causes-sports” group, included three items: “political groups”; “environmental groups”; and “Sports teams (coaching)” (Cronbach’s a =.44; see Table 3 for factor loadings). The multivariate analyses conducted with the unitized scores of these five interest factors by volunteer status and gender produced no significant interac- tion, Wilks’s h = .938,F(5,95)= 1 . 2 6 , ~> .05, nor significant main effects for volunteer status,Wilks’s h = .918,F(5,95) = 1 . 7 1 , ~> .05. However, there was a main effect for gender on the interest factors, Wilks’s h = .651, F(5, 95) = 1 0 . 1 6 , ~< .001. Univariate analyses of variance reiterated that female students were more interested in volunteering with the different groups than were male students in four of the five factor types (see Table 4 for detailed statistics). Additional GenderDifferences The hypothesis that gender differences exist in the types of activities in which students actually volunteered was tested using a series of chi-squares, one for each of the 25 provided programs, with two additions. Women’s groups and youth groups were also included because several students had listed these among the groups with which they volunteered. To guard against the possibility of Type I error, the Bonferroni test was used to set the significance level at .002 (.05/27). One significant difference emerged. Analyses revealed that female students had volunteered with preschool children, ~ ~ ( 1 ,N = 123) = 12.66,p < .002, significantly more than had male students. Gender differences also were anticipated with respect to the styles of volunteering preferred by the participants. The possibility of gender differ- ences was tested with a MANOVA using the eight volunteering styles as the dependent variables. There was a main effect for gender, Wilks’s h = ,805, F(8, 110) = 3.33, p < .005. Univariate analyses indicated that males and females differed with respect to two of the eight volunteering styles: how long subjects preferred to volunteer (short term, consisting of a day or week com- mitment, or long term, lasting for a semester of more) and whether or not they preferred training within the organization. Female students preferred long-term commitments ( M = 3.70) more than did male students (M= 2.90), F(1, 117)= 10.62,~< .005, and male students preferred to volunteer without training (M= 3.18) more than did female students (M = 2.26), F( 1, 117) = 13.40,p < .OO 1 (see Table 5 for means and standard deviations of styles). Extraversionand Social Anxiety Preliminary analyses revealed that the mean and standard deviation of the extraversion-introversion scores of the current study were 12.61 and 4.01,
  • 12. 1878 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN Table 3 VarirnaxRotated Factor Loadings of Interest in VolunteerPrograms Factor Loading % of variance Socially discriminated against 25.9 Substance abusers .73 Survivors of sexual abuse .70 Teen mothers .64 Juvenile delinquents .58 Prison inmates .52 Bi/gay teens .57 Medical-disaster relief Cancer patients AIDS patients Hospital patients Disaster relief Teaching-mentoring The illiterate Teach for America Tutoring students Bilingual students Big BrotherdBig Sisters Special needs Mentally retarded Elderly Homeless (soup kitchens) Mentally ill Habitat for Humanity .76 .65 .59 .57 8.7 6.9 .76 .74 .73 .62 .49 6.6 .78 .72 .59 .53 .49 Social causes-sports 6.0 Political groups .73 Environmental groups .55 Sports teams .53
  • 13. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1879 Table 4 Means and F Statistics of Interest Factorsfor Gender Gender Factor Females Males F(L99) Socially discriminated against 18.68 13.52 24.81**** Medical-disaster relief 13.41 9.86 25.81**** Teaching-mentoring 20.35 17.46 8.22** Special needs 15.40 13.11 ns Social causes-sports 7.26 9.26 ns **p< .01. ****p< .001. respectively.The mean and standard deviation of Eysenck and Eysenck's (1968) original sample of college students (N = 1,003) were 13.10 and 4.10, respec- tively. The mean of the SAD scores was 38.55 with a standard deviation of 20.13. A Pearson correlation revealed that there was a negative linear correla- tion between the levels of extraversion and social anxiety (r = -.55,p < .Ol). To evaluate the hypothesis that volunteers have lower levels of extraversion and higherlevelsof socialanxietythannonvolunteers,a MANOVA wasperformed on these two dependent variables. The hypothesis was not supported, Wilks's h = .976, F(2, 121)= 1 . 5 0 , ~> .05; there were no significant main effects for extraversion,F(l, 122)= 0 . 2 0 , ~>.05,or socialanxiety,F(1,122)= 2 . 7 2 , ~> .05. A standard regression was performed to identify whether extraversion and/or social anxiety in part predict total level of volunteer participation (operationalized as the number of different groups with whom one had volun- teered). Extraversion and social anxiety were regressed on the total number of volunteer programs in which the students participated. The Pearson correla- tions between extraversion and the number of volunteer programs was not significant (r = .O 1,p > .05) while the correlation between social anxiety and the number of volunteer programs was significant (r = -.27,p < .01). Consistent with the correlational analysis, the regression analysis showed that the level of extraversion did not contribute significantly to the model (p = -.21, p > .05), but that social anxiety explained 10%of the variance in level of participation in volunteer programs (R2= .lo), F(2, 101)= 5 . 7 0 , ~< .01; p = -.39, p < .01. The b-weight score (-.06) indicates that as social anxiety increases, the level of volunteer participation decreases.
  • 14. 1880 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN Table 5 Means and Standard Deviations of Semantic Differential Styles of Volunteeringfor Gender Gender - Style of volunteering Males Females Academic focus vs. social/personal focus M SD Short-term vs. long-term M SD With training vs. without training A4 SD With other college students vs. by oneself M SD One on one vs. group situations M SD Planned projects vs. flexibility A4 SD M SD With supervision vs. independently Morning vs. evening M SD 3.59 0.88 2.90 1.09 3.18 1.40 2.95 1.30 2.64 1.31 3.33 1.40 3.56 1.29 3.26 1.31 3.50 1.09 3.70*** 1.40 2.26**** 1.23 3.30 1.22 2.38 1.16 3.44 1.17 3.41 1.16 3.00 1.26 Note. The above means correspond to a semantic differential scale on which 1 indicates a strong preference for the first item through 5 which indicates a strong preference for the item opposite to it. ***p< .005. ****p< .001.
  • 15. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1881 Table 6 Frequency of Participation in VolunteerPrograms in Descending Order No. of students Program (N= 124) No. of students Program (N= 124) Preschool children Tutoring students Homeless (soup kitchens) Elderly Sports teams (coaching) Environmental groups Hospital patients Bilingual students Political groups Mentally retarded Mentally ill Habitat for Humanity Big Brothers/Big Sisters Juvenile delinquents 72 70 46 44 38 38 31 30 29 28 27 24 24 15 Survivors of sexual abuse Substance abusers Youth groups The illiterate Cancer patients Battered women AIDS patients Bi/gay teens Prison inmates Disaster relief Teach for America Teen mothers Women’s groups 14 13 12 12 10 9 8 7 6 6 5 5 4 Participant Characteristics Chi-square analyses provided information about the participant pool. Sig- nificantly more female students (n = 56) than male students (n = 20) were volunteers, x2(1,N = 123)=4 . 4 1 , ~< .05.Significantly more students who had volunteered in high school also volunteered in college, x2(1, N = 124) = 8.61, p < .005.Finally, significantly more juniors and seniors volunteered than did not, x2(3,N= 123)= 1 8 . 3 5 , ~< .0005. Frequency analyses or means provided information about students’ volun- teer program participation in high school and college, their motivation to volunteer with particular programs, and the extent to which the students, overall, were motivated by each of the items on the Motives to Volunteer Scale (Cnaan & Goldberg-Glen, 1991). As illustrated in Table 6, these analyses revealed that of the 124 subjects, 58.1% (n = 72) had volunteered with pre- school children, 56.5% (n= 70) of the subjects had tutored students, 37.1% (n = 46) of the subjects had volunteered to help the homeless, and 35.5% (n = 44)
  • 16. 1882 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN Table 7 Means and Standard Deviations of Interest in VolunteerPrograms in Descending Order Program M SD Tutoring students Big Brothers/Sisters Homeless Preschool children AIDS patients Teach for America Battered women Habitat for Humanity Survivors of abuse The illiterate Cancer patients Environmental groups Hospital patients Disaster relief Elderly Bilingual students Juvenile delinquents Teen mothers Mentally ill Substance abusers Sports teams (coaching) Prison inmates Mentally retarded Bi/gay teens Political groups 3.65 3.63 3.45 3.37 3.26 3.21 3.18 3.16 3.15 3.13 3.12 3.05 3.03 3.03 2.94 2.93 2.90 2.84 2.76 2.75 2.71 2.68 2.67 2.65 2.53 1.46 1.14 1.24 1.46 1.32 1.33 1.37 1.32 1.44 1.23 1.16 1.34 1.21 1.26 1.21 1.44 1.21 1.30 1.43 1.30 1.58 1.28 1.26 1.24 1.45 Note. The above means indicate level of interest, 1 = not interested to 5 = very interested.
  • 17. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1883 Table 8 Means and Standard Deviations of Motives to Volunteer Motive M SD Opportunity to do something worthwhile. It creates a better society. Makes me feel better about myself. Broadening my horizons. Excellent educational experience. Opportunity to change injustices. Opportunity to return my good fortune. Provides challenging activities. It improves my attitude on my own life situation. Experience in providing service. Opportunity to vary my activities. Opportunity for relationships. Opportunity to work with different age groups. Gaining practical experience toward paid employment. Able to relate to clients due to my own similar experience. Agency can provide more for less. Previous contact with professional in this agency. It is God’s expectation. My employerlschool expects it. Nothing else to do with my time. 4.29 4.02 3.92 3.86 3.81 3.57 3.50 3.49 3.49 3.44 3.21 3.21 3.10 3.06 2.80 2.47 1.94 1.73 1.71 1.58 0.88 1.04 1.02 0.98 1.01 1.16 1.11 1.05 1.13 1.16 1.09 1.16 1.23 1.34 1.29 1.14 1.12 1.15 1.01 0.98 Note. The above means indicate how important each motive would be when deciding whether or not to participate in community service, 1 = not important to 5 = very important. of the subjects had volunteered with the elderly (see Table 6 for frequencies of participation in the volunteer programs). With respect to interest in participating in the volunteer programs, student tutors (M= 3.65), Big BrothedSisters (M= 3.63), the homeless (M= 3.45), and preschool children (M=3.37) received the highest means of interest out of a possible 5.00 signifying greatest interest. The elderly category (M= 2.94), which was fourth in actual participation, ranked 15th in interest on the list of 25 (see Table 7 for means and standard deviations of interest in the volunteer programs). Of the possible motives, the participants in this sample were most
  • 18. 1884 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN motivated by “opportunity to do something worthwhile” ( M = 4.29), “it creates a better society” ( M = 4.02), “makes me feel better about myself’ ( M = 3.92), and “broadening my horizons” (A4= 3.86; see Table 8 for means and standard deviations of motives to volunteer). Discussion Human service experience is a valuable commodity in this economy because the economy is “undergoing a shift from goods producingto service producing” (Nathanson& Eggleton, 1993,p. 108).The majority of the studentsin the present study are active in this social change; they volunteer. Students who volunteered in high school also volunteer in college, suggestingthat volunteerism is affected by psychological and social variables, not simply situational circumstances. Contrary to expectation, levels of extraversion and social anxiety did not predict volunteerism in this study. These results contradict those by Carlson and Levy (1973), who found that volunteers were predominantly extraverted; as well as the results of Knapp and Holzberg (1964), who found that volunteers were predominantly introverted. The results of the present study suggest that volunteer populations are diverse, including individuals with varying levels of extraversion and social anxiety. Snyder’s (1993) functional analysis of volun- teerism may best explain the varying levels of extraversion and social anxiety and avoidance found in this study. The similarity between the statistics of the original sample tested with the EPI (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1968)and the present sample further suggests that personality characteristics themselves are rela- tively unaffected by generational pressures; students today appear to be no more extraverted or introverted than they were in 1968. It is important to note that the levelsof extraversionand social anxietyare not independent.The significantnegative linear relationshipbetween these variables demonstrates that extraverted subjects reported less social anxiety, and intro- verted subjects reported more social anxiety. Though these dimensions had no effect on volunteerstatusin the present study,this significantnegativecorrelation indicates that these constructs have a conceptual relationshipthat may be further explored.A problemwiththepresent studyisthat it didnot measurethe affiliation motivation through a theoretical mixture of extraversion and social anxiety. If one tested the need for affiliation and the number of hours one devotes to com- munity service, this might also provide insight into the extent that affiliation motivationaffectsthe activecommunityserviceparticipationof a collegestudent. The positive linear correlationbetweenmotivation to volunteerand interest in volunteeringwith the various programs also merits further study, as multivariate analyses of these variables did not reveal consistent group differences. While volunteers in the present study were more interested in volunteering than were
  • 19. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1885 nonvolunteers, consistent with Hobfoll’s (1980) discussion of social interest motivation for volunteer participation, volunteers in the present study were not significantly more motivated than were nonvolunteers, which contradicts Cnaan and Goldberg-Glen’s (1991) results. Either students have learned what the socially accepted motives to volunteer are and, therefore, simply reproduced them as responses on the questionnaire, or nonvolunteers are equally motivated but have other obligations that precludetheir ability to participate in community service.What this studyshowsis that volunteersare distinguishablefromnonvol- unteersin that they are more interestedin participating in the availableprograms. The results also revealed anticipated gender differences. Just as Signorella and Vegega (1984) reported gender differences in volunteerism for particular research topics, gender differences appear with regard to volunteering for a particular program. Notably, significantly more female students have volun- teered with preschool children than have male students. MANOVA indicated that female students are also more motivated by the altruistic factor than are male students. This evidence conflicts with an earlier review of research that females are not more altruistic (Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). In the authors’ view, it is not surprising that women had nearly significantly higher levels of motivation to volunteer and that women are more interested in volunteering. Female students may be more affiliative than male students, and thus more interested in participating with the variety of populations represented by the many volunteer programs. The programs on the survey may also appeal more to women than to men, as female students are more interested in volunteering with the programs represented in four of the five factors, whereas men are more interested in volunteering only with the factor containing “political groups,” “environmental groups,” and “sports teams.” While there are volunteer oppor- tunities in which male students are more interested than are females, these opportunities tend to mirror stereotypical interpretations of male interest. The students at this small liberal arts college volunteer. They volunteer predominantly with preschool children, with students who require tutoring, with the homeless, and with the elderly. Perhaps these are the more popular volunteer programs because youth, education, homelessness, and old age are concerns of contemporary society.These programs might also attract the largest number of volunteers because they provide students with more opportunities, both in number and convenience. As women’s centers, AIDS programs, and environmental projects receive more funding and, consequently, expand their services, more opportunities to volunteer with these groups should develop. The 10 volunteer programs in which students are most interested (but not necessarilywherethey actuallyvolunteered)involvedopportunitiestoactastutors or mentors; as counselors for AIDS patients, battered women, and survivors of sexual abuse; and as carpenters and food providers for the homeless. Students
  • 20. 1886 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN are least interested in volunteering with political groups. This pattern might indicate a growing concern for humanity, above policy, among students. Of the 23 motives of the Motives to Volunteer Scale (Cnaan & Goldberg- Glen, 1991), the 7 that received the highest ratings from the students were as follows: the first two and two others loaded on the factor labeled altruistic, two loaded on the personal motivation factor, and one loaded on the experience- seeking factor. These results show that students recognize and appreciate community service more as a way of giving back to the community than as a way of gaining the benefits of personal satisfaction and practical experience. These results reveal that altruism is a popular motivation for participation in community service, which corroborates findings reported by Gelineau and Kantor (1969), Gillespie and King (1985), and Wiehe and Isenhour (1977). College students and community service: who, with whom, and why? The findings of this study are significant. More women than men are volunteers. Women are more interested in volunteering than are men. Women volunteer more with preschool children than do men. Women are more motivated to volunteer by altruistic motives than are men. Future research could explore which characteristicsofwomen and men seem to predispose them to participate in community service. This might be accomplished, in part, by comparing gender role orientation to service participation. References Abdennur, A. (Ed.). (1987). The conflict resolution syndrome: Volunteerism, violence, and beyond. Ottawa, Canada: University of Ottawa Press. Abrami, P. C., & Perry, R. P. (1976). Recruitment of college volunteers for community service organizations using the lecture method. Journal of Community Psychology, 4,369-377. Anant, S . S. (1967). Belongingness, anxiety, and self-sufficiency: Pilot study. Psychological Reports, 20, 1137-1138. Batson, C. D., Oleson, K. C., Weeks, J. L., & Healy, S. P. (1989). Religious prosocial motivation: Is it altruistic or egoistic? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,873-884. Cain, B. S. (1988). Divorce among elderly women: A growing social phe- nomenon. Special issue: Life transitions in the elderly. Social Casework, Carlson, R., & Levy, N. (1973). Studies of Jungian typology: I. Memory, social perception, and social action. Journal of Personality, 41, 559-576. Cnaan,R. A., & Goldberg-Glen,R. S. (1991).Measuringmotivationto volunteer in human services. Special issue: Methods for research and intervention with organizations. Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 27,269-284. 69, 563-568.
  • 21. COLLEGE STUDENTS AND COMMUNITY SERVICE 1887 Cooper, J., & Scalise, C. J. (1974). Dissonance produced by deviations from life styles: The interaction of Jungian typology and conformity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4,566-571. Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive: Studies in evalu- ative dependence. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. D’Braunstein, S., & Ebersole, P. (1992). Categories of life meaning for service organization volunteers. Psychological Reports, 70,281-282. DeMan, A. F., & Efraim, D. P. (1988). Selected personality correlates of social participation in university students. Journal of Social Psychology, 128, Dollinger, S. J., & Leong, F. T. L. (1993). Volunteer bias and the five-factor model. TheJournal of Psychology, 127,29-36. Eysenck, H. I., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1968). Eysenck Personality Inventory manual. San Diego, CA: Educational 8c Industrial Testing Series. Farley, F. H. (1971). Some EPI reliability estimates. Journal of Personality Assessment, 35,364-366. Fitch, R. T. (1987). Characteristics and motivations of college students volun- teering for community service. Journal of College Student Personnel, 28, Fitch, R. T. (1991). Differences among community service volunteers, extra- curricular volunteers, and nonvolunteers on the college campus. Journal of College Student Development, 32, 534-540. Fox, S. (1984). The sociability aspect of extraversion as a situation-specific dimension. Social Behavior and Personality, 12, 7-10. Gelineau, V. A., & Kantor, D. (1969). Pro-social commitment among college students. Journal of Social Issues, 20, 112-130. Gillespie, D. F., & King, A. E. (1985). Demographic understanding of volun- teerism. Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare,12,798-816. Gough, H. G. (1957). California Psychological Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Hobfoll, S. E. (1980). Personal characteristics of the college volunteer. Ameri- can Journal of CommunityPsychology, 8,503-506. Knapp, R. H., & Holzberg, J. D. (1964). Characteristics of college students volunteering for service to mental patients. Journal of ConsultingPsychol- Leary, M. R. (1991). Social anxiety, shyness, and related constructs. In J. P. Robinson, P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.),Measures ofperson- ality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 161-194). San Diego, CA: Academic. Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. 265-267. 424-431. ogy, 28, 82-85.
  • 22. 1888 TRUDEAU AND DEVLIN Miller, L. E. (1985). Understanding the motivation of volunteers: An exami- nation of personality differences and characteristics of volunteers’ paid employment. Journal of VoluntaryAction Research, 14, 112-122. Nathanson, I. I., & Eggleton, E. (1993). Motivation versus program effect on length of service: A study of four cohorts of ombudservice volunteers. Journal of Gerontological Social Work, 19,95-114. Phillips, M. (1982). Motivation and expectation in successful volunteerism. Journal of VoluntaryAction Research, 11, 118-125. Rubien, A., & Thorelli, I. M. (1985). Egoistic motives and longevity of partici- pation by social service volunteers. Journal of Applied Behavioral Sci- ence, 20,223-235. Serow, R. C. (1989). Community service, religious commitment, and campus climate. Youth & Society, 21, 105-120. Serow, R. C. (1990). Volunteering and values: An analysis of students’ par- ticipation in community service. Journal of Research and Development, Serow, R. C. (1991). Students and voluntarism: Looking into the motive of community service participants. American Educational Research Journal, Serow, R. C., & Dreyden, J. I. (1990). Community service among college and university students: Individual and institutional relationships. Adoles- cence, 25,553-566. Signorella, M. L., & Vegega, M. E. (1984). A note on gender stereotyping of research topics. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 10, 107-109. Snyder, M. (1993). Basic research and practical problems: The promise of a “functional” personality and social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 251-264. Spitz, R. T., & MacKinnon, J. R. (1993). Predicting success in volunteer community service. Psychological Reports, 73, 815-818. Teichman, Y. (1974). Predisposition for anxiety and affiliation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29,405-410. U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (1993). No. 614. Percent of adult population doing volunteer work: 1989: Statistical Abstract of The United States (p. 386). Washington, DC: U. S. Department of Commerce. Vingoe, F. J. (1968). Validity of the Eysenck Extraversion Scale: Replication and extension. Psychological Reports, 22, 706-708. Watson, D., & Friend, R. (1969). Measurement of social-evaluative anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 33,448-457. Wiehe, V. R., & Isenhour, L. (1977). Motivation of volunteers. Journal of Social Welfare,473-79. 23, 198-203. 28, 543-556.