SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 4
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
M. Thérèse Southgate, MD, Section Coordinator 
Genetics and the Risk for Alcoholism 
Marc A. Schuckit, MD 
The importance of genetic factors in alcoholism is supported by the 
familial nature of this disorder, the significantly higher concordance rate in 
identical than in fraternal twins, and the fourfold higher risk for children of 
alcoholics, even when adopted out at birth. Based on this evidence, the 
search is under way to identify possible genetically influenced factors that 
might contribute to the alcoholism risk. Studies of populations at high risk for 
the future development of alcoholism (eg, sons of alcoholics) have revealed 
a probable decreased intensity of reaction to modest doses of ethanol, a 
possible decreased amplitude of the P300 wave of the event-related 
potential, and a possible decreased amount of g=a-waveactivity on the 
background cortical electroencephalogram. The implications of these and 
other findings and their impact on the practice of medicine are explored. 
(JAMA 1985;254:2614-2617) 
ALCOHOLISM afflicts 10% of adult 
men and 3% to 5% of adult women at 
some time during their lives, with an 
even higher rate among patients 
attending medical clinics.12 This arti¬ 
cle reviews studies evaluating genetic 
factors that might contribute to the 
risk for this prevalent disorder. 
The importance of genetics in the 
vulnerability toward alcoholism is 
supported by evidence from family, 
twin, and adoption studies in humans. 
First, the familial nature of alcohol¬ 
ism (ie, the way it occurs in families) 
has been documented for more than 
100 years.1 The risk appears to 
increase with the number of alcoholic 
relatives and the closeness of the 
genetic relationship. However, many 
familial factors are not genetically 
influenced. 
The second approach, studies of 
twins, takes advantage of an experi¬ 
ment of nature. Twin pairs are born 
at the same time and are likely to 
experience major childhood events 
(eg, death of a parent) at the same 
age. Therefore, if childhood environ¬ 
ment is important in the development 
of alcoholism, the risk should be 
elevated in the twin of an alcoholic, 
no matter what type of twinship is 
involved. However, there are two 
types of twins; identical, who share 
100% of their genes, and fraternal, 
who share only 50% (the same as any 
two full siblings). As a result, if 
alcoholism is genetically influenced, 
the risk for the identical twin of an 
alcoholic should be significantly high¬ 
er than the risk for a fraternal twin. 
While there is some debate,4 the 
majority of studies demonstrate a 
concordance of 60% or higher for the 
identical twin of an alcoholic but a 
risk of only 30% or less if the rela¬ 
tionship is fraternal.5 
The most impressive evidence sup¬ 
porting the importance of genetic 
factors in alcoholism comes from 
adoption-type studies. Investigations 
from different countries using a vari¬ 
ety of methods have demonstrated 
that adopted-away children of alco¬ 
holics are at four times higher risk 
for this disorder than controls.*"8 Once 
the influence of a biological alcoholic 
parent is considered, being reared by 
an alcoholic does not seem to add to 
the risk, and children of nonalcoholics 
raised by alcoholics do not appear to 
have an enhanced rate of this prob¬ 
lem. 
RESEARCH APPROACHES WITH 
POPULATIONS AT HIGH RISK 
FOR ALCOHOLISM 
In response to these findings, a 
number of laboratories have begun to 
ask how the genetic risk might be 
mediated.8 One promising approach 
has been to evaluate nonalcoholic 
close relatives of alcoholics, limiting 
the study to men who are young 
enough to have not entered the major 
age of risk for alcoholism—ie, popula¬ 
tions at high future risk are observed. 
The assets of this research design 
include the almost inexhaustible 
number of potential subjects, the 
large number of families investigated 
so that numerous factors might be 
determined, and the ability to observe 
individuals at high risk before alco¬ 
holism actually develops. 
In most studies of populations at 
high risk, potential subjects are 
males who have an alcoholic family 
member (usually a first-degree rela¬ 
tive and most frequently the father). 
Some investigators have chosen to 
evaluate male children in the preteen 
or early teen years, thus increasing 
the probability that subjects are 
naive to the effects of ethanol. These 
young men are often chosen from 
among families of alcoholics attend¬ 
ing an alcoholic treatment program 
or from teenaged subjects identified 
because of police problems.9" Other 
investigations have focused on older 
groups to avoid the long lag time 
between evaluation and the actual 
development of alcoholism (should 
follow-up studies be planned). This 
also maximizes the opportunity to 
observe potentially important inter¬ 
actions between the genetic predispo¬ 
sition and adaptations to ethanol over 
years of modest drinking. Men are 
chosen for most studies because 
responses to an ethanol challenge 
might be affected by the phase of the 
From the Department of Psychiatry, University of 
California at San Diego School of Medicine, and the 
Alcohol Research Center, San Diego Veterans 
Administration Medical Center. 
Reprint requests to Department of Psychiatry, 
San Diego Veterans Administration Medical Center, 
3350 La Jolla Village Dr, San Diego, CA 92161 (Dr 
Schuckit). 
Downloaded from jama.ama-assn.org at Mt Sinai School Of Medicine on April 23, 2012
After Ethanol Ingestion, min 
Fig 1.—Mean self-ratings on a 0 (none) to 36 (great) scale for drug 
effect after placebo and after 0.75 mL/kg of ethanol for 23 matched 
pairs with positive (close circles) and negative (open circles) family 
histories. Bars indicate SEs. After 0.75 mL/kg, using a two-factor, 
mixed-model, repeated-measures analysis of variance in which sub¬ 
jects were nested with respect to the between factor (family history) 
and crossed with respect to the within factor (time), the differences 
between family groups were significant (F=5.99, 1,44 df, P<.02). 
(Figure reproduced from Archives of General Psychiatry 1984; 
41:879-884.) 
5 
Baseline 140 170 240 
After Ethanol Ingestion, min 
Fig 2.—Percent increase in body sway or standing steadiness 
following 0.75 mL/kg of ethanol for 23 matched pairs with positive 
(close circles) and negative (open circles) family histories. Bars 
indicate SEs. Using the same statistical approach as in Fig 1, after 
0.75 mL/kg of ethanol the two family groups differed significantly 
(F=3.84, 3,197 df, P=.01). (Figure reproduced from Archives of 
General Psychiatry 1985;42:375-379.) 
menstrual cycle or the type of birth 
control pills being consumed by 
women. 
My own laboratory chose to sample 
21- to 25-year-old male students and 
staff at a university who responded to 
a mailed questionnaire. After exclud¬ 
ing those persons who have serious 
alcohol- or drug-related life problems 
or major medical or phychiatric dis¬ 
orders, those drinking but nonalco¬ 
holic young men who report a close 
alcoholic relative are placed in the 
family history-positive or high-risk 
group. Each man with a positive 
family history is matched on de¬ 
mography (age, sex, religion, race, 
educational level, etc), drinking histo¬ 
ry, and height-to-weight ratio with an 
individual in the family history-nega¬ 
tive (low-risk) group for alcoholism. 
In my work, matched high- and 
low-risk men are then individually 
brought to the laboratory three times, 
where raters blind to the family his¬ 
tory measure personality attributes, 
cognitive and psychomotor func¬ 
tioning, and some electroencephalo-graphic 
(EEG) parameters. After 
these baseline procedures have been 
completed, subjects are administered 
one of three beverages; placebo, 0.75 
mL/kg of ethanol, or 1.1 mL/kg of 
ethanol (roughly the equivalent of 
about three and six drinks, respec¬ 
tively), with active doses consumed 
over ten minutes as a 20% by volume 
solution in a sugar-free carbonated 
beverage. Subjects are then observed 
over the subsequent four hours. 
SOME RESULTS OF STUDIES OF 
HIGH-RISK POPULATIONS 
This section briefly reviews results 
from some studies of populations at 
high risk for alcoholism. While I 
emphasize my own work, relevant 
data from different laboratories are 
also included. The results relate to 
differences between high- and low-risk 
groups in both baseline function¬ 
ing and in response to an ethanol 
challenge. 
Three possible differences between 
the high- and low-risk groups stand 
out as a result of these studies of 
groups at elevated risk. Sons of alco¬ 
holics appear to show less intense 
responses to modest ethanol doses, 
demonstrate lower amplitudes of a 
brain wave that might measure selec¬ 
tive attention, and may have different 
characteristics of brain a rhythms. 
Other interesting and potentially im¬ 
portant results are also reviewed in 
the following paragraphs. 
A Decreased Intensity of 
Response to Ethanol 
The risk of developing alcohol-related 
problems might increase if a 
person were relatively less able to 
estimate how intoxicated he was 
becoming at a modest blood alcohol 
concentration.12 This could make it 
more difficult to learn when to stop 
drinking; ie, when a few more drinks 
will result in drunkenness. To test the 
possibility that this might relate to 
the future alcoholism risk, in my own 
and other laboratories men in highl¬ 
and low-risk groups have been admin¬ 
istered a series of subjective, cogni-tive/ 
psychomotor, and biological 
tests before and after beverage alco¬ 
hol consumption. 
Before consuming a test drink, the 
two family history groups had similar 
expectations of what ethanol was 
likely to do to them.13 u Also, the blood 
alcohol concentrations achieved fol¬ 
lowing the two active ethanol chal¬ 
lenges were identical for the two 
family groups, indicating similar pat¬ 
terns of absorption, distribution, and 
metabolism of ethanol."" High-risk 
and low-risk group members have 
also shown parallel changes on most 
measures of intoxication following 
Downloaded from jama.ama-assn.org at Mt Sinai School Of Medicine on April 23, 2012
placebo. Thus, any family history 
group differences on the intensity of 
acute reaction to ethanol are unlikely 
to reflect levels of expectancy, placebo 
response, or differences in blood alco¬ 
hol concentrations. 
The first indication that high- and 
low-risk groups might differ signifi¬ 
cantly in their actual reaction to 
ethanol came from self-reports of 
levels of intoxication following chal¬ 
lenges. Even after partialing out the 
subjects' feelings after drinking pla¬ 
cebo, three studies in my own labora¬ 
tory and two additional investiga¬ 
tions have shown that the group with 
a positive family history for alcohol¬ 
ism report significantly less subjec¬ 
tive intoxication after drinking than 
the group with a negative family 
history.13 " These data are exemplified 
in Fig 1, which shows the significant 
differences in the two groups after 
the lower ethanol challenge (0.75 mL/ 
kg). A similar trend was also 
observed after achieving higher blood 
alcohol concentrations (eg, after 1.1 
mL/kg of ethanol), but the group 
differences were not as marked. 
Aspects of cognitive and psychomo-tor 
test performance as well as some 
biological reactions after drinking 
parallel the subjective levels of intox¬ 
ication. In my laboratory, subjects 
with a positive family history demon¬ 
strated significantly less increase in 
the amount of ethanol induced body 
sway or static ataxia (a modified 
Romberg sign) than those in the 
low-risk group (Fig 2); again, there 
was a greater differential following 
the lower ethanol dose." Further bol¬ 
stering these results, preliminary 
data showed a greater decrement in 
performance on a number of cognitive 
and psychomotor tests (eg, a divided-attention 
task, the Trail Making Test, 
etc) in the low-risk group, as well as 
more intense changes in two hor¬ 
mones known to react to an acute 
ethanol challenge, cortisol and prolac-tin. 
1718 
In summary, there is consistent 
evidence that those in the high-risk 
group demonstrate significantly less 
intense reactions to modest doses of 
ethanol than those in the low-risk 
group. It may be that they are feeling 
less ethanol effect at the blood alco¬ 
hol concentrations at which most peo¬ 
ple make a decision to stop drinking. 
Electrophysiological Measures 
Two additional promising areas of 
research involve EEG differences be¬ 
tween the high- and low-risk groups. 
First, brain-stem auditory event-related 
potentials (ERPs) are com-puter- 
averaged brain waves mea¬ 
sured by exposing subjects to a train 
of regular stimuli (eg, clicks or 
flashes of light) as they are asked to 
discern a randomly occurring unusual 
stimulus.' When the anticipated rare 
event occurs (eg, a tone of a different 
frequency than the others), a positive 
brain wave is recorded between 300 
and 500 ms (the P300) following the 
stimulus. Studies of préadolescent 
sons of alcoholics by Begleiter and 
colleagues' have revealed a lower 
P300 amplitude for these boys, even 
without ethanol. This might indicate 
that some of these young men may 
experience difficulties in adequately 
focusing attention on their surround¬ 
ings. It is possible that this phenome¬ 
non might contribute to the decreased 
sensitivity to modest ethanol doses 
reported in my work. 
Second, alcoholics seem to have a 
deficiency in the amount of a rhythm, 
or slow waves, in their background 
cortical EEGs." Similar a-wave defi¬ 
ciencies may be seen in sons of alco¬ 
holics, along with a possible greater 
increase for waves in the a band after 
drinking.20 If the amount of a rhythm 
relates to feelings of relaxation,21 then 
these data may indicate that there is 
a qualitatively different type of 
"high" in the high- and low-risk 
groups. 
Some Additional 
Areas of Research With 
High-Risk Populations 
A number of studies have ad¬ 
dressed whether high- and low-risk 
groups differ on the metabolism of 
ethanol. The two family history 
groups have similar blood alcohol 
concentration patterns after drink¬ 
ing,13"22 but there is a possible differ¬ 
ence between the groups on the level 
of accumulation of the toxic and psy-choactive 
first breakdown product of 
ethanol, acetaldehyde. While not all 
studies agree,2324 three groups have 
reported that blood and breath levels 
of this potent substance tend to be 
higher after drinking in the sons of 
alcoholics than in controls.2527 How¬ 
ever, conclusions are undermined by 
inadequate technology for the mea¬ 
surement of acetaldehyde, with re¬ 
sulting disagreements about the va¬ 
lidity and sensitivity of the assay procedures.23 
A variety of studies have also 
looked at personality profiles of high-risk 
and low-risk pairs. In my labora¬ 
tory, using students closely matched 
for demography and drinking history, 
the two family groups were similar on 
most subtests of the Minnesota Multi-phasic 
Personality Inventory (MMPI), 
the Eysenck Personality Inven¬ 
tory measures of extroversion and neu-roticism, 
levels of anxiety, and the 
feelings of control over external 
events.2831 Other researchers, looking 
at less highly selected groups usually 
identified as a consequence of police 
problems or because their fathers 
were currently in treatment for alco¬ 
holism, have reported profiles of 
increased risk-taking and impulsivity 
in those with a positive family histo¬ 
ry32 and indicated a possible associa¬ 
tion between signs of hyperactivity in 
childhood and the later development 
of alcoholism.33 The divergence of 
results from different studies does 
not justify solid conclusions on the 
association between measurable per¬ 
sonality attributes and the risk for 
the future development of alcoholism, 
but this remains an important area 
for future research. 
Young children of alcoholics have 
also been reported to demonstrate 
poorer performance on verbal intelli¬ 
gence and the Categories Test of the 
Halstead-Reitan Battery and tend to 
show more difficulties in psycho-motor 
performance, abstracting 
abilities, and auditory wordspan per¬ 
formance.10" However, studies of col¬ 
lege-age children of alcoholics have 
revealed no differences before ethanol 
challenge on body sway, memory, and 
divided-attention tasks, nor on other 
cognitive or psychomotor test per¬ 
formance measures.13 " 3435 In addition, 
prospective studies of population co¬ 
horts that included some individuals 
who later became alcoholic have dem¬ 
onstrated no consistent correlation 
between relatively obvious neurologi¬ 
cal or intellectual problems and the 
later development of alcoholism.34 In 
the final analysis, the associations 
between the alcoholism risk and mea¬ 
surable signs of neurological damage 
in children of the average alcoholic 
are complex and might differ with the 
sample selected for study. 
CONCLUSIONS 
This discussion has centered on 
some exciting research generated 
over the last decade. The studies 
suggest that genetic influences are 
important in alcoholism and reflect 
multiple genes interacting with envi¬ 
ronment to produce a final level of 
risk. In this theory, no one is predes¬ 
tined to become an alcoholic, but 
genetic factors increase or decrease 
the level of vulnerability toward this 
disorder. Taking advantage of the 
fourfold higher risk in sons of alco¬ 
holics, subjects predisposed toward 
Downloaded from jama.ama-assn.org at Mt Sinai School Of Medicine on April 23, 2012
alcoholism have been studied in the 
preteen years and in the early 20s, 
with populations selected from cam¬ 
puses, court dockets, and from fami¬ 
lies of alcoholics in treatment. The 
evaluation of levels of functioning 
both before and after an ethanol 
challenge has revealed some interest¬ 
ing differences between high-risk 
populations and controls. 
The most consistent data indicate 
that an alcoholism vulnerability 
might be, in part, related to factors 
that result in a decreased intensity of 
reaction to ethanol. This might subse¬ 
quently interfere with the ability to 
learn to discriminate the blood alco¬ 
hol concentrations at which drinking 
should cease during an evening. Other 
studies indicate possible differences 
between groups with a negative or 
positive family history on the ability 
to attend adequately to a stimulus 
(one possible mechanism for an 
inability to discriminate modest blood 
alcohol concentration effects) and 
potential differences on the effects of 
ethanol on a. rhythms in the back¬ 
ground cortical EEGs (perhaps re¬ 
flecting the ability to feel relaxation 
in the presence of ethanol). 
Despite this progress, these studies 
are still in their infancy. Research 
must next establish the generalizabil-ity 
of results to women and divergent 
ethnic and socioeconomic groups. 
Also, the actual biological mecha¬ 
nisms responsible for the reactions to 
ethanol and/or EEG attributes of a 
positive family history of alcoholism 
must be elucidated, the level of 
genetic control of these phenomena 
must be established, and the actual 
linkage, if any, to the final develop¬ 
ment of alcoholism must be proved. 
Even recognizing these limitations, 
there are a number of implications of 
the data for the daily practice of 
medicine. First, the evidence support¬ 
ing the importance of genetic factors 
in this disorder, combined with clini¬ 
cal experience, underscores the im¬ 
portance of recognizing that alco¬ 
holism is a biologically influenced 
problem, not a moral weakness. Many 
physicians may need to change their 
stereotype of alcoholics or they will 
continue to misdiagnose the average 
middle-class alcoholic who needs 
help.3637 Second, no matter how they 
are mediated, the genetic factors can 
help us begin to work on preventing 
this illness. While the optimum 
approach to prevention has not been 
found, it makes sense that children of 
alcoholics should be educated about 
their risk, taught that they may not 
react to alcohol the way their peers 
do, and informed that attempting to 
drink like others could be a dangerous 
undertaking. 
Finally, the implications for the 
future of prevention and treatment of 
alcoholism are even more marked. 
Identifying factors that actually in¬ 
crease the risk could help us to pin¬ 
point those children of alcoholics who 
are most likely to become alcoholic 
themselves and to develop more effec¬ 
tive and specific prevention ap¬ 
proaches. Similarly, understanding 
more about factors influencing the 
development of alcoholism might help 
clinicians to discover more effective 
treatment approaches. 
This work was supported by grant PHS 
AA05526-03 from the National Institute on Alco¬ 
hol Abuse and Alcoholism, by the Veterans 
Administration Research Service, and by a grant 
from the Joan B. Kroc Foundation. 
Thanks to Eric Gold, Karen Croot, and Chey-vonne 
Frontiero for their devotion to excellence 
in the series of studies reported herein. 
References 
1. Schuckit MA: The epidemiology of alcohol-ism, 
in Schuckit MA (ed): Alcohol Patterns and 
Problems, Series in Psychosocial Epidemiology. 
New Brunswick, NJ, Rutgers Press, 1985, vol 5, 
pp 1-42. 
2. Robins LN, et al: Lifetime prevalence of 
specific psychiatric disorders in sites. Arch Gen 
Psychiatry 1984;41:949-958. 
3. Cotton NS: The familial incidence of alco-holism: 
A review. J Stud Alcohol 1979;40:89-116. 
4. Gurling HM: Genetic epidemiology in medi-cine 
p=m-recenttwin research. Br Med J 1984; 
288:3-5. 
5. Schuckit MA: Twin studies on substance 
abuse: An overview, in Gedda L, Parisi P, Nance 
W (eds): Twin Research: 3. Epidemiological and 
Clinical Studies. New York, Alan R Liss Inc, 
1981. 
6. Goodwin DW, Schulsinger F, Moller N, et 
al: Drinking problems in adopted and non-adopted 
sons of alcoholics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 
1974;31:164-169. 
7. Bohman M, Sigvardsson S, Cloninger R: 
Material inheritance of alcohol abuse. Arch Gen 
Psychiatry 1981;38:965-969. 
8. Schuckit MA: Studies of populations at high 
risk for alcoholism. Psychiatr Dev 1985;3:31-63. 
9. Begleiter H, Porjesz B, Bihari B, et al: 
Event-related brain potentials in boys at risk for 
alcoholism. Science 1984;225:1493-1495. 
10. Schaeffer KW, Parsons OQ, Yohman JR: 
Neuropsychological differences between male 
familial and nonfamilial alcoholics and nonalco-holics. 
Alcoholism Clin Exp Res 1984;8:347-358. 
11. Hedegus AM, Alterman AI, Tarter RE: 
Learning achievement in sons of alcoholics. 
Alcoholism Clin Exp Res 1984;8:330-333. 
12. Lipscomb TR, Nathan PE: Blood alcohol 
level discrimination: The effects of family his-tory 
and alcoholism, drinking pattern, and toler-ance. 
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980;37:571-576. 
13. Schuckit MA: Subjective responses to alco-hol 
in sons of alcoholics and controls. Arch Gen 
Psychiatry 1984;41:879-884. 
14. O'Malley SS, Maisto SA: The effects of 
family drinking history on responses to alcohol: 
Expectancies and reactions to intoxication. J 
Stud Alcohol 1985;46:289-297. 
15. Mednick SA: Subjects at risk for alcohol-ism: 
Recent reports. Presented at the 14th 
Annual Medical Conference Scientific Confer-ence 
of the National Alcoholism Forum (Re-search 
Society on Alcoholism), Houston, April 4, 
1983. 
16. Schuckit MA: Ethanol-induced changes in 
body sway in men at high alcoholism risk. Arch 
Gen Psychiatry 1985;42:375-379. 
17. Schuckit MA, Parker DC, Rossman LR: 
Ethanol-related prolactin responses and risk for 
alcoholism. Biol Psychiatry 1983;18:1153-1159. 
18. Schuckit MA: Differences in plasma corti-sol 
after ingestion of ethanol in relatives of 
alcoholics and controls: Preliminary results. J 
Clin Psychiatry 1984;45:374-376. 
19. Propping P, Kruger J, Mark N: Genetic 
disposition to alcoholism: An EEG study in 
alcoholics and their relatives. Hum Genet 1981; 
59:51-59. 
20. Pollock VE, Volavka J, Goodwin DW, et al: 
The EEG after alcohol administration in men at 
risk for alcoholism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 
40:857-861. 
21. Schuckit MA, Engstrom D, Alpert R, et al: 
Differences in muscle-tension response to etha-nol 
in young men with and without family 
histories of alcoholism. J Stud Alcohol 1981; 
42:918-924. 
22. Schuckit MA: Peak blood alcohol levels in 
men at high risk for the future development of 
alcoholism. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res 1981;5:64x=req- 
66. 
23. Eriksson CJ: Human blood acetaldehyde 
concentration during ethanol oxidation (update, 
1982). Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1983;18:141x=req- 
150. 
24. Behar D, Berg CJ, Rapoport JL: Behavioral 
and physiological effects of ethanol in high-risk 
and control children: A pilot study. Alcoholism 
Clin Exp Res 1983;7:404-410. 
25. Schuckit MA, Rayses V: Ethanol ingestion: 
Differences in blood acetaldehyde concentra-tions 
in relatives of alcoholics and controls. 
Science 1979;203:54-55. 
26. Zeiner AR, Krug R, Kegg P, et al: Cardio-vascular 
and pharmacokinetic effects of ethanol 
in offspring of alcoholics and social drinkers. 
Presented at the 20th International Congress of 
Applied Psychology, Edinburgh, Scotland, July 
12, 1982. 
27. Ward K, Weir DG, McCrodden JM, et al: 
Blood acetaldehyde levels in relatives of alcohol-ics 
following ethanol ingestion. ICRS Med Sci 
1983;11:950. 
28. Saunders GR, Schuckit MA: Brief commu-nication: 
MMPI scores in young men with alco-holic 
relatives and controls. J Nerv Ment Dis 
1981;168:456-458. 
29. Schuckit MA: Anxiety and assertiveness in 
the relatives of alcoholics and controls. J Clin 
Psychiatry 1982;43:238-239. 
30. Schuckit MA: Extroversion and neuroti-cism 
in young men. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 
140:1223-1224. 
31. Morrison C, Schuckit MA: Locus of control 
in young men with alcoholic relatives and con-trols. 
J Clin Psychiatry 1983;44:306-307. 
32. MacAndrew C: On the possibility of the 
psychometric detection of persons who are prone 
to the abuse of alcohol and other substances. 
Addict Behav 1979;4:11-20. 
33. Goodwin DW, Schulsinger F, Hermansen 
L, et al: Alcoholism and the hyperactive child 
syndrome. J Nerv Ment Dis 1975;160:349-353. 
34. Vaillant GE, Milofsky ES: The etiology of 
alcoholism: A prospective viewpoint. Am Psychol 
1982;37:494-502. 
35. Schuckit MA: Genetic and biochemical 
factors in the etiology of alcoholism, in Grin-spoon 
L (ed): Psychiatry Update. Washington, 
DC, American Psychiatric Press Inc, 1984, vol 3, 
pp 320-327. 
36. Schuckit MA: Drug and Alcohol Abuse: A 
Clinical Guide to Diagnosis and Treatment. New 
York, Plenum Publishing Corp, 1984. 
37. Schuckit MA: Alcoholism and other psy-chiatric 
disorders. Hosp Community Psychiatry 
1983;34:1022-1027. 
Downloaded from jama.ama-assn.org at Mt Sinai School Of Medicine on April 23, 2012

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Kürzlich hochgeladen

Introduction,importance and scope of horticulture.pptx
Introduction,importance and scope of horticulture.pptxIntroduction,importance and scope of horticulture.pptx
Introduction,importance and scope of horticulture.pptx
Bhagirath Gogikar
 
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disksFormation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
Sérgio Sacani
 
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
PirithiRaju
 
Pests of mustard_Identification_Management_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of mustard_Identification_Management_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of mustard_Identification_Management_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of mustard_Identification_Management_Dr.UPR.pdf
PirithiRaju
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

CELL -Structural and Functional unit of life.pdf
CELL -Structural and Functional unit of life.pdfCELL -Structural and Functional unit of life.pdf
CELL -Structural and Functional unit of life.pdf
 
Introduction,importance and scope of horticulture.pptx
Introduction,importance and scope of horticulture.pptxIntroduction,importance and scope of horticulture.pptx
Introduction,importance and scope of horticulture.pptx
 
Factory Acceptance Test( FAT).pptx .
Factory Acceptance Test( FAT).pptx       .Factory Acceptance Test( FAT).pptx       .
Factory Acceptance Test( FAT).pptx .
 
GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)
GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)
GBSN - Biochemistry (Unit 1)
 
Clean In Place(CIP).pptx .
Clean In Place(CIP).pptx                 .Clean In Place(CIP).pptx                 .
Clean In Place(CIP).pptx .
 
Site Acceptance Test .
Site Acceptance Test                    .Site Acceptance Test                    .
Site Acceptance Test .
 
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disksFormation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
 
COST ESTIMATION FOR A RESEARCH PROJECT.pptx
COST ESTIMATION FOR A RESEARCH PROJECT.pptxCOST ESTIMATION FOR A RESEARCH PROJECT.pptx
COST ESTIMATION FOR A RESEARCH PROJECT.pptx
 
9999266834 Call Girls In Noida Sector 22 (Delhi) Call Girl Service
9999266834 Call Girls In Noida Sector 22 (Delhi) Call Girl Service9999266834 Call Girls In Noida Sector 22 (Delhi) Call Girl Service
9999266834 Call Girls In Noida Sector 22 (Delhi) Call Girl Service
 
Call Girls Alandi Call Me 7737669865 Budget Friendly No Advance Booking
Call Girls Alandi Call Me 7737669865 Budget Friendly No Advance BookingCall Girls Alandi Call Me 7737669865 Budget Friendly No Advance Booking
Call Girls Alandi Call Me 7737669865 Budget Friendly No Advance Booking
 
PSYCHOSOCIAL NEEDS. in nursing II sem pptx
PSYCHOSOCIAL NEEDS. in nursing II sem pptxPSYCHOSOCIAL NEEDS. in nursing II sem pptx
PSYCHOSOCIAL NEEDS. in nursing II sem pptx
 
Connaught Place, Delhi Call girls :8448380779 Model Escorts | 100% verified
Connaught Place, Delhi Call girls :8448380779 Model Escorts | 100% verifiedConnaught Place, Delhi Call girls :8448380779 Model Escorts | 100% verified
Connaught Place, Delhi Call girls :8448380779 Model Escorts | 100% verified
 
High Profile 🔝 8250077686 📞 Call Girls Service in GTB Nagar🍑
High Profile 🔝 8250077686 📞 Call Girls Service in GTB Nagar🍑High Profile 🔝 8250077686 📞 Call Girls Service in GTB Nagar🍑
High Profile 🔝 8250077686 📞 Call Girls Service in GTB Nagar🍑
 
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
 
Locating and isolating a gene, FISH, GISH, Chromosome walking and jumping, te...
Locating and isolating a gene, FISH, GISH, Chromosome walking and jumping, te...Locating and isolating a gene, FISH, GISH, Chromosome walking and jumping, te...
Locating and isolating a gene, FISH, GISH, Chromosome walking and jumping, te...
 
Kochi ❤CALL GIRL 84099*07087 ❤CALL GIRLS IN Kochi ESCORT SERVICE❤CALL GIRL
Kochi ❤CALL GIRL 84099*07087 ❤CALL GIRLS IN Kochi ESCORT SERVICE❤CALL GIRLKochi ❤CALL GIRL 84099*07087 ❤CALL GIRLS IN Kochi ESCORT SERVICE❤CALL GIRL
Kochi ❤CALL GIRL 84099*07087 ❤CALL GIRLS IN Kochi ESCORT SERVICE❤CALL GIRL
 
FAIRSpectra - Enabling the FAIRification of Spectroscopy and Spectrometry
FAIRSpectra - Enabling the FAIRification of Spectroscopy and SpectrometryFAIRSpectra - Enabling the FAIRification of Spectroscopy and Spectrometry
FAIRSpectra - Enabling the FAIRification of Spectroscopy and Spectrometry
 
Pests of mustard_Identification_Management_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of mustard_Identification_Management_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of mustard_Identification_Management_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of mustard_Identification_Management_Dr.UPR.pdf
 
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 3)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 3)GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 3)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 3)
 
9654467111 Call Girls In Raj Nagar Delhi Short 1500 Night 6000
9654467111 Call Girls In Raj Nagar Delhi Short 1500 Night 60009654467111 Call Girls In Raj Nagar Delhi Short 1500 Night 6000
9654467111 Call Girls In Raj Nagar Delhi Short 1500 Night 6000
 

Empfohlen

Social Media Marketing Trends 2024 // The Global Indie Insights
Social Media Marketing Trends 2024 // The Global Indie InsightsSocial Media Marketing Trends 2024 // The Global Indie Insights
Social Media Marketing Trends 2024 // The Global Indie Insights
Kurio // The Social Media Age(ncy)
 
Good Stuff Happens in 1:1 Meetings: Why you need them and how to do them well
Good Stuff Happens in 1:1 Meetings: Why you need them and how to do them wellGood Stuff Happens in 1:1 Meetings: Why you need them and how to do them well
Good Stuff Happens in 1:1 Meetings: Why you need them and how to do them well
Saba Software
 

Empfohlen (20)

Content Methodology: A Best Practices Report (Webinar)
Content Methodology: A Best Practices Report (Webinar)Content Methodology: A Best Practices Report (Webinar)
Content Methodology: A Best Practices Report (Webinar)
 
How to Prepare For a Successful Job Search for 2024
How to Prepare For a Successful Job Search for 2024How to Prepare For a Successful Job Search for 2024
How to Prepare For a Successful Job Search for 2024
 
Social Media Marketing Trends 2024 // The Global Indie Insights
Social Media Marketing Trends 2024 // The Global Indie InsightsSocial Media Marketing Trends 2024 // The Global Indie Insights
Social Media Marketing Trends 2024 // The Global Indie Insights
 
Trends In Paid Search: Navigating The Digital Landscape In 2024
Trends In Paid Search: Navigating The Digital Landscape In 2024Trends In Paid Search: Navigating The Digital Landscape In 2024
Trends In Paid Search: Navigating The Digital Landscape In 2024
 
5 Public speaking tips from TED - Visualized summary
5 Public speaking tips from TED - Visualized summary5 Public speaking tips from TED - Visualized summary
5 Public speaking tips from TED - Visualized summary
 
ChatGPT and the Future of Work - Clark Boyd
ChatGPT and the Future of Work - Clark Boyd ChatGPT and the Future of Work - Clark Boyd
ChatGPT and the Future of Work - Clark Boyd
 
Getting into the tech field. what next
Getting into the tech field. what next Getting into the tech field. what next
Getting into the tech field. what next
 
Google's Just Not That Into You: Understanding Core Updates & Search Intent
Google's Just Not That Into You: Understanding Core Updates & Search IntentGoogle's Just Not That Into You: Understanding Core Updates & Search Intent
Google's Just Not That Into You: Understanding Core Updates & Search Intent
 
How to have difficult conversations
How to have difficult conversations How to have difficult conversations
How to have difficult conversations
 
Introduction to Data Science
Introduction to Data ScienceIntroduction to Data Science
Introduction to Data Science
 
Time Management & Productivity - Best Practices
Time Management & Productivity -  Best PracticesTime Management & Productivity -  Best Practices
Time Management & Productivity - Best Practices
 
The six step guide to practical project management
The six step guide to practical project managementThe six step guide to practical project management
The six step guide to practical project management
 
Beginners Guide to TikTok for Search - Rachel Pearson - We are Tilt __ Bright...
Beginners Guide to TikTok for Search - Rachel Pearson - We are Tilt __ Bright...Beginners Guide to TikTok for Search - Rachel Pearson - We are Tilt __ Bright...
Beginners Guide to TikTok for Search - Rachel Pearson - We are Tilt __ Bright...
 
Unlocking the Power of ChatGPT and AI in Testing - A Real-World Look, present...
Unlocking the Power of ChatGPT and AI in Testing - A Real-World Look, present...Unlocking the Power of ChatGPT and AI in Testing - A Real-World Look, present...
Unlocking the Power of ChatGPT and AI in Testing - A Real-World Look, present...
 
12 Ways to Increase Your Influence at Work
12 Ways to Increase Your Influence at Work12 Ways to Increase Your Influence at Work
12 Ways to Increase Your Influence at Work
 
ChatGPT webinar slides
ChatGPT webinar slidesChatGPT webinar slides
ChatGPT webinar slides
 
More than Just Lines on a Map: Best Practices for U.S Bike Routes
More than Just Lines on a Map: Best Practices for U.S Bike RoutesMore than Just Lines on a Map: Best Practices for U.S Bike Routes
More than Just Lines on a Map: Best Practices for U.S Bike Routes
 
Ride the Storm: Navigating Through Unstable Periods / Katerina Rudko (Belka G...
Ride the Storm: Navigating Through Unstable Periods / Katerina Rudko (Belka G...Ride the Storm: Navigating Through Unstable Periods / Katerina Rudko (Belka G...
Ride the Storm: Navigating Through Unstable Periods / Katerina Rudko (Belka G...
 
Barbie - Brand Strategy Presentation
Barbie - Brand Strategy PresentationBarbie - Brand Strategy Presentation
Barbie - Brand Strategy Presentation
 
Good Stuff Happens in 1:1 Meetings: Why you need them and how to do them well
Good Stuff Happens in 1:1 Meetings: Why you need them and how to do them wellGood Stuff Happens in 1:1 Meetings: Why you need them and how to do them well
Good Stuff Happens in 1:1 Meetings: Why you need them and how to do them well
 

Schuckit alcoholism (1) psychology A2 addiction

  • 1. M. Thérèse Southgate, MD, Section Coordinator Genetics and the Risk for Alcoholism Marc A. Schuckit, MD The importance of genetic factors in alcoholism is supported by the familial nature of this disorder, the significantly higher concordance rate in identical than in fraternal twins, and the fourfold higher risk for children of alcoholics, even when adopted out at birth. Based on this evidence, the search is under way to identify possible genetically influenced factors that might contribute to the alcoholism risk. Studies of populations at high risk for the future development of alcoholism (eg, sons of alcoholics) have revealed a probable decreased intensity of reaction to modest doses of ethanol, a possible decreased amplitude of the P300 wave of the event-related potential, and a possible decreased amount of g=a-waveactivity on the background cortical electroencephalogram. The implications of these and other findings and their impact on the practice of medicine are explored. (JAMA 1985;254:2614-2617) ALCOHOLISM afflicts 10% of adult men and 3% to 5% of adult women at some time during their lives, with an even higher rate among patients attending medical clinics.12 This arti¬ cle reviews studies evaluating genetic factors that might contribute to the risk for this prevalent disorder. The importance of genetics in the vulnerability toward alcoholism is supported by evidence from family, twin, and adoption studies in humans. First, the familial nature of alcohol¬ ism (ie, the way it occurs in families) has been documented for more than 100 years.1 The risk appears to increase with the number of alcoholic relatives and the closeness of the genetic relationship. However, many familial factors are not genetically influenced. The second approach, studies of twins, takes advantage of an experi¬ ment of nature. Twin pairs are born at the same time and are likely to experience major childhood events (eg, death of a parent) at the same age. Therefore, if childhood environ¬ ment is important in the development of alcoholism, the risk should be elevated in the twin of an alcoholic, no matter what type of twinship is involved. However, there are two types of twins; identical, who share 100% of their genes, and fraternal, who share only 50% (the same as any two full siblings). As a result, if alcoholism is genetically influenced, the risk for the identical twin of an alcoholic should be significantly high¬ er than the risk for a fraternal twin. While there is some debate,4 the majority of studies demonstrate a concordance of 60% or higher for the identical twin of an alcoholic but a risk of only 30% or less if the rela¬ tionship is fraternal.5 The most impressive evidence sup¬ porting the importance of genetic factors in alcoholism comes from adoption-type studies. Investigations from different countries using a vari¬ ety of methods have demonstrated that adopted-away children of alco¬ holics are at four times higher risk for this disorder than controls.*"8 Once the influence of a biological alcoholic parent is considered, being reared by an alcoholic does not seem to add to the risk, and children of nonalcoholics raised by alcoholics do not appear to have an enhanced rate of this prob¬ lem. RESEARCH APPROACHES WITH POPULATIONS AT HIGH RISK FOR ALCOHOLISM In response to these findings, a number of laboratories have begun to ask how the genetic risk might be mediated.8 One promising approach has been to evaluate nonalcoholic close relatives of alcoholics, limiting the study to men who are young enough to have not entered the major age of risk for alcoholism—ie, popula¬ tions at high future risk are observed. The assets of this research design include the almost inexhaustible number of potential subjects, the large number of families investigated so that numerous factors might be determined, and the ability to observe individuals at high risk before alco¬ holism actually develops. In most studies of populations at high risk, potential subjects are males who have an alcoholic family member (usually a first-degree rela¬ tive and most frequently the father). Some investigators have chosen to evaluate male children in the preteen or early teen years, thus increasing the probability that subjects are naive to the effects of ethanol. These young men are often chosen from among families of alcoholics attend¬ ing an alcoholic treatment program or from teenaged subjects identified because of police problems.9" Other investigations have focused on older groups to avoid the long lag time between evaluation and the actual development of alcoholism (should follow-up studies be planned). This also maximizes the opportunity to observe potentially important inter¬ actions between the genetic predispo¬ sition and adaptations to ethanol over years of modest drinking. Men are chosen for most studies because responses to an ethanol challenge might be affected by the phase of the From the Department of Psychiatry, University of California at San Diego School of Medicine, and the Alcohol Research Center, San Diego Veterans Administration Medical Center. Reprint requests to Department of Psychiatry, San Diego Veterans Administration Medical Center, 3350 La Jolla Village Dr, San Diego, CA 92161 (Dr Schuckit). Downloaded from jama.ama-assn.org at Mt Sinai School Of Medicine on April 23, 2012
  • 2. After Ethanol Ingestion, min Fig 1.—Mean self-ratings on a 0 (none) to 36 (great) scale for drug effect after placebo and after 0.75 mL/kg of ethanol for 23 matched pairs with positive (close circles) and negative (open circles) family histories. Bars indicate SEs. After 0.75 mL/kg, using a two-factor, mixed-model, repeated-measures analysis of variance in which sub¬ jects were nested with respect to the between factor (family history) and crossed with respect to the within factor (time), the differences between family groups were significant (F=5.99, 1,44 df, P<.02). (Figure reproduced from Archives of General Psychiatry 1984; 41:879-884.) 5 Baseline 140 170 240 After Ethanol Ingestion, min Fig 2.—Percent increase in body sway or standing steadiness following 0.75 mL/kg of ethanol for 23 matched pairs with positive (close circles) and negative (open circles) family histories. Bars indicate SEs. Using the same statistical approach as in Fig 1, after 0.75 mL/kg of ethanol the two family groups differed significantly (F=3.84, 3,197 df, P=.01). (Figure reproduced from Archives of General Psychiatry 1985;42:375-379.) menstrual cycle or the type of birth control pills being consumed by women. My own laboratory chose to sample 21- to 25-year-old male students and staff at a university who responded to a mailed questionnaire. After exclud¬ ing those persons who have serious alcohol- or drug-related life problems or major medical or phychiatric dis¬ orders, those drinking but nonalco¬ holic young men who report a close alcoholic relative are placed in the family history-positive or high-risk group. Each man with a positive family history is matched on de¬ mography (age, sex, religion, race, educational level, etc), drinking histo¬ ry, and height-to-weight ratio with an individual in the family history-nega¬ tive (low-risk) group for alcoholism. In my work, matched high- and low-risk men are then individually brought to the laboratory three times, where raters blind to the family his¬ tory measure personality attributes, cognitive and psychomotor func¬ tioning, and some electroencephalo-graphic (EEG) parameters. After these baseline procedures have been completed, subjects are administered one of three beverages; placebo, 0.75 mL/kg of ethanol, or 1.1 mL/kg of ethanol (roughly the equivalent of about three and six drinks, respec¬ tively), with active doses consumed over ten minutes as a 20% by volume solution in a sugar-free carbonated beverage. Subjects are then observed over the subsequent four hours. SOME RESULTS OF STUDIES OF HIGH-RISK POPULATIONS This section briefly reviews results from some studies of populations at high risk for alcoholism. While I emphasize my own work, relevant data from different laboratories are also included. The results relate to differences between high- and low-risk groups in both baseline function¬ ing and in response to an ethanol challenge. Three possible differences between the high- and low-risk groups stand out as a result of these studies of groups at elevated risk. Sons of alco¬ holics appear to show less intense responses to modest ethanol doses, demonstrate lower amplitudes of a brain wave that might measure selec¬ tive attention, and may have different characteristics of brain a rhythms. Other interesting and potentially im¬ portant results are also reviewed in the following paragraphs. A Decreased Intensity of Response to Ethanol The risk of developing alcohol-related problems might increase if a person were relatively less able to estimate how intoxicated he was becoming at a modest blood alcohol concentration.12 This could make it more difficult to learn when to stop drinking; ie, when a few more drinks will result in drunkenness. To test the possibility that this might relate to the future alcoholism risk, in my own and other laboratories men in highl¬ and low-risk groups have been admin¬ istered a series of subjective, cogni-tive/ psychomotor, and biological tests before and after beverage alco¬ hol consumption. Before consuming a test drink, the two family history groups had similar expectations of what ethanol was likely to do to them.13 u Also, the blood alcohol concentrations achieved fol¬ lowing the two active ethanol chal¬ lenges were identical for the two family groups, indicating similar pat¬ terns of absorption, distribution, and metabolism of ethanol."" High-risk and low-risk group members have also shown parallel changes on most measures of intoxication following Downloaded from jama.ama-assn.org at Mt Sinai School Of Medicine on April 23, 2012
  • 3. placebo. Thus, any family history group differences on the intensity of acute reaction to ethanol are unlikely to reflect levels of expectancy, placebo response, or differences in blood alco¬ hol concentrations. The first indication that high- and low-risk groups might differ signifi¬ cantly in their actual reaction to ethanol came from self-reports of levels of intoxication following chal¬ lenges. Even after partialing out the subjects' feelings after drinking pla¬ cebo, three studies in my own labora¬ tory and two additional investiga¬ tions have shown that the group with a positive family history for alcohol¬ ism report significantly less subjec¬ tive intoxication after drinking than the group with a negative family history.13 " These data are exemplified in Fig 1, which shows the significant differences in the two groups after the lower ethanol challenge (0.75 mL/ kg). A similar trend was also observed after achieving higher blood alcohol concentrations (eg, after 1.1 mL/kg of ethanol), but the group differences were not as marked. Aspects of cognitive and psychomo-tor test performance as well as some biological reactions after drinking parallel the subjective levels of intox¬ ication. In my laboratory, subjects with a positive family history demon¬ strated significantly less increase in the amount of ethanol induced body sway or static ataxia (a modified Romberg sign) than those in the low-risk group (Fig 2); again, there was a greater differential following the lower ethanol dose." Further bol¬ stering these results, preliminary data showed a greater decrement in performance on a number of cognitive and psychomotor tests (eg, a divided-attention task, the Trail Making Test, etc) in the low-risk group, as well as more intense changes in two hor¬ mones known to react to an acute ethanol challenge, cortisol and prolac-tin. 1718 In summary, there is consistent evidence that those in the high-risk group demonstrate significantly less intense reactions to modest doses of ethanol than those in the low-risk group. It may be that they are feeling less ethanol effect at the blood alco¬ hol concentrations at which most peo¬ ple make a decision to stop drinking. Electrophysiological Measures Two additional promising areas of research involve EEG differences be¬ tween the high- and low-risk groups. First, brain-stem auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) are com-puter- averaged brain waves mea¬ sured by exposing subjects to a train of regular stimuli (eg, clicks or flashes of light) as they are asked to discern a randomly occurring unusual stimulus.' When the anticipated rare event occurs (eg, a tone of a different frequency than the others), a positive brain wave is recorded between 300 and 500 ms (the P300) following the stimulus. Studies of préadolescent sons of alcoholics by Begleiter and colleagues' have revealed a lower P300 amplitude for these boys, even without ethanol. This might indicate that some of these young men may experience difficulties in adequately focusing attention on their surround¬ ings. It is possible that this phenome¬ non might contribute to the decreased sensitivity to modest ethanol doses reported in my work. Second, alcoholics seem to have a deficiency in the amount of a rhythm, or slow waves, in their background cortical EEGs." Similar a-wave defi¬ ciencies may be seen in sons of alco¬ holics, along with a possible greater increase for waves in the a band after drinking.20 If the amount of a rhythm relates to feelings of relaxation,21 then these data may indicate that there is a qualitatively different type of "high" in the high- and low-risk groups. Some Additional Areas of Research With High-Risk Populations A number of studies have ad¬ dressed whether high- and low-risk groups differ on the metabolism of ethanol. The two family history groups have similar blood alcohol concentration patterns after drink¬ ing,13"22 but there is a possible differ¬ ence between the groups on the level of accumulation of the toxic and psy-choactive first breakdown product of ethanol, acetaldehyde. While not all studies agree,2324 three groups have reported that blood and breath levels of this potent substance tend to be higher after drinking in the sons of alcoholics than in controls.2527 How¬ ever, conclusions are undermined by inadequate technology for the mea¬ surement of acetaldehyde, with re¬ sulting disagreements about the va¬ lidity and sensitivity of the assay procedures.23 A variety of studies have also looked at personality profiles of high-risk and low-risk pairs. In my labora¬ tory, using students closely matched for demography and drinking history, the two family groups were similar on most subtests of the Minnesota Multi-phasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), the Eysenck Personality Inven¬ tory measures of extroversion and neu-roticism, levels of anxiety, and the feelings of control over external events.2831 Other researchers, looking at less highly selected groups usually identified as a consequence of police problems or because their fathers were currently in treatment for alco¬ holism, have reported profiles of increased risk-taking and impulsivity in those with a positive family histo¬ ry32 and indicated a possible associa¬ tion between signs of hyperactivity in childhood and the later development of alcoholism.33 The divergence of results from different studies does not justify solid conclusions on the association between measurable per¬ sonality attributes and the risk for the future development of alcoholism, but this remains an important area for future research. Young children of alcoholics have also been reported to demonstrate poorer performance on verbal intelli¬ gence and the Categories Test of the Halstead-Reitan Battery and tend to show more difficulties in psycho-motor performance, abstracting abilities, and auditory wordspan per¬ formance.10" However, studies of col¬ lege-age children of alcoholics have revealed no differences before ethanol challenge on body sway, memory, and divided-attention tasks, nor on other cognitive or psychomotor test per¬ formance measures.13 " 3435 In addition, prospective studies of population co¬ horts that included some individuals who later became alcoholic have dem¬ onstrated no consistent correlation between relatively obvious neurologi¬ cal or intellectual problems and the later development of alcoholism.34 In the final analysis, the associations between the alcoholism risk and mea¬ surable signs of neurological damage in children of the average alcoholic are complex and might differ with the sample selected for study. CONCLUSIONS This discussion has centered on some exciting research generated over the last decade. The studies suggest that genetic influences are important in alcoholism and reflect multiple genes interacting with envi¬ ronment to produce a final level of risk. In this theory, no one is predes¬ tined to become an alcoholic, but genetic factors increase or decrease the level of vulnerability toward this disorder. Taking advantage of the fourfold higher risk in sons of alco¬ holics, subjects predisposed toward Downloaded from jama.ama-assn.org at Mt Sinai School Of Medicine on April 23, 2012
  • 4. alcoholism have been studied in the preteen years and in the early 20s, with populations selected from cam¬ puses, court dockets, and from fami¬ lies of alcoholics in treatment. The evaluation of levels of functioning both before and after an ethanol challenge has revealed some interest¬ ing differences between high-risk populations and controls. The most consistent data indicate that an alcoholism vulnerability might be, in part, related to factors that result in a decreased intensity of reaction to ethanol. This might subse¬ quently interfere with the ability to learn to discriminate the blood alco¬ hol concentrations at which drinking should cease during an evening. Other studies indicate possible differences between groups with a negative or positive family history on the ability to attend adequately to a stimulus (one possible mechanism for an inability to discriminate modest blood alcohol concentration effects) and potential differences on the effects of ethanol on a. rhythms in the back¬ ground cortical EEGs (perhaps re¬ flecting the ability to feel relaxation in the presence of ethanol). Despite this progress, these studies are still in their infancy. Research must next establish the generalizabil-ity of results to women and divergent ethnic and socioeconomic groups. Also, the actual biological mecha¬ nisms responsible for the reactions to ethanol and/or EEG attributes of a positive family history of alcoholism must be elucidated, the level of genetic control of these phenomena must be established, and the actual linkage, if any, to the final develop¬ ment of alcoholism must be proved. Even recognizing these limitations, there are a number of implications of the data for the daily practice of medicine. First, the evidence support¬ ing the importance of genetic factors in this disorder, combined with clini¬ cal experience, underscores the im¬ portance of recognizing that alco¬ holism is a biologically influenced problem, not a moral weakness. Many physicians may need to change their stereotype of alcoholics or they will continue to misdiagnose the average middle-class alcoholic who needs help.3637 Second, no matter how they are mediated, the genetic factors can help us begin to work on preventing this illness. While the optimum approach to prevention has not been found, it makes sense that children of alcoholics should be educated about their risk, taught that they may not react to alcohol the way their peers do, and informed that attempting to drink like others could be a dangerous undertaking. Finally, the implications for the future of prevention and treatment of alcoholism are even more marked. Identifying factors that actually in¬ crease the risk could help us to pin¬ point those children of alcoholics who are most likely to become alcoholic themselves and to develop more effec¬ tive and specific prevention ap¬ proaches. Similarly, understanding more about factors influencing the development of alcoholism might help clinicians to discover more effective treatment approaches. This work was supported by grant PHS AA05526-03 from the National Institute on Alco¬ hol Abuse and Alcoholism, by the Veterans Administration Research Service, and by a grant from the Joan B. Kroc Foundation. Thanks to Eric Gold, Karen Croot, and Chey-vonne Frontiero for their devotion to excellence in the series of studies reported herein. References 1. Schuckit MA: The epidemiology of alcohol-ism, in Schuckit MA (ed): Alcohol Patterns and Problems, Series in Psychosocial Epidemiology. New Brunswick, NJ, Rutgers Press, 1985, vol 5, pp 1-42. 2. Robins LN, et al: Lifetime prevalence of specific psychiatric disorders in sites. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984;41:949-958. 3. Cotton NS: The familial incidence of alco-holism: A review. J Stud Alcohol 1979;40:89-116. 4. Gurling HM: Genetic epidemiology in medi-cine p=m-recenttwin research. Br Med J 1984; 288:3-5. 5. Schuckit MA: Twin studies on substance abuse: An overview, in Gedda L, Parisi P, Nance W (eds): Twin Research: 3. Epidemiological and Clinical Studies. New York, Alan R Liss Inc, 1981. 6. Goodwin DW, Schulsinger F, Moller N, et al: Drinking problems in adopted and non-adopted sons of alcoholics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1974;31:164-169. 7. Bohman M, Sigvardsson S, Cloninger R: Material inheritance of alcohol abuse. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981;38:965-969. 8. Schuckit MA: Studies of populations at high risk for alcoholism. Psychiatr Dev 1985;3:31-63. 9. Begleiter H, Porjesz B, Bihari B, et al: Event-related brain potentials in boys at risk for alcoholism. Science 1984;225:1493-1495. 10. Schaeffer KW, Parsons OQ, Yohman JR: Neuropsychological differences between male familial and nonfamilial alcoholics and nonalco-holics. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res 1984;8:347-358. 11. Hedegus AM, Alterman AI, Tarter RE: Learning achievement in sons of alcoholics. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res 1984;8:330-333. 12. Lipscomb TR, Nathan PE: Blood alcohol level discrimination: The effects of family his-tory and alcoholism, drinking pattern, and toler-ance. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980;37:571-576. 13. Schuckit MA: Subjective responses to alco-hol in sons of alcoholics and controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984;41:879-884. 14. O'Malley SS, Maisto SA: The effects of family drinking history on responses to alcohol: Expectancies and reactions to intoxication. J Stud Alcohol 1985;46:289-297. 15. Mednick SA: Subjects at risk for alcohol-ism: Recent reports. Presented at the 14th Annual Medical Conference Scientific Confer-ence of the National Alcoholism Forum (Re-search Society on Alcoholism), Houston, April 4, 1983. 16. Schuckit MA: Ethanol-induced changes in body sway in men at high alcoholism risk. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1985;42:375-379. 17. Schuckit MA, Parker DC, Rossman LR: Ethanol-related prolactin responses and risk for alcoholism. Biol Psychiatry 1983;18:1153-1159. 18. Schuckit MA: Differences in plasma corti-sol after ingestion of ethanol in relatives of alcoholics and controls: Preliminary results. J Clin Psychiatry 1984;45:374-376. 19. Propping P, Kruger J, Mark N: Genetic disposition to alcoholism: An EEG study in alcoholics and their relatives. Hum Genet 1981; 59:51-59. 20. Pollock VE, Volavka J, Goodwin DW, et al: The EEG after alcohol administration in men at risk for alcoholism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40:857-861. 21. Schuckit MA, Engstrom D, Alpert R, et al: Differences in muscle-tension response to etha-nol in young men with and without family histories of alcoholism. J Stud Alcohol 1981; 42:918-924. 22. Schuckit MA: Peak blood alcohol levels in men at high risk for the future development of alcoholism. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res 1981;5:64x=req- 66. 23. Eriksson CJ: Human blood acetaldehyde concentration during ethanol oxidation (update, 1982). Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1983;18:141x=req- 150. 24. Behar D, Berg CJ, Rapoport JL: Behavioral and physiological effects of ethanol in high-risk and control children: A pilot study. Alcoholism Clin Exp Res 1983;7:404-410. 25. Schuckit MA, Rayses V: Ethanol ingestion: Differences in blood acetaldehyde concentra-tions in relatives of alcoholics and controls. Science 1979;203:54-55. 26. Zeiner AR, Krug R, Kegg P, et al: Cardio-vascular and pharmacokinetic effects of ethanol in offspring of alcoholics and social drinkers. Presented at the 20th International Congress of Applied Psychology, Edinburgh, Scotland, July 12, 1982. 27. Ward K, Weir DG, McCrodden JM, et al: Blood acetaldehyde levels in relatives of alcohol-ics following ethanol ingestion. ICRS Med Sci 1983;11:950. 28. Saunders GR, Schuckit MA: Brief commu-nication: MMPI scores in young men with alco-holic relatives and controls. J Nerv Ment Dis 1981;168:456-458. 29. Schuckit MA: Anxiety and assertiveness in the relatives of alcoholics and controls. J Clin Psychiatry 1982;43:238-239. 30. Schuckit MA: Extroversion and neuroti-cism in young men. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140:1223-1224. 31. Morrison C, Schuckit MA: Locus of control in young men with alcoholic relatives and con-trols. J Clin Psychiatry 1983;44:306-307. 32. MacAndrew C: On the possibility of the psychometric detection of persons who are prone to the abuse of alcohol and other substances. Addict Behav 1979;4:11-20. 33. Goodwin DW, Schulsinger F, Hermansen L, et al: Alcoholism and the hyperactive child syndrome. J Nerv Ment Dis 1975;160:349-353. 34. Vaillant GE, Milofsky ES: The etiology of alcoholism: A prospective viewpoint. Am Psychol 1982;37:494-502. 35. Schuckit MA: Genetic and biochemical factors in the etiology of alcoholism, in Grin-spoon L (ed): Psychiatry Update. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press Inc, 1984, vol 3, pp 320-327. 36. Schuckit MA: Drug and Alcohol Abuse: A Clinical Guide to Diagnosis and Treatment. New York, Plenum Publishing Corp, 1984. 37. Schuckit MA: Alcoholism and other psy-chiatric disorders. Hosp Community Psychiatry 1983;34:1022-1027. Downloaded from jama.ama-assn.org at Mt Sinai School Of Medicine on April 23, 2012