SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 10
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
1
Crazy Futures: Why Plausibility is Maladaptive
A thought essay for the Bucharest ‘Crazy Futures’ Workshop
27 June – 1 July 2011
Wendy Schultz | DRAFT: syntax may be bumpy/turbulent.
One person's craziness is another person's reality.
Tim Burton
We are gathered together to discuss crazy futures – and possibly crazy
futurists as well. This paper is my personal attempt to prepare my mind for
that discussion. It addresses the need for ‘crazy futures’ by highlighting the
relationship between futures thinking – specifically, the imaginative
production of images of the future – and our understanding of both
complexity and chaos. The explorations below consider:
1. The meanings of both components of the term –
a) First, ‘crazy’ as contrasted with ‘normal’ generally, and then
specifically with ‘plausibility’ as a term of art over-used in futures
and foresight practice, especially scenario planning; and
b) Next, ‘futures’, ie, images of non-existent, forward temporally
displaced situations / contexts and their generation, especially as
contributing to perceptions of ‘craziness’.
2. Basic notions of complexity and chaos, and what they imply for the
usefulness of crazy futures in contrast to plausible futures; and
3. Communicating usefully crazy futures.
Discussing crazy futurists is probably not worth investing time, given that the
greater percentage of any given population already perceives the term to
be redundant.
What is ‘crazy’?
* Crazy * music, dudes and dudettes! …he’s * crazy * about his dog / fishing /
iPad… You want to go bungee-jumping? Are you * crazy *?
The first known use of ‘crazy’ was in 1566, according to Merriam-Webster (I’ll
replace that with the history as documented by the OED as soon as I’m home
again and have OED online access). It originally meant “full of cracks or
flaws” – ie, like the glaze on a pot can be ‘crazed’ with cracks. The meaning
“of unsound mind, or behaving as so,” emerged later, in the early 1600s. The
jazz slang sense of crazy as ‘cool’ or ‘exciting’ sprang up in the late 1920s
(clearly what we mean when we call ourselves ‘crazy futurists’). Nowadays it
often means simply out of the ordinary, unusual – or impractical.
So we need to think about two aspects of the term: 1) being flawed,
unsound, or broken mentally; and 2) being unusual, and out of the ordinary.
In the last 400 years, both physiological and psychological research have
resulted in significant progress in our understanding the full range of illnesses
and syndromes that contribute to a broad range of mental perspectives and
2
resulting behaviours that observers might label ‘crazy.’ Those illnesses cause
serious pain to both sufferers and their loved ones, and this discussion in no
way is meant to downplay that.
But judging behaviour as ‘crazy’ is subjectively relative. When I was a child, if
I were walking down the street and the person coming towards me was
talking to herself out loud, I would very likely cross the street to avoid her.
Now we are all surrounded by crowds of people ‘talking to themselves’ – and
no longer consider it ‘crazy’ because it is contextually appropriate in an era
of mobile phone earpieces. It is neither unusual behaviour, nor out of the
ordinary, given a specific technological setting. The same applies, of course,
to different cultural settings: flooding a bathroom by using the shower hose
outside of the stall is ‘crazy’ behaviour in the USA – but perfectly rational in
Japan, where the bathing room has a drain in the floor, and one is expected
to be clean * before * entering the bath.
So if we define ‘crazy’ by contrast with ‘normal’, then it is the unusual as
contrasted against the usual. ‘Crazy’ is subjectively relative to internal
expectations filtered and biased by milieu, culture, and technological setting,
among other things. That is precisely its utility to futures thinking. ‘Crazy’ –
and the sense of nervous apprehension it engenders in viewers – highlights
and problematises the assumptions and points of view that compose the
normal. If the various futures we face are composed of surprises, of novelty –
of the abnormal – then crazy is just what we need: it exposes our blind spots,
the dangerous limitations of our assumptions.
But a more specific new antonym to ‘crazy’ has emerged in futures practice
in the last few decades. Rather than opposing crazy with ‘normal,’ it equates
‘crazy’ with ‘impossible’, and opposes it with ‘plausible.’
You keep using that word. I do not think it means what you think it means.
Inigo Montoya to Vizzini, in The Princess Bride
In the early 80’s Roy Amara gave us a classic conceptualization of the set of
all images of the future as roughly divisible into possible, probable, and
preferred. This was useful because it was robust:
• possible was the set of everything – every future possible to imagine,
whether or not they had already been imagined;
• probable was monitorable, if not measurable – researchers could observe
emerging issues growing in momentum, becoming trends, evolving into
greater probability; and
• preferable was articulable – researchers could engage stakeholders in
value discussions and judgements and essentially map the value territory.
In Venn diagram terms, the categories overlap, but are still useful as a
conceptual base for futures research methods.
Yet somehow over the intervening decades, the terms have morphed to
‘possible, plausible, probable, and preferable futures’. Sometimes the wide
and woolly set of ‘possible’ drops entirely from the field of view, and only
‘probable, plausible, and preferable’ futures remain. ‘Plausibility’ has
emerged as a primary operating assumption, even a criterion for excellence,
3
within English-speaking scenario practice (especially within the community of
‘scenario planners’).
Here is a partial inventory of the evidence:
• FOR-LEARN: “To be effective, scenarios must be plausible, consistent and
offer insights into the future. Plausibility: A scenario must be plausible. This
means that it must fall within the limits of what might conceivably
happen.”
• From Thinking About the Future: Guidelines for Strategic Foresight,
“…plausible futures: reasonable outcomes, with a discernable pathway
from the present to the future. For example, discovering extraterrestrial life
within the next decade is possible, but not plausible.”
• From Godet, Creating Futures: “…scenarios are only credible and useful if
they meet the following five conditions that we believe instill rigor:
relevance, coherence, plausibility, importance and transparency.”
• From Lindgren and Bandhold, Scenario Planning: “Scenarios are vivid
descriptions of plausible futures.”
• From Ralston and Wilson, The Scenario Planning Handbook: “…scenarios
must meet the following criteria: they must be plausible – that is, they must
fall within the limits of what might reasonably be expected to happen.”
• From Sharpe and van der Heijden, eds., Scenarios for Success: “A
scenario is a self-consistent account of one plausible way in which
uncertain future events may play out with a bearing on the future of an
organization and its ability to fulfil its purpose.”
• From Delft University of Technology’s report for the European Community,
Roadmapping eGovernment RTD 2020: “Scenarios are internally
consistent, mutually different and plausible stories about a future.”
• From Oliver Freeman of the Neville Freeman Agency, “Scenario planning is
a metaphor-rich narrative designed to help you consider alternative,
plausible futures.” [NB: but apparently only two of them: “One consistent
feature though is the development of two scenarios.”!!!]
• From x, y, and z, “A Review of Scenario Planning Literature” in Futures
Research Quarterly, “Selecting a scenario space means examining the
various future states the drivers could produce. Illogical and non-plausible
situations should be rejected. Selecting alternative worlds to be detailed
involves limiting the number of future stories, since it would be impossible
to explore every option. The key is to select plausible futures that will
challenge current thinking.”
• From Paul J. H. Shoemaker, “When and How to Use Scenario Planning,”
Journal of Forecasting: “…the scenarios should bound the range of
plausible uncertainties and challenge managerial thinking.”
• From [too many people to list on one page], “A formal framework for
scenario development in support of environmental decision-making,” in
Environmental Modelling & Software, “Scenarios are possible future states
of the world that represent alternative plausible conditions under different
assumptions.”
Let me emphasise the first two of these quotes, because they clarify the
matter by offering a definition, and an example, of plausibility. FOR-LEARN
suggests that a plausible scenario “must fall within the limits of what might
conceivably happen.” The authors of Thinking About the Future suggest that
4
plausible futures offer “…reasonable outcomes, with a discernable pathway
from the present to the future.” They further clarify with an example: “For
example, discovering extraterrestrial life within the next decade is possible,
but not plausible.” The difficulty with both of these lies in the subjective
capability and state of knowledge of the viewer: the more knowledgeable
the viewer on the topic of the scenario, or its component details, the more
events and futures they are capable of conceiving could happen. In the
example given of discovering extraterrestrial life, the scenario is possible, but
perhaps of low probability. But it is in fact plausible, because discernible
pathways exist not only to the evolution of extraterrestrial life, but also to our
discovery of it (given the various robotic surveys of other planets we have
launched recently).
Defining ‘plausibility’ is problematic. This limits its usefulness as a criterion for
excellence in futures thinking, even assuming that it is an appropriate criterion
for excellence. So let’s hit the dictionaries once again: what is the technical
definition of ‘plausible,’ and what is its etymology?
The Merriam-Webster Dictionary tells us that plausible means
“superficially fair, reasonable, or valuable but often
specious <a plausible pretext>; superficially pleasing or persuasive <a
swindler… , then a quack, then a smooth, plausible gentleman — R. W.
Emerson>; appearing worthy of belief <the argument was both
powerful and plausible>.”
Embedded within the structure of this word is the professional vulnerability
that all futures researchers face in practicing an intellectual discipline for
which there are no future facts, in a world of decision-makers hungry for an
evidence base: how to seem valuable when we are suspected of purveying
specious results and being quacks. The OED entry is even more telling, in
offering us the older, now obsolete uses of the word, all of which revolve
around pleasing the public and thereby winning approval.
Oxford English Dictionary: Acceptable, agreeable, pleasing, gratifying;
winning public approval, popular. Obs.; Expressing applause or
approbation; plausive, applausive. Obs. ; Deserving of applause or
approval; praiseworthy, laudable, commendable. Obs.; Of an
argument, an idea, a statement, etc.: seeming reasonable, probable,
or truthful; convincing, believable; (formerly) spec. having a false
appearance of reason or veracity; specious. Of a person: convincing
or persuasive, esp. with the intention to deceive.
The OED then clarifies current uses, suggesting that plausible ideas seem
reasonable or probable – while pointing out that it formerly implied such an
appearance of reason was false. Furthermore, when applied to a person, it
* still * implies an intention to deceive cloaked in false persuasion.
My apologies for dwelling on these historical facts of etymological evolution
at such length. But my observations of how consultants use the label
‘plausible scenarios’, or ‘plausible futures’, suggest that it is actually code for
“…don’t give the clients crazy futures, or they’ll reject them, reject us, and we
5
won’t get paid and will never work in this town again.” How often in strategic
foresight projects do the end results offer truly transformational futures that
challenge participants to consider the possibilities of deep structural change?
Of worlds with entirely different economic or political systems? Of usefully
crazy futures?
What are ‘futures’ – and why and how do we think about them?
While scenario thinking per se originated in Herman Kahn’s policy strategy
concerns, the origin of futures thinking is rooted in the image of the future,
and Fred Polak’s concern for the vitality of human culture and civilizations.
We shouldn’t limit the ‘futures’ in ‘crazy futures’ to strategic scenarios alone.
Such purpose-designed images of the future compete for mental and
emotional space with a nearly endless supply of images of the future
generated across human activity. Imagining long-range futures is a talent
unique to our species – so what images do we create, how do we create
them, and what are they for?
Futures studies as an intellectual endeavour includes the inventory and
content analysis of existing images of the future. Humans express imagined
futures in all media, as all variety of cultural constructs, and in varying scales
of personal and civilizational usefulness. Images of the future in ads, in
counselling programs, and in daydreams target personal behaviour by
expressing self-fulfilling or self-defeating prophecies. In the same way,
community, organizational, and political futures attempt to inspire group
action through both cautionary tales (“doom and gloom”; nightmare
futures), and through aspirational tomorrows (visions). Images of the future
are embedded in all political discourses and ideologies, whether they warn of
imminent national collapse at the hands of the opposition (nightmare
futures), call for ‘Holding the course! Steady on!’ (present-trends-extended
6
futures), or depict a happy era to come (Conservatives: A return to the
Golden Age! Liberals: An All-New Brighter Tomorrow!). At the largest scale,
Polak’s call for aspirational, transformative images of the future was meant to
catalyze civilization-level vitality.
But categorizing any specific image of the future as a nightmare or a vision is
entirely subjective. Furthermore, it is a subjective judgment that can generate
tragedies. Conflicts arise when people fear a nightmare future is the hidden
goal of others around them. And any extreme of difference is labelled
‘crazy,’ whether it is extremely good, extremely bad, or extremely different.
We could probably conceptualize a craziness scale for futures, anchored at
sanity/normality/plausibility on the one end, and ‘completely bug@%$@
crazy’ at the other extreme. It might be operationalized as the percentage
of any given population that perceives a specific image of the future as
offensively, scarily transgressive and transformative beyond all bounds of
reason and decency. If nobody feels the future is beyond all bounds, it’s
normal and plausible as normal can be. If 50% of the people feel it’s beyond
all bounds of reason and decency, and the other 50% do not, then it’s only
moderately crazy. And so on.
It is not just the image itself that is judged. ‘Crazy futures’ often earn that
label despite being prosaic and mundane in content, if they have
transgressed in process. The current decision-making environment for many
economic, political, and social issues is instrumentalist, evidence-based and
biased towards Western empiricism. Sadly, a field that engages in research
despite lacking an ability to observe its subject directly (until such time as
tachyon-powered time machines or traversable wormholes enable field
research in the futures) often lacks credibility as well. In a previous essay, I
summarised this ‘cultural contradiction’ between the criteria for excellence in
empirical, evidence-based research, and for excellence in futures research
(specifically horizon scanning), as follows:
Empirical/
Evidence-based Research
Futures Research,
especially emerging issues scanning & analysis
• Credible;
• Documented;
• Authoritative;
• Statistically
significant;
• Coherent: the data
agree;
• Consensus-based:
the experts agree;
• Any emerging issue unusual enough to be useful
will probably lack apparent credibility;
• it will be difficult to document, as only one or
two cases of the change may yet exist;
• it will emerge from marginalized populations,
and be noticed initially by fringe sources, hardly
the sort of authoritative sources that civil
servants feel confident in citing;
• as emerging issues are by definition only one or
two cases, they are also by definition statistically
insignificant;
• the data will vary widely, converging over time
only if the emerging issue matures into a trend;
• not only will consensus be lacking, but experts
will often violently attack reports of emerging
issues of change, as they represent challenges
to current paradigms and structures of expertise,
7
• Theoretically
grounded; and
• Mono-disciplinary.
power, and entitlement;
• emerging issues of change often challenge
previous theoretical structures and necessitate
the construction of new theories;
• and the most interesting new change emerges
where disciplines converge and clash. As the
impacts ripple out across all the systems of
reality, emerging changes and their impacts
require a multi-disciplinary analytic perspective.
Bishop, Hines, and Collins have inventoried formal futures methods for
generating images of the future – both scenarios (extrapolations) and visions
(value-based preferred future articulations) – and described almost two
dozen rigorously structured processes. These range from logical and
quantitative methods using statistical trend extrapolation, computer-aided
cross-impact matrices, and systems dynamics modelling, through facilitated
group process dialogue, to meditative, ‘guided visualization’ techniques.
Evidence-based decision support cultures prefer their futures heavily salted
with data and quantitative extrapolation. They are likely to shy away from
guided visualization workshops. So decision-makers, observers – and
practitioners – also judge some means of generating futures as ‘crazy’.
Generally, the more intuitive methods are greeted with the most scepticism
and distrust.
Scenario planners and scenario builders are not alone in devising images of
the future. Artists, advertisers, novelists, screenwriters, animators, sculptors,
analysts, and leaders all generate stories, images, and artifacts expressing
different future outcomes and environments. So do prophets, astrologers,
tea-leaf readers, shamans, and particularly skilled remote viewers. By
extension, if rigorous but intuitive tools such as guided visualization earn
scepticism, artistic inspiration may as well – and astrologers, shamans, and
remote viewers earn outright derision.
Why shouldn’t they? Why shouldn’t we just discard images of the future
generated by ‘crazy’ methods such as astrological computations and
shamanic trances and remote viewing? To answer that we must return to the
conceptual foundations and core assumptions of futures studies as a field of
research, which include:
• There is not one single future, but multiple alternative futures;
• People’s beliefs about the future, and their images of the future, affect
their decisions and actions, which in turn create the futures as an
emergent property of aggregated interconnected actions;
• Because any given lived future at any given moment is an emergent
property of a complex system that frequently exhibits chaotic
behavior, it is not possible to ‘predict’ human futures.
Based on these assumptions, we can see that it is not important which future
is correct, or which future is best supported by empirically credible data, or
which future is most plausible. The most important future is the future the
greatest number of people believe the most: it is the future on which they are
basing their decisions and actions. And therefore it is absolutely irrelevant
how that future was generated, and how credible its empirical underpinnings
8
are. If people read astrological forecasts or tea leaves or goat entrails, and
then act on those images of the future, then those images of the future are
important for us as futures researchers to consider. The craziest methods can
generate the most compelling futures, and crazy or not, those are the futures
with which we should be most concerned.
Why are crazy futures the most useful?
We all agree that your theory is crazy, but is it crazy enough?
Niels Bohr
Touching on the base assumptions of
futures studies leads us directly to why
crazy futures are the most useful: we are
embedded in – and are ourselves –
complex systems flirting daily with chaos.
Chris Langton’s famous diagram, left,
illustrates this concisely. There are fixed
systems, existing in a state of maximum
thermodynamic equilibrium, meaning
maximum entropy – or death. There are
periodic systems, which are ordered but not adaptive [expand]. There are
chaotic systems, characterized by sensitive dependence on initial conditions,
and non-linear determinism.
Finally, there are complex systems that are self-organizing, self-directing, self-
repairing, and adaptive [check for complete list of characteristics]. Their self-
directing and adaptive characteristics result in evolutionary change over
time, producing in turn novel emergent properties. They generate surprises. If
sentient, they undoubtedly surprise themselves. Emergent properties are ‘out
of the ordinary’, if by ordinary we mean the previous patterns of ordered
system behaviour. So any complex adaptive system (and all human systems
– whether single individuals or collections as organizations, or communities, or
nation-states – are complex adaptive/evolving systems) will at one point or
another generate ‘crazy states’ (apologies to Dror).
It becomes even more likely that these systems will exhibit ‘crazy’, ie, ‘out of
the ordinary’ behaviour if they are stressed by larger energy or information
flows. One response to stress in a complex system is a phase change into
chaotic behaviour. This phase change also creates the potential for novel
and unusual behaviour outside the ordinary.
So in the end, a focus on ‘crazy futures’ may be the most adaptive strategy
we can encourage people to adopt, and a focus on ‘plausibility’ the most
maladaptive. Is your future crazy enough to help you / your organization /
your community evolve? Better that we rehearse the full range of surprises
that may await us across our futures, than be ill-prepared and unable to
adapt. Emergence and evolution are preferable to equilibrium.
9
How can we best communicate craziness?
Before bowing out, there’s time for a brief thought on how we communicate
compelling craziness. Animators have recently identified an interesting
perceptual space they call ‘the uncanny valley.’ The uncanny valley
hypothesis suggests that when human ‘replicas’ – either animated or robotic
– look and act almost, but not perfectly, human, people response with
revulsion. Granted, James Cameron seems to have overcome the effect in
generating the Navi characters over the motion capture performance of his
actors. The relevance to useful crazy futures is that something similar exists
in conveying radically transgressive, transformative images of possible futures:
up until a point, increasing craziness increases how exciting, provocative, and
challenging they are. Beyond that point, increasing craziness pushes the
futures into the uncanny valley of the unthinkable, on the other side of which
is the transgression of perfect conceptual chaos. Whatever their degree of
craziness, useful futures are compelling – people respond to them, adopt
them, and use them to inform action.
So how do we decrease the uncanny valley of the unthinkable – how do we
avoid Cassandra syndrome? The endeavour of deploying crazy futures asks
us to balance on a knife edge of usability: too normal, and no mind-shift
results; too crazy, and brain-freeze occurs. Likewise, futures too divorced
from our own experience may feel very crazy, but not be very compelling;
too near to our own experience, and the futures will be too subjective to be
useful – compelling, but insufficiently out of the ordinary.
A possible antidote can be found in audience participation. Cutting edge
methods in scenario building include projects like Jane McGonigal’s
SuperStruct, and Evoke, and the growing body of work using the Institute for
10
the Future’s “Foresight Engine”. These projects all rely on massively crowd-
sourced, participatory futures formation via on-line game environments. They
evolve from each individual’s own participation, which is very compelling –
but they evolve. The futures generated are emergent properties of the
participants’ interactions with each other, and the useful strangeness arises
from those interactions. In The Art of Immersion, Frank Rose offers a range of
case studies underlining how powerfully engaging the unfinished story can
be. This is the Web 2.0 corollary of McLuhan’s ‘media hot and cool’: the
most compelling media are the ‘cool’ media, conveying ideas in low
definition and inviting us to participate in completing the details. So
whatever crazy futures we imagine, we should imagine them with holes, with
interstitial spaces that invite other people to adapt them and adopt them: a
crazy future must be compelling to be useful.
Coda
Alice laughed. "There's no use trying," she said. "One can't believe impossible
things."
"I dare say you haven't had much practice," said the queen. "When I was your
age, I always did it for half an hour a day. Why, sometimes I've believed as
many as six impossible things before breakfast."
Lewis Carroll / Charles Dodgson, Alice in Wonderland
I want to believe.
Fox Mulder’s wall poster, The X-Files
In order to thrive in whatever futures we pass through, it helps to rehearse
what our values, assumptions, decisions and actions -- our very sense of self –
might be in those futures. Authentic rehearsal inevitably requires that at some
level we choose to believe not only what is plausible, and not just what is
probable or possible, but that we stretch our values, assumptions, and sense
of self to believe and rehearse for the impossible as well.
So call me crazy.
You have to go on and be crazy. Craziness is like heaven.
Jimi Hendrix

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

Jacques Fresco_Designing the Future
Jacques Fresco_Designing the FutureJacques Fresco_Designing the Future
Jacques Fresco_Designing the Future
bueno buono good
 
What lies over the horizon? Scenarios to the Future of Social Sciences in the...
What lies over the horizon? Scenarios to the Future of Social Sciences in the...What lies over the horizon? Scenarios to the Future of Social Sciences in the...
What lies over the horizon? Scenarios to the Future of Social Sciences in the...
Shermon Cruz
 
2007 ten year-forecast
2007 ten year-forecast2007 ten year-forecast
2007 ten year-forecast
atelier t*h
 

Was ist angesagt? (11)

Mind compass.b copy
Mind compass.b copyMind compass.b copy
Mind compass.b copy
 
Prosthetic lecture.a copy 5
Prosthetic lecture.a copy 5Prosthetic lecture.a copy 5
Prosthetic lecture.a copy 5
 
Shared Joy: a problem-to-vision interactive exercise.
Shared Joy: a problem-to-vision interactive exercise.Shared Joy: a problem-to-vision interactive exercise.
Shared Joy: a problem-to-vision interactive exercise.
 
Career 3.0
Career 3.0Career 3.0
Career 3.0
 
Jacques Fresco_Designing the Future
Jacques Fresco_Designing the FutureJacques Fresco_Designing the Future
Jacques Fresco_Designing the Future
 
Jacque fresco2
Jacque fresco2Jacque fresco2
Jacque fresco2
 
Science Fiction and Data
Science Fiction and DataScience Fiction and Data
Science Fiction and Data
 
Searching for the Second foundation
Searching for the Second foundationSearching for the Second foundation
Searching for the Second foundation
 
E=mc2®
E=mc2®E=mc2®
E=mc2®
 
What lies over the horizon? Scenarios to the Future of Social Sciences in the...
What lies over the horizon? Scenarios to the Future of Social Sciences in the...What lies over the horizon? Scenarios to the Future of Social Sciences in the...
What lies over the horizon? Scenarios to the Future of Social Sciences in the...
 
2007 ten year-forecast
2007 ten year-forecast2007 ten year-forecast
2007 ten year-forecast
 

Andere mochten auch

2009 - Publicité interactive
2009 - Publicité interactive2009 - Publicité interactive
2009 - Publicité interactive
Richard91
 
Designing Governance for Native Hawaiians
Designing Governance for Native HawaiiansDesigning Governance for Native Hawaiians
Designing Governance for Native Hawaiians
Richard Lum
 
An Introduction to "Verge"
An Introduction to "Verge"An Introduction to "Verge"
An Introduction to "Verge"
Richard Lum
 
Summary of VERGE (ethnographic futures framework devised by Richard Lum and M...
Summary of VERGE (ethnographic futures framework devised by Richard Lum and M...Summary of VERGE (ethnographic futures framework devised by Richard Lum and M...
Summary of VERGE (ethnographic futures framework devised by Richard Lum and M...
Wendy Schultz
 

Andere mochten auch (20)

Forrester EA Council Roundtable 29/4/10 (Short)
Forrester EA Council Roundtable 29/4/10 (Short)Forrester EA Council Roundtable 29/4/10 (Short)
Forrester EA Council Roundtable 29/4/10 (Short)
 
Verge Explore Board
Verge Explore BoardVerge Explore Board
Verge Explore Board
 
Theory-Driven Scenario Development
Theory-Driven Scenario DevelopmentTheory-Driven Scenario Development
Theory-Driven Scenario Development
 
2011 autumn e business 1
2011 autumn e business 12011 autumn e business 1
2011 autumn e business 1
 
2011 autumn e business models 2
2011 autumn e business models 22011 autumn e business models 2
2011 autumn e business models 2
 
2009 - Publicité interactive
2009 - Publicité interactive2009 - Publicité interactive
2009 - Publicité interactive
 
Designing Governance for Native Hawaiians
Designing Governance for Native HawaiiansDesigning Governance for Native Hawaiians
Designing Governance for Native Hawaiians
 
Work Skills Occupations and Innovation 2010 Ser Inn Bman62052 4
Work Skills Occupations and Innovation 2010 Ser Inn Bman62052 4 Work Skills Occupations and Innovation 2010 Ser Inn Bman62052 4
Work Skills Occupations and Innovation 2010 Ser Inn Bman62052 4
 
The Third Era (highlight booklet)
The Third Era (highlight booklet)The Third Era (highlight booklet)
The Third Era (highlight booklet)
 
An Introduction to "Verge"
An Introduction to "Verge"An Introduction to "Verge"
An Introduction to "Verge"
 
The Infinite Economy (highlight booklet)
The Infinite Economy (highlight booklet)The Infinite Economy (highlight booklet)
The Infinite Economy (highlight booklet)
 
Forrester EA Council Roundtable 29/4/10 Futures Toolkit (Short)
Forrester EA Council Roundtable 29/4/10 Futures Toolkit (Short)Forrester EA Council Roundtable 29/4/10 Futures Toolkit (Short)
Forrester EA Council Roundtable 29/4/10 Futures Toolkit (Short)
 
Dicey 2016 Dinner Lecture Wendy Schultz revised
Dicey 2016 Dinner Lecture Wendy Schultz revisedDicey 2016 Dinner Lecture Wendy Schultz revised
Dicey 2016 Dinner Lecture Wendy Schultz revised
 
Grand challenges and uk foresight
Grand challenges and uk foresightGrand challenges and uk foresight
Grand challenges and uk foresight
 
Open Innovation and Intellectual Property
Open Innovation and Intellectual PropertyOpen Innovation and Intellectual Property
Open Innovation and Intellectual Property
 
Applied Futures Research Overview, 2002
Applied Futures Research Overview, 2002Applied Futures Research Overview, 2002
Applied Futures Research Overview, 2002
 
Cultural Contradictions of Scanning in an Evidence-based Policy Environment
Cultural Contradictions of Scanning in an Evidence-based Policy EnvironmentCultural Contradictions of Scanning in an Evidence-based Policy Environment
Cultural Contradictions of Scanning in an Evidence-based Policy Environment
 
Designing Native Hawaiian Governance
Designing Native Hawaiian GovernanceDesigning Native Hawaiian Governance
Designing Native Hawaiian Governance
 
What's Next for the Future?
What's Next for the Future?What's Next for the Future?
What's Next for the Future?
 
Summary of VERGE (ethnographic futures framework devised by Richard Lum and M...
Summary of VERGE (ethnographic futures framework devised by Richard Lum and M...Summary of VERGE (ethnographic futures framework devised by Richard Lum and M...
Summary of VERGE (ethnographic futures framework devised by Richard Lum and M...
 

Ähnlich wie Crazy Futures I an exploration on the necessity of pushing your thinking past normal to explore potential futures

1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
SantosConleyha
 
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
BenitoSumpter862
 
How To Write Essay Papers.pdf
How To Write Essay Papers.pdfHow To Write Essay Papers.pdf
How To Write Essay Papers.pdf
Monica Hawkins
 
The Future of Humanity  Nick Bostrom Future of Humanit.docx
The Future of Humanity  Nick Bostrom Future of Humanit.docxThe Future of Humanity  Nick Bostrom Future of Humanit.docx
The Future of Humanity  Nick Bostrom Future of Humanit.docx
barbaran11
 

Ähnlich wie Crazy Futures I an exploration on the necessity of pushing your thinking past normal to explore potential futures (20)

Crazy Futures I an exploration on the necessity of pushing your thinking pas...
 Crazy Futures I an exploration on the necessity of pushing your thinking pas... Crazy Futures I an exploration on the necessity of pushing your thinking pas...
Crazy Futures I an exploration on the necessity of pushing your thinking pas...
 
Crazy Futures: Why Plausibility is Maladaptive
Crazy Futures: Why Plausibility is MaladaptiveCrazy Futures: Why Plausibility is Maladaptive
Crazy Futures: Why Plausibility is Maladaptive
 
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
 
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
1. Why is the relationship between Geertz and Weber, assuming that
 
Critiquing Qualitative Research Essay.pdf
Critiquing Qualitative Research Essay.pdfCritiquing Qualitative Research Essay.pdf
Critiquing Qualitative Research Essay.pdf
 
Essays On Transcendentalism. . Transcendentalism Study Guide
Essays On Transcendentalism. . Transcendentalism Study GuideEssays On Transcendentalism. . Transcendentalism Study Guide
Essays On Transcendentalism. . Transcendentalism Study Guide
 
Personal Legend Essay. Legend Essay 2 .docx - RUNNING HEADER: ESSAY ON LEGEND...
Personal Legend Essay. Legend Essay 2 .docx - RUNNING HEADER: ESSAY ON LEGEND...Personal Legend Essay. Legend Essay 2 .docx - RUNNING HEADER: ESSAY ON LEGEND...
Personal Legend Essay. Legend Essay 2 .docx - RUNNING HEADER: ESSAY ON LEGEND...
 
Descriptive Essay Describing A Person
Descriptive Essay Describing A PersonDescriptive Essay Describing A Person
Descriptive Essay Describing A Person
 
Students Life Essay. Student essays: Essay about school life
Students Life Essay. Student essays: Essay about school lifeStudents Life Essay. Student essays: Essay about school life
Students Life Essay. Student essays: Essay about school life
 
Essay For Night By Elie Wiesel
Essay For Night By Elie WieselEssay For Night By Elie Wiesel
Essay For Night By Elie Wiesel
 
Compare Contrast Essay Topics. 018 Comparison Contrast Essay Topics Quiz Work...
Compare Contrast Essay Topics. 018 Comparison Contrast Essay Topics Quiz Work...Compare Contrast Essay Topics. 018 Comparison Contrast Essay Topics Quiz Work...
Compare Contrast Essay Topics. 018 Comparison Contrast Essay Topics Quiz Work...
 
Doust
DoustDoust
Doust
 
Starry Night Essay
Starry Night EssayStarry Night Essay
Starry Night Essay
 
A Reflective Essay On Personal Experiences.pdf
A Reflective Essay On Personal Experiences.pdfA Reflective Essay On Personal Experiences.pdf
A Reflective Essay On Personal Experiences.pdf
 
How To Write Essay Papers.pdf
How To Write Essay Papers.pdfHow To Write Essay Papers.pdf
How To Write Essay Papers.pdf
 
Use Your Words: Content Strategy to Influence Behavior
Use Your Words: Content Strategy to Influence BehaviorUse Your Words: Content Strategy to Influence Behavior
Use Your Words: Content Strategy to Influence Behavior
 
The Future of Humanity  Nick Bostrom Future of Humanit.docx
The Future of Humanity  Nick Bostrom Future of Humanit.docxThe Future of Humanity  Nick Bostrom Future of Humanit.docx
The Future of Humanity  Nick Bostrom Future of Humanit.docx
 
Truth Essay.pdf
Truth Essay.pdfTruth Essay.pdf
Truth Essay.pdf
 
Mkt421 Week 4 Perceptive Maps
Mkt421 Week 4 Perceptive MapsMkt421 Week 4 Perceptive Maps
Mkt421 Week 4 Perceptive Maps
 
Comments from the Edge
Comments from the EdgeComments from the Edge
Comments from the Edge
 

Mehr von Wendy Schultz

Mehr von Wendy Schultz (20)

WHO Foresight Approaches in Public Health.pdf
WHO Foresight Approaches in Public Health.pdfWHO Foresight Approaches in Public Health.pdf
WHO Foresight Approaches in Public Health.pdf
 
Chaos Complexity LIminality for WFSF 2023.pptx
Chaos Complexity LIminality for WFSF 2023.pptxChaos Complexity LIminality for WFSF 2023.pptx
Chaos Complexity LIminality for WFSF 2023.pptx
 
Chaos Turtles Redux for WFSF 2021.pptx
Chaos Turtles Redux for WFSF 2021.pptxChaos Turtles Redux for WFSF 2021.pptx
Chaos Turtles Redux for WFSF 2021.pptx
 
Example futures conceptual framework prepared by Infinite Futures / Jigsaw Fo...
Example futures conceptual framework prepared by Infinite Futures / Jigsaw Fo...Example futures conceptual framework prepared by Infinite Futures / Jigsaw Fo...
Example futures conceptual framework prepared by Infinite Futures / Jigsaw Fo...
 
Scenario Archetypes - A Map of Basic Structures.
Scenario Archetypes - A Map of Basic Structures.Scenario Archetypes - A Map of Basic Structures.
Scenario Archetypes - A Map of Basic Structures.
 
"It's Chaos Turtles All the Way Down" - presentation for the Global Foresight...
"It's Chaos Turtles All the Way Down" - presentation for the Global Foresight..."It's Chaos Turtles All the Way Down" - presentation for the Global Foresight...
"It's Chaos Turtles All the Way Down" - presentation for the Global Foresight...
 
A brief history and description of visioning tools.
A brief history and description of visioning tools.A brief history and description of visioning tools.
A brief history and description of visioning tools.
 
The List: Ten Sparks for Thought on AI
The List: Ten Sparks for Thought on AIThe List: Ten Sparks for Thought on AI
The List: Ten Sparks for Thought on AI
 
Museum mash-up, or vectors of visioning
Museum mash-up, or vectors of visioningMuseum mash-up, or vectors of visioning
Museum mash-up, or vectors of visioning
 
Melding machine learning and participatory foresight
Melding machine learning and participatory foresightMelding machine learning and participatory foresight
Melding machine learning and participatory foresight
 
Tick TOCS Tick TOCS - channeling change through theory into scenarios
Tick TOCS Tick TOCS - channeling change through theory into scenariosTick TOCS Tick TOCS - channeling change through theory into scenarios
Tick TOCS Tick TOCS - channeling change through theory into scenarios
 
Philanthropy Transformed
Philanthropy TransformedPhilanthropy Transformed
Philanthropy Transformed
 
ORI BAM Warwick Scenarios 2018 Crowdsourcing Harman's Fan
ORI BAM Warwick Scenarios 2018 Crowdsourcing Harman's FanORI BAM Warwick Scenarios 2018 Crowdsourcing Harman's Fan
ORI BAM Warwick Scenarios 2018 Crowdsourcing Harman's Fan
 
Wendy Schultz In Celebration of Vision
Wendy Schultz In Celebration of VisionWendy Schultz In Celebration of Vision
Wendy Schultz In Celebration of Vision
 
Keen eyes, sharp wits, kind hearts: women in futures studies.
Keen eyes, sharp wits, kind hearts: women in futures studies.Keen eyes, sharp wits, kind hearts: women in futures studies.
Keen eyes, sharp wits, kind hearts: women in futures studies.
 
APF V Gathering 2017
APF V Gathering 2017APF V Gathering 2017
APF V Gathering 2017
 
Methods inventory jigsaw presentation
Methods inventory jigsaw presentationMethods inventory jigsaw presentation
Methods inventory jigsaw presentation
 
"Blowing the Cobwebs Off Your Mind" Bootcamp
"Blowing the Cobwebs Off Your Mind" Bootcamp"Blowing the Cobwebs Off Your Mind" Bootcamp
"Blowing the Cobwebs Off Your Mind" Bootcamp
 
CLA (Causal Layered Analysis) - brief introduction
CLA (Causal Layered Analysis) - brief introductionCLA (Causal Layered Analysis) - brief introduction
CLA (Causal Layered Analysis) - brief introduction
 
Collecting stories about future uses of blockchain technology
Collecting stories about future uses of blockchain technologyCollecting stories about future uses of blockchain technology
Collecting stories about future uses of blockchain technology
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

The Psychology Of Motivation - Richard Brown
The Psychology Of Motivation - Richard BrownThe Psychology Of Motivation - Richard Brown
The Psychology Of Motivation - Richard Brown
SandaliGurusinghe2
 
Abortion pills in Jeddah |• +966572737505 ] GET CYTOTEC
Abortion pills in Jeddah |• +966572737505 ] GET CYTOTECAbortion pills in Jeddah |• +966572737505 ] GET CYTOTEC
Abortion pills in Jeddah |• +966572737505 ] GET CYTOTEC
Abortion pills in Riyadh +966572737505 get cytotec
 
Agile Coaching Change Management Framework.pptx
Agile Coaching Change Management Framework.pptxAgile Coaching Change Management Framework.pptx
Agile Coaching Change Management Framework.pptx
alinstan901
 
Beyond the Codes_Repositioning towards sustainable development
Beyond the Codes_Repositioning towards sustainable developmentBeyond the Codes_Repositioning towards sustainable development
Beyond the Codes_Repositioning towards sustainable development
Nimot Muili
 
internship thesis pakistan aeronautical complex kamra
internship thesis pakistan aeronautical complex kamrainternship thesis pakistan aeronautical complex kamra
internship thesis pakistan aeronautical complex kamra
AllTops
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (17)

The Psychology Of Motivation - Richard Brown
The Psychology Of Motivation - Richard BrownThe Psychology Of Motivation - Richard Brown
The Psychology Of Motivation - Richard Brown
 
Abortion pills in Jeddah |• +966572737505 ] GET CYTOTEC
Abortion pills in Jeddah |• +966572737505 ] GET CYTOTECAbortion pills in Jeddah |• +966572737505 ] GET CYTOTEC
Abortion pills in Jeddah |• +966572737505 ] GET CYTOTEC
 
Agile Coaching Change Management Framework.pptx
Agile Coaching Change Management Framework.pptxAgile Coaching Change Management Framework.pptx
Agile Coaching Change Management Framework.pptx
 
Reviewing and summarization of university ranking system to.pptx
Reviewing and summarization of university ranking system  to.pptxReviewing and summarization of university ranking system  to.pptx
Reviewing and summarization of university ranking system to.pptx
 
International Ocean Transportation p.pdf
International Ocean Transportation p.pdfInternational Ocean Transportation p.pdf
International Ocean Transportation p.pdf
 
Dealing with Poor Performance - get the full picture from 3C Performance Mana...
Dealing with Poor Performance - get the full picture from 3C Performance Mana...Dealing with Poor Performance - get the full picture from 3C Performance Mana...
Dealing with Poor Performance - get the full picture from 3C Performance Mana...
 
Strategic Management, Vision Mission, Internal Analsysis
Strategic Management, Vision Mission, Internal AnalsysisStrategic Management, Vision Mission, Internal Analsysis
Strategic Management, Vision Mission, Internal Analsysis
 
How Software Developers Destroy Business Value.pptx
How Software Developers Destroy Business Value.pptxHow Software Developers Destroy Business Value.pptx
How Software Developers Destroy Business Value.pptx
 
W.H.Bender Quote 62 - Always strive to be a Hospitality Service professional
W.H.Bender Quote 62 - Always strive to be a Hospitality Service professionalW.H.Bender Quote 62 - Always strive to be a Hospitality Service professional
W.H.Bender Quote 62 - Always strive to be a Hospitality Service professional
 
Beyond the Codes_Repositioning towards sustainable development
Beyond the Codes_Repositioning towards sustainable developmentBeyond the Codes_Repositioning towards sustainable development
Beyond the Codes_Repositioning towards sustainable development
 
Leaders enhance communication by actively listening, providing constructive f...
Leaders enhance communication by actively listening, providing constructive f...Leaders enhance communication by actively listening, providing constructive f...
Leaders enhance communication by actively listening, providing constructive f...
 
digital Human resource management presentation.pdf
digital Human resource management presentation.pdfdigital Human resource management presentation.pdf
digital Human resource management presentation.pdf
 
internship thesis pakistan aeronautical complex kamra
internship thesis pakistan aeronautical complex kamrainternship thesis pakistan aeronautical complex kamra
internship thesis pakistan aeronautical complex kamra
 
Marketing Management 16th edition by Philip Kotler test bank.docx
Marketing Management 16th edition by Philip Kotler test bank.docxMarketing Management 16th edition by Philip Kotler test bank.docx
Marketing Management 16th edition by Philip Kotler test bank.docx
 
Safety T fire missions army field Artillery
Safety T fire missions army field ArtillerySafety T fire missions army field Artillery
Safety T fire missions army field Artillery
 
Intro_University_Ranking_Introduction.pptx
Intro_University_Ranking_Introduction.pptxIntro_University_Ranking_Introduction.pptx
Intro_University_Ranking_Introduction.pptx
 
Independent Escorts Vikaspuri / 9899900591 High Profile Escort Service in Delhi
Independent Escorts Vikaspuri  / 9899900591 High Profile Escort Service in DelhiIndependent Escorts Vikaspuri  / 9899900591 High Profile Escort Service in Delhi
Independent Escorts Vikaspuri / 9899900591 High Profile Escort Service in Delhi
 

Crazy Futures I an exploration on the necessity of pushing your thinking past normal to explore potential futures

  • 1. 1 Crazy Futures: Why Plausibility is Maladaptive A thought essay for the Bucharest ‘Crazy Futures’ Workshop 27 June – 1 July 2011 Wendy Schultz | DRAFT: syntax may be bumpy/turbulent. One person's craziness is another person's reality. Tim Burton We are gathered together to discuss crazy futures – and possibly crazy futurists as well. This paper is my personal attempt to prepare my mind for that discussion. It addresses the need for ‘crazy futures’ by highlighting the relationship between futures thinking – specifically, the imaginative production of images of the future – and our understanding of both complexity and chaos. The explorations below consider: 1. The meanings of both components of the term – a) First, ‘crazy’ as contrasted with ‘normal’ generally, and then specifically with ‘plausibility’ as a term of art over-used in futures and foresight practice, especially scenario planning; and b) Next, ‘futures’, ie, images of non-existent, forward temporally displaced situations / contexts and their generation, especially as contributing to perceptions of ‘craziness’. 2. Basic notions of complexity and chaos, and what they imply for the usefulness of crazy futures in contrast to plausible futures; and 3. Communicating usefully crazy futures. Discussing crazy futurists is probably not worth investing time, given that the greater percentage of any given population already perceives the term to be redundant. What is ‘crazy’? * Crazy * music, dudes and dudettes! …he’s * crazy * about his dog / fishing / iPad… You want to go bungee-jumping? Are you * crazy *? The first known use of ‘crazy’ was in 1566, according to Merriam-Webster (I’ll replace that with the history as documented by the OED as soon as I’m home again and have OED online access). It originally meant “full of cracks or flaws” – ie, like the glaze on a pot can be ‘crazed’ with cracks. The meaning “of unsound mind, or behaving as so,” emerged later, in the early 1600s. The jazz slang sense of crazy as ‘cool’ or ‘exciting’ sprang up in the late 1920s (clearly what we mean when we call ourselves ‘crazy futurists’). Nowadays it often means simply out of the ordinary, unusual – or impractical. So we need to think about two aspects of the term: 1) being flawed, unsound, or broken mentally; and 2) being unusual, and out of the ordinary. In the last 400 years, both physiological and psychological research have resulted in significant progress in our understanding the full range of illnesses and syndromes that contribute to a broad range of mental perspectives and
  • 2. 2 resulting behaviours that observers might label ‘crazy.’ Those illnesses cause serious pain to both sufferers and their loved ones, and this discussion in no way is meant to downplay that. But judging behaviour as ‘crazy’ is subjectively relative. When I was a child, if I were walking down the street and the person coming towards me was talking to herself out loud, I would very likely cross the street to avoid her. Now we are all surrounded by crowds of people ‘talking to themselves’ – and no longer consider it ‘crazy’ because it is contextually appropriate in an era of mobile phone earpieces. It is neither unusual behaviour, nor out of the ordinary, given a specific technological setting. The same applies, of course, to different cultural settings: flooding a bathroom by using the shower hose outside of the stall is ‘crazy’ behaviour in the USA – but perfectly rational in Japan, where the bathing room has a drain in the floor, and one is expected to be clean * before * entering the bath. So if we define ‘crazy’ by contrast with ‘normal’, then it is the unusual as contrasted against the usual. ‘Crazy’ is subjectively relative to internal expectations filtered and biased by milieu, culture, and technological setting, among other things. That is precisely its utility to futures thinking. ‘Crazy’ – and the sense of nervous apprehension it engenders in viewers – highlights and problematises the assumptions and points of view that compose the normal. If the various futures we face are composed of surprises, of novelty – of the abnormal – then crazy is just what we need: it exposes our blind spots, the dangerous limitations of our assumptions. But a more specific new antonym to ‘crazy’ has emerged in futures practice in the last few decades. Rather than opposing crazy with ‘normal,’ it equates ‘crazy’ with ‘impossible’, and opposes it with ‘plausible.’ You keep using that word. I do not think it means what you think it means. Inigo Montoya to Vizzini, in The Princess Bride In the early 80’s Roy Amara gave us a classic conceptualization of the set of all images of the future as roughly divisible into possible, probable, and preferred. This was useful because it was robust: • possible was the set of everything – every future possible to imagine, whether or not they had already been imagined; • probable was monitorable, if not measurable – researchers could observe emerging issues growing in momentum, becoming trends, evolving into greater probability; and • preferable was articulable – researchers could engage stakeholders in value discussions and judgements and essentially map the value territory. In Venn diagram terms, the categories overlap, but are still useful as a conceptual base for futures research methods. Yet somehow over the intervening decades, the terms have morphed to ‘possible, plausible, probable, and preferable futures’. Sometimes the wide and woolly set of ‘possible’ drops entirely from the field of view, and only ‘probable, plausible, and preferable’ futures remain. ‘Plausibility’ has emerged as a primary operating assumption, even a criterion for excellence,
  • 3. 3 within English-speaking scenario practice (especially within the community of ‘scenario planners’). Here is a partial inventory of the evidence: • FOR-LEARN: “To be effective, scenarios must be plausible, consistent and offer insights into the future. Plausibility: A scenario must be plausible. This means that it must fall within the limits of what might conceivably happen.” • From Thinking About the Future: Guidelines for Strategic Foresight, “…plausible futures: reasonable outcomes, with a discernable pathway from the present to the future. For example, discovering extraterrestrial life within the next decade is possible, but not plausible.” • From Godet, Creating Futures: “…scenarios are only credible and useful if they meet the following five conditions that we believe instill rigor: relevance, coherence, plausibility, importance and transparency.” • From Lindgren and Bandhold, Scenario Planning: “Scenarios are vivid descriptions of plausible futures.” • From Ralston and Wilson, The Scenario Planning Handbook: “…scenarios must meet the following criteria: they must be plausible – that is, they must fall within the limits of what might reasonably be expected to happen.” • From Sharpe and van der Heijden, eds., Scenarios for Success: “A scenario is a self-consistent account of one plausible way in which uncertain future events may play out with a bearing on the future of an organization and its ability to fulfil its purpose.” • From Delft University of Technology’s report for the European Community, Roadmapping eGovernment RTD 2020: “Scenarios are internally consistent, mutually different and plausible stories about a future.” • From Oliver Freeman of the Neville Freeman Agency, “Scenario planning is a metaphor-rich narrative designed to help you consider alternative, plausible futures.” [NB: but apparently only two of them: “One consistent feature though is the development of two scenarios.”!!!] • From x, y, and z, “A Review of Scenario Planning Literature” in Futures Research Quarterly, “Selecting a scenario space means examining the various future states the drivers could produce. Illogical and non-plausible situations should be rejected. Selecting alternative worlds to be detailed involves limiting the number of future stories, since it would be impossible to explore every option. The key is to select plausible futures that will challenge current thinking.” • From Paul J. H. Shoemaker, “When and How to Use Scenario Planning,” Journal of Forecasting: “…the scenarios should bound the range of plausible uncertainties and challenge managerial thinking.” • From [too many people to list on one page], “A formal framework for scenario development in support of environmental decision-making,” in Environmental Modelling & Software, “Scenarios are possible future states of the world that represent alternative plausible conditions under different assumptions.” Let me emphasise the first two of these quotes, because they clarify the matter by offering a definition, and an example, of plausibility. FOR-LEARN suggests that a plausible scenario “must fall within the limits of what might conceivably happen.” The authors of Thinking About the Future suggest that
  • 4. 4 plausible futures offer “…reasonable outcomes, with a discernable pathway from the present to the future.” They further clarify with an example: “For example, discovering extraterrestrial life within the next decade is possible, but not plausible.” The difficulty with both of these lies in the subjective capability and state of knowledge of the viewer: the more knowledgeable the viewer on the topic of the scenario, or its component details, the more events and futures they are capable of conceiving could happen. In the example given of discovering extraterrestrial life, the scenario is possible, but perhaps of low probability. But it is in fact plausible, because discernible pathways exist not only to the evolution of extraterrestrial life, but also to our discovery of it (given the various robotic surveys of other planets we have launched recently). Defining ‘plausibility’ is problematic. This limits its usefulness as a criterion for excellence in futures thinking, even assuming that it is an appropriate criterion for excellence. So let’s hit the dictionaries once again: what is the technical definition of ‘plausible,’ and what is its etymology? The Merriam-Webster Dictionary tells us that plausible means “superficially fair, reasonable, or valuable but often specious <a plausible pretext>; superficially pleasing or persuasive <a swindler… , then a quack, then a smooth, plausible gentleman — R. W. Emerson>; appearing worthy of belief <the argument was both powerful and plausible>.” Embedded within the structure of this word is the professional vulnerability that all futures researchers face in practicing an intellectual discipline for which there are no future facts, in a world of decision-makers hungry for an evidence base: how to seem valuable when we are suspected of purveying specious results and being quacks. The OED entry is even more telling, in offering us the older, now obsolete uses of the word, all of which revolve around pleasing the public and thereby winning approval. Oxford English Dictionary: Acceptable, agreeable, pleasing, gratifying; winning public approval, popular. Obs.; Expressing applause or approbation; plausive, applausive. Obs. ; Deserving of applause or approval; praiseworthy, laudable, commendable. Obs.; Of an argument, an idea, a statement, etc.: seeming reasonable, probable, or truthful; convincing, believable; (formerly) spec. having a false appearance of reason or veracity; specious. Of a person: convincing or persuasive, esp. with the intention to deceive. The OED then clarifies current uses, suggesting that plausible ideas seem reasonable or probable – while pointing out that it formerly implied such an appearance of reason was false. Furthermore, when applied to a person, it * still * implies an intention to deceive cloaked in false persuasion. My apologies for dwelling on these historical facts of etymological evolution at such length. But my observations of how consultants use the label ‘plausible scenarios’, or ‘plausible futures’, suggest that it is actually code for “…don’t give the clients crazy futures, or they’ll reject them, reject us, and we
  • 5. 5 won’t get paid and will never work in this town again.” How often in strategic foresight projects do the end results offer truly transformational futures that challenge participants to consider the possibilities of deep structural change? Of worlds with entirely different economic or political systems? Of usefully crazy futures? What are ‘futures’ – and why and how do we think about them? While scenario thinking per se originated in Herman Kahn’s policy strategy concerns, the origin of futures thinking is rooted in the image of the future, and Fred Polak’s concern for the vitality of human culture and civilizations. We shouldn’t limit the ‘futures’ in ‘crazy futures’ to strategic scenarios alone. Such purpose-designed images of the future compete for mental and emotional space with a nearly endless supply of images of the future generated across human activity. Imagining long-range futures is a talent unique to our species – so what images do we create, how do we create them, and what are they for? Futures studies as an intellectual endeavour includes the inventory and content analysis of existing images of the future. Humans express imagined futures in all media, as all variety of cultural constructs, and in varying scales of personal and civilizational usefulness. Images of the future in ads, in counselling programs, and in daydreams target personal behaviour by expressing self-fulfilling or self-defeating prophecies. In the same way, community, organizational, and political futures attempt to inspire group action through both cautionary tales (“doom and gloom”; nightmare futures), and through aspirational tomorrows (visions). Images of the future are embedded in all political discourses and ideologies, whether they warn of imminent national collapse at the hands of the opposition (nightmare futures), call for ‘Holding the course! Steady on!’ (present-trends-extended
  • 6. 6 futures), or depict a happy era to come (Conservatives: A return to the Golden Age! Liberals: An All-New Brighter Tomorrow!). At the largest scale, Polak’s call for aspirational, transformative images of the future was meant to catalyze civilization-level vitality. But categorizing any specific image of the future as a nightmare or a vision is entirely subjective. Furthermore, it is a subjective judgment that can generate tragedies. Conflicts arise when people fear a nightmare future is the hidden goal of others around them. And any extreme of difference is labelled ‘crazy,’ whether it is extremely good, extremely bad, or extremely different. We could probably conceptualize a craziness scale for futures, anchored at sanity/normality/plausibility on the one end, and ‘completely bug@%$@ crazy’ at the other extreme. It might be operationalized as the percentage of any given population that perceives a specific image of the future as offensively, scarily transgressive and transformative beyond all bounds of reason and decency. If nobody feels the future is beyond all bounds, it’s normal and plausible as normal can be. If 50% of the people feel it’s beyond all bounds of reason and decency, and the other 50% do not, then it’s only moderately crazy. And so on. It is not just the image itself that is judged. ‘Crazy futures’ often earn that label despite being prosaic and mundane in content, if they have transgressed in process. The current decision-making environment for many economic, political, and social issues is instrumentalist, evidence-based and biased towards Western empiricism. Sadly, a field that engages in research despite lacking an ability to observe its subject directly (until such time as tachyon-powered time machines or traversable wormholes enable field research in the futures) often lacks credibility as well. In a previous essay, I summarised this ‘cultural contradiction’ between the criteria for excellence in empirical, evidence-based research, and for excellence in futures research (specifically horizon scanning), as follows: Empirical/ Evidence-based Research Futures Research, especially emerging issues scanning & analysis • Credible; • Documented; • Authoritative; • Statistically significant; • Coherent: the data agree; • Consensus-based: the experts agree; • Any emerging issue unusual enough to be useful will probably lack apparent credibility; • it will be difficult to document, as only one or two cases of the change may yet exist; • it will emerge from marginalized populations, and be noticed initially by fringe sources, hardly the sort of authoritative sources that civil servants feel confident in citing; • as emerging issues are by definition only one or two cases, they are also by definition statistically insignificant; • the data will vary widely, converging over time only if the emerging issue matures into a trend; • not only will consensus be lacking, but experts will often violently attack reports of emerging issues of change, as they represent challenges to current paradigms and structures of expertise,
  • 7. 7 • Theoretically grounded; and • Mono-disciplinary. power, and entitlement; • emerging issues of change often challenge previous theoretical structures and necessitate the construction of new theories; • and the most interesting new change emerges where disciplines converge and clash. As the impacts ripple out across all the systems of reality, emerging changes and their impacts require a multi-disciplinary analytic perspective. Bishop, Hines, and Collins have inventoried formal futures methods for generating images of the future – both scenarios (extrapolations) and visions (value-based preferred future articulations) – and described almost two dozen rigorously structured processes. These range from logical and quantitative methods using statistical trend extrapolation, computer-aided cross-impact matrices, and systems dynamics modelling, through facilitated group process dialogue, to meditative, ‘guided visualization’ techniques. Evidence-based decision support cultures prefer their futures heavily salted with data and quantitative extrapolation. They are likely to shy away from guided visualization workshops. So decision-makers, observers – and practitioners – also judge some means of generating futures as ‘crazy’. Generally, the more intuitive methods are greeted with the most scepticism and distrust. Scenario planners and scenario builders are not alone in devising images of the future. Artists, advertisers, novelists, screenwriters, animators, sculptors, analysts, and leaders all generate stories, images, and artifacts expressing different future outcomes and environments. So do prophets, astrologers, tea-leaf readers, shamans, and particularly skilled remote viewers. By extension, if rigorous but intuitive tools such as guided visualization earn scepticism, artistic inspiration may as well – and astrologers, shamans, and remote viewers earn outright derision. Why shouldn’t they? Why shouldn’t we just discard images of the future generated by ‘crazy’ methods such as astrological computations and shamanic trances and remote viewing? To answer that we must return to the conceptual foundations and core assumptions of futures studies as a field of research, which include: • There is not one single future, but multiple alternative futures; • People’s beliefs about the future, and their images of the future, affect their decisions and actions, which in turn create the futures as an emergent property of aggregated interconnected actions; • Because any given lived future at any given moment is an emergent property of a complex system that frequently exhibits chaotic behavior, it is not possible to ‘predict’ human futures. Based on these assumptions, we can see that it is not important which future is correct, or which future is best supported by empirically credible data, or which future is most plausible. The most important future is the future the greatest number of people believe the most: it is the future on which they are basing their decisions and actions. And therefore it is absolutely irrelevant how that future was generated, and how credible its empirical underpinnings
  • 8. 8 are. If people read astrological forecasts or tea leaves or goat entrails, and then act on those images of the future, then those images of the future are important for us as futures researchers to consider. The craziest methods can generate the most compelling futures, and crazy or not, those are the futures with which we should be most concerned. Why are crazy futures the most useful? We all agree that your theory is crazy, but is it crazy enough? Niels Bohr Touching on the base assumptions of futures studies leads us directly to why crazy futures are the most useful: we are embedded in – and are ourselves – complex systems flirting daily with chaos. Chris Langton’s famous diagram, left, illustrates this concisely. There are fixed systems, existing in a state of maximum thermodynamic equilibrium, meaning maximum entropy – or death. There are periodic systems, which are ordered but not adaptive [expand]. There are chaotic systems, characterized by sensitive dependence on initial conditions, and non-linear determinism. Finally, there are complex systems that are self-organizing, self-directing, self- repairing, and adaptive [check for complete list of characteristics]. Their self- directing and adaptive characteristics result in evolutionary change over time, producing in turn novel emergent properties. They generate surprises. If sentient, they undoubtedly surprise themselves. Emergent properties are ‘out of the ordinary’, if by ordinary we mean the previous patterns of ordered system behaviour. So any complex adaptive system (and all human systems – whether single individuals or collections as organizations, or communities, or nation-states – are complex adaptive/evolving systems) will at one point or another generate ‘crazy states’ (apologies to Dror). It becomes even more likely that these systems will exhibit ‘crazy’, ie, ‘out of the ordinary’ behaviour if they are stressed by larger energy or information flows. One response to stress in a complex system is a phase change into chaotic behaviour. This phase change also creates the potential for novel and unusual behaviour outside the ordinary. So in the end, a focus on ‘crazy futures’ may be the most adaptive strategy we can encourage people to adopt, and a focus on ‘plausibility’ the most maladaptive. Is your future crazy enough to help you / your organization / your community evolve? Better that we rehearse the full range of surprises that may await us across our futures, than be ill-prepared and unable to adapt. Emergence and evolution are preferable to equilibrium.
  • 9. 9 How can we best communicate craziness? Before bowing out, there’s time for a brief thought on how we communicate compelling craziness. Animators have recently identified an interesting perceptual space they call ‘the uncanny valley.’ The uncanny valley hypothesis suggests that when human ‘replicas’ – either animated or robotic – look and act almost, but not perfectly, human, people response with revulsion. Granted, James Cameron seems to have overcome the effect in generating the Navi characters over the motion capture performance of his actors. The relevance to useful crazy futures is that something similar exists in conveying radically transgressive, transformative images of possible futures: up until a point, increasing craziness increases how exciting, provocative, and challenging they are. Beyond that point, increasing craziness pushes the futures into the uncanny valley of the unthinkable, on the other side of which is the transgression of perfect conceptual chaos. Whatever their degree of craziness, useful futures are compelling – people respond to them, adopt them, and use them to inform action. So how do we decrease the uncanny valley of the unthinkable – how do we avoid Cassandra syndrome? The endeavour of deploying crazy futures asks us to balance on a knife edge of usability: too normal, and no mind-shift results; too crazy, and brain-freeze occurs. Likewise, futures too divorced from our own experience may feel very crazy, but not be very compelling; too near to our own experience, and the futures will be too subjective to be useful – compelling, but insufficiently out of the ordinary. A possible antidote can be found in audience participation. Cutting edge methods in scenario building include projects like Jane McGonigal’s SuperStruct, and Evoke, and the growing body of work using the Institute for
  • 10. 10 the Future’s “Foresight Engine”. These projects all rely on massively crowd- sourced, participatory futures formation via on-line game environments. They evolve from each individual’s own participation, which is very compelling – but they evolve. The futures generated are emergent properties of the participants’ interactions with each other, and the useful strangeness arises from those interactions. In The Art of Immersion, Frank Rose offers a range of case studies underlining how powerfully engaging the unfinished story can be. This is the Web 2.0 corollary of McLuhan’s ‘media hot and cool’: the most compelling media are the ‘cool’ media, conveying ideas in low definition and inviting us to participate in completing the details. So whatever crazy futures we imagine, we should imagine them with holes, with interstitial spaces that invite other people to adapt them and adopt them: a crazy future must be compelling to be useful. Coda Alice laughed. "There's no use trying," she said. "One can't believe impossible things." "I dare say you haven't had much practice," said the queen. "When I was your age, I always did it for half an hour a day. Why, sometimes I've believed as many as six impossible things before breakfast." Lewis Carroll / Charles Dodgson, Alice in Wonderland I want to believe. Fox Mulder’s wall poster, The X-Files In order to thrive in whatever futures we pass through, it helps to rehearse what our values, assumptions, decisions and actions -- our very sense of self – might be in those futures. Authentic rehearsal inevitably requires that at some level we choose to believe not only what is plausible, and not just what is probable or possible, but that we stretch our values, assumptions, and sense of self to believe and rehearse for the impossible as well. So call me crazy. You have to go on and be crazy. Craziness is like heaven. Jimi Hendrix