SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 12
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
PSYCHOANALYTIC PSYCHOLOGY, 12(), 115-126
Copyright © 1995, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.




               Adult Attachment Style and
                Narcissistic Vulnerability

                                 M. Carole Pistole
                   Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey


   Although attachment theory has traditionally emphasized adaptive responses in
   the child-parent relationship, researchers have more recently applied attach-
   ment theory to adult love relationships. Both the child and adult literature have
   explored individual differences in attachment behavior and identified stylistic
   categories of secure and insecure attachments. Although the insecure categories
   are characterized by overt behavior which appears quite different (i.e., clinging
   vs. distance), in adult relationships where attachment is reciprocal, these stylis-
   tic patterns may achieve a similar function. In this article, I argue that, in adult
   relationships, insecure attachments reflect strategies for managing a greater
   level of narcissistic vulnerability than exists in secure attachment.


Attachment theory, which has a long-standing history and extensive liter-
ature in child development, has recently been extended to investigating
adults' love relationships. Both the child and adult literature have ad-
dressed qualitative or stylistic differences in how relatively healthy per-
sons function in attachment relationships. In the adult literature,
researchers have focused on individual differences in secure and insecure
categories and have not yet considered how insecure attachments, which
seem different from one another, may serve a similar purpose. Such a
distinction may be relevant only for adult attachment relationships, in
which the partners serve as attachment figures for each other and in which
the caregiving and sexual systems are also active in the relationship
(Shaver, Hazan, & Bradshaw, 1988). In this article, I link attachment and
narcissism to illuminate how different insecure categories of adult attach-
ment may be similar despite different overt characteristics.


  Requests for reprints should be sent to M. Carole Pistole, Department of Educational Psy-
chology, Graduate School of Education, Rutgers University, 10 Seminary Place, New Bruns-
wick, NJ 08903.
116     PISTOLE

                              ADULT ATTACHMENT

Attachment (Bowlby, 1979, 1988) influences development, psycholog cal
organization, and adult love (Shaver & Hazan, 1988; Shaver et: al., 19 !8).
According to Bowlby, attachment is a behavioral system experienced . s a
bond with a particular other who is sensed as a source of security and sal ;ty.
As a safe base, the attachment figure facilitates exploration and the qus lity
of development throughout the lifespan. More successful exploration  re-
duces a stronger sense of capability, and this in turn contributes to appro m-
ate self-reliance, autonomy, and success in relationships and work.
   Behavior within attachment relationships is planned and guided b / a
cognitive-affective schema or "internal working model" that is origin .11 y
constructed in infancy from interactions (Bowlby, 1979, 1988). This mo lei,
which also mediates the experiencing and meaning of the relationship, in-
cludes (a) expectations about the other's caring and responsiveness, (b)
beliefs about the self's worthiness of care and attention, and (c) rules for
affect regulation (e.g., in negative or distressing situations; Kobak & Sc& rj,
1988; Mikulincer, Florian, & Tolmacz, 1990). These working models are
thought to underlie individual differences in adult attachment style.
   Stimulated by an interest in love (Shaver & Hazan, 1988) and building on
the work of Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978) with infants, Ha :an
and Shaver (1987) identified three forms of adult attachment relationshi DS:
secure, insecure-anxious-ambivalent, and insecure-avoidant. More rec ;nt
research (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), which conceptualized styles
logically by crossing positive and negative models of both self and otl er,
suggests that a four-style model (secure, preoccupied, avoidant-fearful, ; nd
avoidant-dismissing) may more accurately describe adult attachment. I i a
comparison of the three and four-style models, Brennan, Shaver, and Tot ey
(1991) found systematic correspondence between the frameworks. The c is-
cussion in this article is organized along a three-style model because feai : u.
and dismissing are both avoidant styles with much in common.1
   Briefly, the research indicates that the securely attached are more coi fi-
dent and competent in their emotional interactions, more "happy, frienc ly
and trusting" (Hazan & Shaver, 1987, p. 515). Preoccupied (or anxious-a n-
bivalent) attachment is characterized by clinging and neediness and an
intense focus on the partner (Feeney & Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 198 7).
Avoidant attachment is distinguished by emotional distance and a comp il-
sive self-reliance (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Hazan & Shaver, 198 7).

                                    NARCISSISM

As introduced by Freud (1914/1961), the term narcissism has been used to
describe a variety of clinical phenomena, including the libidinal investmi nt

  'Distinctions between fearful and dismissing avoidance are articulated when meaningful.
ATTACHMENT       11 7


of the self (Moore & Fine, 1990; Sandier, Person, & Fonagy, 1991). In
current usage, the term narcissism, despite theoretical differences between
authors, is often used in the context of self-esteem and refers to an aspect of
personality, (i.e., of ego organization) that manifests in both healthy and
pathological ways (Blanck & Blanck, 1979; Kernberg, 1985; Kohut & Wolf,
1978; Moore & Fine, 1990; Sandier et a l , 1991).
   Narcissism is related to the cognitive-affective patterning or structuring
of the intrapsychic self (Blanck & Blanck, 1979; Kernberg, 1985; Kohut &
Wolf, 1978). With a well-patterned or solid intrapsychic structure, the person
is able to (a) soothe and comfort self (i.e., regulate esteem internally; Baker
& Baker, 1987), (b) sustain goals and relationships (Patton & Robbins,
1982), and (c) value both self and significant others (i.e., there is an even
distribution of self-esteem and other-esteem; Blanck & Blanck, 1979; Moore
& Fine, 1990). If, however, the self-structure is less patterned or more
nondifferentiated, positive valuing of the self and management of esteem
functions depend more on others' behaving in ways that support the self—
that is, provide valuing, confirming, or comforting functions (Baker &
Baker, 1987; Patton & Robbins, 1982). With a more fragile self-structure,
the person has more difficulty maintaining an inner sense of comfort and
esteem and so is more easily wounded or hurt (i.e., more narcissistically
vulnerable).


                   ATTACHMENT AND NARCISSISM

Although attachment and narcissism share some theoretical components,
they target separate phenomena. Both theories involve cognitive-affective
patterning, address affect regulation, and can accommodate healthy as well
as pathological development and functioning (see Armstrong & Roth, 1989;
Belsky & Nezworski, 1988; Bowlby, 1988; West & Sheldon, 1988). Attach-
ment, however, addresses the person's "need for proximity, care, and secu-
rity from another who can be experienced as separate from the s e l f
(Silverman, 1991, p. 183). Although Bowlby (1988) proposed that the attach-
ment system becomes integrated as an aspect of personality, the emphasis of
the theory is on interpersonal behavior and its representation. In contrast,
narcissism encompasses more general self-regard and undifferentiated or
merged aspects of ego organization. When pathological, narcissism ad-
dresses a "sense of self lacking sufficient inner resources to give meaning to
life simply by living it fully" (Bromberg, 1986, p. 441). Nevertheless, look-
ing at adult attachment relationships without reference to narcissism may
obscure how attachment patterns are related to esteem and self-protection. In
this article, I argue that insecure attachment is characterized by a greater
degree of narcissistic vulnerability than secure attachment. Concomitantly,
preoccupied and avoidant attachment reflect different strategies for manag-
ing vulnerability and self-esteem.
11 8    PISTOLE


                   ATTACHMENT AND SELF-ESTEEM
                          MANAGEMENT

All persons experience fluctuations in self-esteem (Kohut & Wolf, 19' 8),
but persons with less narcissistic vulnerability are more able "to man igs
feelings like inadequacy, weakness, incompetence, or guilt" (Kinston, l c 87,
p. 220). Being appropriately self-reliant, experiencing competence and n as-
tery in relation to internal standards and goals, is also a way of manag ng
esteem (see Elson, 1987; Kernberg, 1985). In addition, self-esteem is reh ted
to feelings about one's worth and value (Solomon, 1989). Although the
previous components have not been examined directly in adult attachm> nt,
research can be construed as supporting the notion that the securely attac led
are more capable at managing esteem. In secure attachment, more compel snt
affect regulation is suggested by a more frequent occurrence of posii ive
emotion (Simpson, 1990), fewer symptoms of distress (e.g., anxiety, ho; til-
ity, loneliness; Hazan & Shaver, 1990; Kobak & Sceery, 1988), and gre; ter
ego resilience (Kobak & Hazan, 1991; Kobak & Sceery, 1988). Sec ire
attachment is also associated with lower levels of self-conscious anxi ;ty
(Feeney & Noller, 1990). Other research indicates that the securely attaci ed
have more competency or mastery experiences with which to regulate ss-
teem by living up to internal standards. Securely attached adults have a 1 ;s:>
emotionally permeated approach to goals evidenced by greater satisfact on
with work, less difficulty completing tasks, and less fear of failure or rej ac-
tion from co-workers (Hazan & Shaver, 1990). In addition, attachment in: lu-
ences college students' adjustment arid career maturity (Blustein, Walbrid *e,
Friedlander, & Palladino, 1991; Kenny, 1987a, 1987b; Lapsley, Rice, &
Fitzgerald, 1990). Secure attachment is also associated with more success rul
relationship functioning as demonstrated through longer relationship  ife
(Hazan & Shaver, 1987); use of an integrating style of conflict resolut on
(Pistole, 1989); and higher levels of passion, commitment, and satisfact on
(Levy & Davis, 1988). Furthermore, measured in various ways in seve ra
studies, self-worth is consistently higher among the securely attached (C j  -
lins & Read, 1990; Feeney & Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Ryar &.
Lynch, 1989). Research suggests, therefore, that secure attachment is disl n-
guished by more effective self-esteem management and, by implication, v, th
a more solid self-structure and less narcissistic vulnerability.


           INTERPERSONAL EFFECTS OF NARCISSISTIC
                      VULNERABILITY

Narcissistic vulnerability affects the management of adults' love relatii n-
ships (Elson, 1987; Solomon, 1989), because the person needs to obt in
self-functions from the environment. Relationships that are not driven oy
narcissistic vulnerability involve "a mutuality in which the focus on the s ;li
ATTACHMENT        11 9


is balanced by recognition of another as a separate, autonomous self (Solo-
mon, 1989, p. 47). The self is solid enough that the partner is intellectually
and emotionally experienced as different and separate from self (i.e., with
separate interests and desires). Although involvement with the partner does
heighten self-esteem, esteem enhancement is provided through a sense of
mastery or competence including success in the relationship and apprecia-
tion of the partner as a way of fulfilling internal values and standards.
   Research indicates that secure attachment relationships demonstrate a
sense of self and the partner as separate. For example, secure attachment has
been associated with more positive views of others (Collins & Read, 1990;
Hazan & Shaver, 1987), interdependence (Simpson, 1990), intimacy
(Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Levy & Davis, 1988), trust (Feeney &
Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987), and mutuality (Feeney & Noller,
1991). Securely attached adults have reported "being able to accept and
support the partner despite the partner's faults" (Hazan & Shaver, 1987, p.
515); and in describing their relationships, they "emphasize the importance
of openness and closeness . . . while at the same time seeking to retain their
individual identity" (Feeney & Noller, 1991, p. 208). The picture that
emerges of secure attachment includes an appreciation of both self and other
as well as a capacity for openness and cooperativeness. This description is
consistent with others being perceived as separate people and with esteem
being distributed between self and other.


Narcissistic Use of the Partner

More narcissistically based relationships are characterized by the needs of
the self assuming a primary importance. The self is more fragile, and esteem
is more difficult to manage internally—that is, there exists a greater degree
of narcissistic vulnerability (Solomon, 1989). The person is more sensitive
to emotional injury, focuses attention more on personal needs than on the
partner, and expects partner to behave in affirming and self-enhancing ways.
Interactions with the partner are often dictated by the need to stabilize a
sense of worth and to regulate feelings, especially negative feelings about
self (see Kinston, 1987).
   More narcissistically vulnerable persons, in adapting, organize defensive
structures (i.e., patterns "of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors"; Patton &
Robbins, 1982, p. 880) that attempt to cover over or compensate for the
vulnerability and thereby protect the self. Rather than regulating esteem
needs through an internal self-confirming process, self-regard is accom-
plished through a pattern of approaching (e.g., merging; Kohut & Wolf,
1978) or distancing from (Akhtar & Thomson, 1982) significant others. The
other person's importance stems more from bolstering or maintaining the
self and less from an appreciation of the other in his or her separateness (i.e.,
likeness and "differentness") from self. For example, the person phenomeno-
120    PISTOLE

logically enhances self-esteem through fusion with a partner who is >er-
ceived as possessing "all greatness, all power, all esteem, all worth ind
value" (Elson, 1987, p. 40). With the partner serving as a selfobject (Kc hut
& Wolf, 1978), the power and worth are experienced as belonging to s ;lf.
The partner is valued as part of the self (Elson, 1987, p. 40) and "for ths
internal functions and the emotional stability" (Baker & Baker, 1987, p 2)
he or she augments. Moreover, as a part of the self, the partner is expectei to
interact in a way that is congruent with the self's defensive strategies.
   If the partner does not meet the self's narcissistic needs (e.g., for ck se-
ness or distance), then the person is subject to an awareness of differentr sss
between self and partner. This incongruence would be experienced a > a
separation threat and trigger intense separation anxiety, which would aro lse
the attachment system (Bowlby, 1988). The ensuing attachment beha ior
might also be contaminated by defensive behavior designed to regulate ; nd
protect self (rather than regain security). That is, the needs of the attachm ;nt
system would be to experience the partner as either symbolically or ph1 si-
cally available. Defensive needs would be to protect the self "from expi ri-
encing needs for love, understanding, and validation" (Basch, 1987, p. 31 8).


    Preoccupied attachment.         Preoccupied attachment can be constri ed
as a defensive strategy in which narcissistic vulnerability is manaj ec.
through merger with the partner. In research, preoccupied romantic relati< >n-
ships were characterized by high levels of idealizing the partner and ar
extreme approach to love which includes obsessive preoccupation (Peer ey
& Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987), hypervigilance to separation,
greater distress over separation (Mikulincer et al., 1990), and attending tc
distress (Kobak & Sceery, 1988). These characteristics indicate more inti n-
sity in attention to partner than is required for interdependency and intim; -y
(see Elson, 1987), which indeed are not so well accomplished in preoccup: sd
attachment. In addition, the strong clinging and idealized focus on partnei is
consistent with gaining affirmation through merger—that is, experienci ig
the idealized other, who contains worth and value, as if he or she were a p irt
of self, were a selfobject.
    Moreover, although subtle, language also indicates fusion with the pa :t-
ner. In one study, persons with a preoccupied attachment exhibited a higl er
level of couple references ("we" vs. "I") associated with the perception of
problems in the relationship (Feeney & Noller, 1991). Only when there ; re
problems (i.e., incongruence between self and partner) is a "we" (two ps o-
ple) versus an "I" (fusion) recognized.
    Other relationship characteristics also suggest that a component of nee< !i-
ness directs the relationship behaviors. Studies have found that preoccupi ;d
attachment is characterized by more emotional dependence, a desire i :>r
more commitment (Feeney & Noller, 1990), greater reliance on the partn ;r,
more use of others as a safe base (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), a id
ATTACHMENT       121


inappropriately high levels of self-disclosure (Mikulincer & Nachshon,
 1991). Further, persons with a preoccupied attachment experience more
emotional ups and downs within the relationship (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). In
sum, they seem to "depend on others to maintain positive self regard"
(Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991, p. 234).
   The interpretation of clinging preoccupation as a defensive strategy is
supported by other research. Preoccupied attachment has been associated
with lower levels of esteem (Collins & Read, 1990; Feeney & Noller,
1990), and research indicates that low self-esteem persons use interper-
sonal behavior "to enhance their self-affect" (Baumgardner, Kaufman, &
Levy, 1989, p. 919). Consistent with this view, Mikulincer et al. (1990)
concluded that persons with preoccupied attachments "do not emphasize
the caring component in close relationships and their behavior is not
motivated by consideration of others' interests" (p. 278). Other research
finding lower levels of friendship in their love relationships (Feeney &
Noller, 1991) and a control component in their pattern of interpersonal
difficulties (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991) also supports the view that
preoccupied relationship behavior is motivated by self-sustaining needs.
There appears to be an "overwhelming need . . . to simply be in a relation-
ship, no matter what or with whom—the primary goal is emotional secu-
rity" (Newcomb, 1981, p. 134).

   Avoidant attachment. In avoidant attachment, narcissistic vulnera-
bility is managed through distancing from the partner, thereby, avoiding
closeness and intimacy (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Hazan &
Shaver, 1987). These relationships are associated with low levels of
relying on others, using others as a safe base, romantic involvement
(Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), self-disclosure (Bartholomew & Horo-
witz, 1991; Mikulincer & Nachshon, 1991), intensity (Bartholomew,
1990; Feeney & Noller, 1990), and higher separation distress than the
securely attached (Mikulincer et al., 1990). Further, affect is regulated
through dismissing the importance of attachment (Bartholomew & Horo-
witz, 1991), dismissing distress (Kobak & Sceery, 1988), directing atten-
tion toward nonemotional domains (e.g., work; Hazan & Shaver, 1990),
and idealizing self or other (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). The need to wall off
or reject a portion of experience (e.g., intense feelings) is indicative of
narcissistic vulnerability and a need for partner's cooperation in manag-
ing self. Because self-regard is based "on the ability to temporarily
tolerate negative affects in order to achieve mastery over threatening or
frustrating situations" (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988, p. 304), the defensive
function of avoidant strategies leaves the self-structure still vulnerable.
   An additional indication of narcissistic vulnerability in avoidant attach-
ment comes from the functioning of anger in relationships. "Anger and
hostility are often instigated by threats to self-esteem of an interpersonal
122     PISTOLE

nature" (Kernis, Grannemann, & Barclay, 1989, p. 1013). Perhaps the ho: til -
ity associated with avoidant attachment (Bartholomew & Horowitz, IS 31;
Kobak & Sceery, 1988) is triggered as a self-protective mechanism that (a)
defends against anxiety and negative feelings about self or (b) function to
repair damaged self-esteem and preserve a feeling of well-being (Kerni ; et
al.( 1989; Solomon, 1989).
    Self-defense in the relationship is also suggested by the avoidantly at-
tached person's endorsement of love as friendship in the absence of a co re-
sponding endorsement of romantic love, passion, commitment, or
satisfaction (see Feeney & Noller, 1990, 1991; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; L ;y
& Davis, 1988). The endorsement of friendship can be interpreted as a me ins
of maintaining safer levels of emotional intensity, which is consistent wi h a
more fragile self-structure and with using a defensive style rather t lan
internal resources to regulate esteem.
    In a seeming contradiction to this argument, like the securely attached nd
unlike fearful avoidants, dismissing avoidants have reported high self es-
teem and self-acceptance (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). The uniqt sly
high, positive evaluation of self was coupled with a uniquely low leve of
subjective distress and with interpersonal problems characterized by ho: til-
ity and coldness (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). This constellation of
findings, interpreted in conjunction with directing attention away from i is-
tress (Kobak & Sceery, 1988) and dismissing attachment needs (B .rt-
holomew & Horowitz, 1991), can be construed as indicating a defensiv ily
bolstered self. Meaningful self-worth would be accompanied by compete ice
in relationship and affect management (Basch, 1988), which is associa :ed
with secure but not dismissing attachment.
    In sum, the primary characteristic of avoidant attachment is avoidance of
closeness and ensuing intimacy. The defensive strategy creates a sort of
safety in the perceived "detachment" from the partner. Distance facility tes
cutting off or never being "touched" by perceived criticism or the experie ice
of intense emotions and, thereby, protects a fragile self from being emoti >n-
ally overwhelmed with unmanageable emotion. Similarly, the stance of
detachment functions to keep away from the self-structure "anything t lat
would diminish it" (Akhtar & Thomson, 1982, p. 13). For instance, by
distancing, fearful avoidant people hold at bay their fear of intimacy, pro )a-
ble rejection, and the self's being overwhelmed with unmanageable emot on
(see Bartholomew, 1990). Similarly, the high self-concept of dismiss ng
avoidance can be construed as an idealization of self. Distancing ft )m
emotional closeness with partner helps ensure that the facade is not pu ic-
tured, self-esteem is not injured, and unmanageable emotion is not exp ri-
enced. In avoidant attachment, the person protects self against he
dangerousness of others (Kinston, 1987). It is as if the persons' "fragile se lse
of self will disintegrate" (Modell, 1986, p. 299) or be emotionally o^ er-
whelmed or swallowed up (see Kohut & Wolf, 1978) if the partners get cl >se
or if feelings are intense.
ATTACHMENT        123

                               CONCLUSION

Looking at attachment through the lens of narcissism stimulates making a
distinction between appropriate security needs and narcissistic use of the
partner to manage self and avoid being hurt. In preoccupied attachment, the
defensive strategy is to merge with an idealized other who bolsters feelings
of worth. In avoidant attachment, the partner is distanced to maintain self
through a behavioral or phenomenological response that strictly avoids
closeness and any ensuing intense or negative feelings. One avoidant strat-
egy keeps the self contained, closed, passive, and nonassertive; the other
strategy protects through idealizing the self and discounting the importance
of the attachment system.
    Although I explored how healthy personalities navigate narcissistic is-
sues, attachment style may also be relevant to psychopathology. Fearful
avoidance corresponds closely to avoidant personality disorder (Bart-
holomew, 1990), and dismissing avoidance is reminiscent of narcissistic
personality disorder. The high, defensive self-concept of dismissing attach-
ment is similar to the idealized, narcissistic grandiose self; both patterns
involve latent vulnerability, coldness, hostility, and using others (American
Psychiatric Association, 1987; Kernberg, 1984).
   Finally, the distinction between attachment and narcissistic needs can be
useful to both clinicians and researchers. With relationship issues, therapists
can facilitate clients' progress by defining and validating attachment needs
and also clarifying how narcissistic needs related to self-regard, self-esteem
management, and ego organization are compromising autonomy and inti-
macy. Further, because avoidant attachment is associated with hostility,
which is in turn associated with pathological aspects of narcissism (see
Raskin, Novacek, & Hogan, 1991) and with shame (Tangney, Wagner,
Fletcher, & Gramzow, 1992), research investigating associations between
attachment style, narcissism, and shame might be productive. Last, research
designed to separate aspects of personality organized around specific attach-
ment needs from more global aspects of personality organized around needs
to manage esteem and defensively protect self would be useful. Investigating
adults' attachment behavior under conditions of unexpected separation—that
is, when proximity seeking and security needs are active and strongest
(Bowlby, 1979, 1988)—may lead to distinctions between attachment and
narcissistic vulnerability and thereby enrich the science and practice of
psychology.

                         ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

A portion of this article was presented at the 1993 Convention of the Ameri-
can Psychological Association.
   I appreciate the helpful comments of Richard De Lisi and the anonymous
reviewers.
124      PISTOLE

                                     REFERENCES

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C , Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of artachme. t: A
  psychological study of the strange situation. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Akhtar, S., & Thomson, J. A., Jr. (1982). Overview: Narcissistic personality disorder. Amei can
  Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 12-20.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental d ^or-
  ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
Armstrong, J. G., & Roth, D. M. (1989). Attachment and separation difficulties in e; ling
  disorders: A preliminary investigation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8, 4 1 -
   155.
Baker, H. S., & Baker, M. N. (1987). Heinz Kohut's self psychology: An overview. Amet can
  Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1-9.
Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment perspective. Journal of Si civ.l
  and Personal Relationships, 7, 147-178.
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test ;>f a
  four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244.
Basch, M. F. (1987). The interpersonal and the intrapsychic: Conflict or harmony'' Contei pc-
  rary Psychoanalysis, 23, 367-381.
Basch, M. F. (1988). Understanding psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.
Baumgardner, A. H., Kaufman, C. M., & Levy, P. E. (1989). Regulating affect intcrperson Sly.
  When low esteem leads to greater enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psyc IOI-
  ogy, 56, 907-921.
Belsky, ]., & Nezworski, T. (Eds.). (1988). Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ:
  Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Blanck, G., & Blanck, R. (1979). Ego psychology 11: Psychoanalytic developmental psychol <gy.
  New York: Columbia University Press.
Blustein, D. L., Walbridge, M. M., Friedlander, M. L., & Palladino, D. E. (1991). Contribut :>ns
  of psychological separation and parental attachment to the career development proc ;ss.
  Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 39-50.
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. New York: Tavistock.
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. New York: Basic Books.
Brennan, K. A., Shaver, P. R., & Tobey, A. E. (1991). Attachment styles, gender and pare ital
  problem drinking. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 451-466.
Bromberg, P. M. (1986). The mirror and the mask: On narcissism and psychoanalytic growtl hi
  A. P. Morrison (Ed.), Essential papers on narcissism (pp. 438-466). New York: New ^ ark
  University Press.
Cassidy, J., & Kobak, R. R. (1988). Avoidance and its relation to other defensive processes In
  J. Belsky & T. Nezworksi (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 300-3: 3)
  Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship qua ity
  in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644-663.
Elson, M. (Ed.). (1987). The Kohut seminars. New York: Norton.
Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic relat: )n
  ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 281-291.
Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1991). Attachment style and verbal descriptions of r o m a tic
  partners. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 187—215.
Freud, S. (1961). On narcissism: An introduction. London: Hogarth. (Original work publis iec
  1914)
Hazan, C , & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Jout iai
  of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524.
Hazan, C , & Shaver, P. R. (1990). Love and work: An attachment-theoretical perspect ipe
ATTACHMENT            125


  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 270-280.
Kenny, M. E. (1987a). The extent and function of parental attachment among first-year college
  students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 17-29.
Kenny, M. E. (1987b). Family ties and leaving home for college: Recent findings and im-
  plications. Journal of College Student Personnel, 28, 438-442.
Kernberg, O. F. (1984). Severe personality disorders. New Haven, CT: Yale University.
Kernberg, 0 . F. (1985). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Northvale, NJ:
  Aronson.
Kernis, M. H., Grannemann, B. D., & Barclay, L. C. (1989). Stability and level of self-esteem
  as predictors of anger arousal and hostility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
  1013-1022.
Kinston, W. (1987). The shame of narcissism. In D. L. Nathanson (Ed.), The many faces of
  shame (pp. 214-245). New York: Guilford.
Kobak, R. R., & Hazan, C. (1991). Attachment in marriage: Effects of security and accuracy of
  working models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 861-869.
Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect
  regulation, and representations of self and others. Child Development, 59, 135-146.
Kohut, H., & Wolf, E. S. (1978). The disorders of the self and their treatment: An outline.
  International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 59, 413-425.
Lapsley, D. K., Rice, K. G., & Fitzgerald, D. P. (1990). Adolescent attachment, identity, and
  adjustment to college: Implications for the continuity of adaptation hypothesis. Journal of
  Counseling and Development, 68, 561-565.
Levy, M. C , & Davis, K. E. (1988). Lovestyles and attachment styles compared: Their relations
  to each other and to various relationship characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal
  Relationships, 5, 439-471.
Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Tolmacz, R. (1990). Attachment styles and fear of personal death:
  A case study of affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 273—280.
Mikulincer, M., & Nachshon, O. (1991). Attachment styles and patterns of self-disclosure.
  Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61. 321-331.
Model], A. H. (1986). A narcissistic defence against affects and the illusion of self-sufficiency.
  In A. P. Morrison (Ed.), Essential papers on narcissism (pp. 293-307). New York: New York
  University.
Moore, B. E., & Fine, B. D. (1990). Psychoanalytic terms and concepts. New Haven, CT: The
  American Psychoanalytic Association and Yale University Press.
Newcomb, M. D. (1981). Heterosexual cohabitation relationships. In S. Duck & R. Gilmour
  (Eds.), Personal relationships 1: Studying personal relationships (pp. 131-164). New York:
  Academic.
Patton, M. J., & Robbins, S. B. (1982). Kohut's self-psychology as a model for college-student
  counseling. Professional Psychology, 13, 876-888.
Pistole, M. C. (1989). Attachment in adult romantic relationships: Style of conflict resolution
  and relationship satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 505-510.
Raskin, R., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. (1991). Narcissistic self-esteem management. Journal of
  Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 911-918.
Ryan, R. M., & Lynch, J. J. (1989). Emotional autonomy versus detachment: Revisiting the
  vicissitudes of adolescence and young adulthood. Child Development, 60, 340-356.
Sandier, J., Person, E. S., & Fonagy, P. (Eds.). (1991). Freud's "On narcissism: An introduc-
  tion." New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Shaver, P., & Hazan, C. (1988). A biased overview of the study of love. Journal of Social and
  Personal Relationships, 5, 473-501.
Shaver, P., Hazan, C , & Bradshaw, D. (1988). Love as attachment: The integration of three
  behavioral systems. In R. Sternberg & M. Barnes (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 68-99).
  New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
126      PISTOLE


Silverman, D. K. (1991). Attachment patterns and Freudian theory: An integrative prop >sal.
  Psychoanalytic Psychology, 8, 169-193.
Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic relationships. Journ, I of
  Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 971-980.
Solomon, M. F. (1989). Narcissism and intimacy. New York: Norton.
Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., Fletcher, C , & Gramzow, R. (1992). Shamed into anger? The reh :ion
  of shame and guilt to anger and self-reported aggression. Journal of Personality and S( -id
  Psychology, 62, 669-675.
West, M., & Sheldon, A. E. R. (1988). Classification of pathological attachment patterr ; in
  adults. Journal of Personality Disorders, 2, 153-159.

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Fds the symptom is not a sign but a means to an end
Fds  the symptom is not a sign but a means to an endFds  the symptom is not a sign but a means to an end
Fds the symptom is not a sign but a means to an endMosheAlmagor
 
Family Systems Theory
Family Systems TheoryFamily Systems Theory
Family Systems TheoryJason Wrench
 
Bs 101 module 4a - attitude and theories of attribution
Bs 101   module 4a - attitude and theories of attributionBs 101   module 4a - attitude and theories of attribution
Bs 101 module 4a - attitude and theories of attributionTamojit Das
 

Was ist angesagt? (7)

Neffadaptive
NeffadaptiveNeffadaptive
Neffadaptive
 
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
Why Education Predicts Decreased Belief in Conspiracy Theories. JAN-WILLEM VA...
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Fds the symptom is not a sign but a means to an end
Fds  the symptom is not a sign but a means to an endFds  the symptom is not a sign but a means to an end
Fds the symptom is not a sign but a means to an end
 
Family Systems Theory
Family Systems TheoryFamily Systems Theory
Family Systems Theory
 
Bowen Family Systems Theory Sept 2017
Bowen Family Systems Theory Sept 2017Bowen Family Systems Theory Sept 2017
Bowen Family Systems Theory Sept 2017
 
Bs 101 module 4a - attitude and theories of attribution
Bs 101   module 4a - attitude and theories of attributionBs 101   module 4a - attitude and theories of attribution
Bs 101 module 4a - attitude and theories of attribution
 

Ähnlich wie Narcissism and vulnerability

Attachment And Romantic Relationships The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
Attachment And Romantic Relationships  The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...Attachment And Romantic Relationships  The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
Attachment And Romantic Relationships The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...Joshua Gorinson
 
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology A Review Of The ...
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology  A Review Of The ...A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology  A Review Of The ...
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology A Review Of The ...Scott Faria
 
Attachment Theory And Gestalt Psychology 2007
Attachment Theory And Gestalt Psychology  2007Attachment Theory And Gestalt Psychology  2007
Attachment Theory And Gestalt Psychology 2007Lori Moore
 
Addiction And Attachment Theory
Addiction And Attachment TheoryAddiction And Attachment Theory
Addiction And Attachment TheoryKristen Carter
 
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentesAutocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentesCátia Rodrigues
 
Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between
Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between
Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between sherni1
 
An Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
An Emerging Theory Of Human RelatednessAn Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
An Emerging Theory Of Human RelatednessJim Webb
 
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And PeersAdolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peersgaz12000
 
Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docx
Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docxEmotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docx
Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docxSALU18
 
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults Spontaneous Self-Representations
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults  Spontaneous Self-RepresentationsAgency And Communion Attributes In Adults  Spontaneous Self-Representations
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults Spontaneous Self-RepresentationsAudrey Britton
 
Journal olPersonality and Social Psychology1995. Vol. 68. N.docx
Journal olPersonality and Social Psychology1995. Vol. 68. N.docxJournal olPersonality and Social Psychology1995. Vol. 68. N.docx
Journal olPersonality and Social Psychology1995. Vol. 68. N.docxtawnyataylor528
 
The Five Factor Model Of Personality
The Five Factor Model Of PersonalityThe Five Factor Model Of Personality
The Five Factor Model Of PersonalityAngie Lee
 
Adult Attachment Analysis
Adult Attachment AnalysisAdult Attachment Analysis
Adult Attachment AnalysisRachel Davis
 
Adult Attachment Measures A 25-Year Review
Adult Attachment Measures  A 25-Year ReviewAdult Attachment Measures  A 25-Year Review
Adult Attachment Measures A 25-Year ReviewJoe Andelija
 

Ähnlich wie Narcissism and vulnerability (20)

Attachment And Romantic Relationships The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
Attachment And Romantic Relationships  The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...Attachment And Romantic Relationships  The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
Attachment And Romantic Relationships The Role Of Working Models Of Self And...
 
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology A Review Of The ...
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology  A Review Of The ...A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology  A Review Of The ...
A Comparison Of Attachment Theory And Individual Psychology A Review Of The ...
 
ISI Paper 1 (1)
ISI Paper 1 (1)ISI Paper 1 (1)
ISI Paper 1 (1)
 
Clinical Paper
Clinical PaperClinical Paper
Clinical Paper
 
Attachment Theory And Gestalt Psychology 2007
Attachment Theory And Gestalt Psychology  2007Attachment Theory And Gestalt Psychology  2007
Attachment Theory And Gestalt Psychology 2007
 
Addiction And Attachment Theory
Addiction And Attachment TheoryAddiction And Attachment Theory
Addiction And Attachment Theory
 
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentesAutocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
Autocompaixao e autojulgamento em adolescentes
 
Family Systems Theory
Family Systems TheoryFamily Systems Theory
Family Systems Theory
 
Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between
Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between
Maladaptive perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between
 
An Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
An Emerging Theory Of Human RelatednessAn Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
An Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
 
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And PeersAdolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
Adolescent Attachment To Parents And Peers
 
Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docx
Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docxEmotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docx
Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docx
 
Family psychology
Family psychologyFamily psychology
Family psychology
 
Forgiveness
ForgivenessForgiveness
Forgiveness
 
Autismo 3
Autismo 3Autismo 3
Autismo 3
 
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults Spontaneous Self-Representations
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults  Spontaneous Self-RepresentationsAgency And Communion Attributes In Adults  Spontaneous Self-Representations
Agency And Communion Attributes In Adults Spontaneous Self-Representations
 
Journal olPersonality and Social Psychology1995. Vol. 68. N.docx
Journal olPersonality and Social Psychology1995. Vol. 68. N.docxJournal olPersonality and Social Psychology1995. Vol. 68. N.docx
Journal olPersonality and Social Psychology1995. Vol. 68. N.docx
 
The Five Factor Model Of Personality
The Five Factor Model Of PersonalityThe Five Factor Model Of Personality
The Five Factor Model Of Personality
 
Adult Attachment Analysis
Adult Attachment AnalysisAdult Attachment Analysis
Adult Attachment Analysis
 
Adult Attachment Measures A 25-Year Review
Adult Attachment Measures  A 25-Year ReviewAdult Attachment Measures  A 25-Year Review
Adult Attachment Measures A 25-Year Review
 

Mehr von veropabon

Body language
Body languageBody language
Body languageveropabon
 
C.g. jung lo inconsciente en la vida psiquica normal y patololica
C.g. jung   lo inconsciente en la vida psiquica normal y patololicaC.g. jung   lo inconsciente en la vida psiquica normal y patololica
C.g. jung lo inconsciente en la vida psiquica normal y patololicaveropabon
 
Bioenergetica tartamudez
Bioenergetica tartamudezBioenergetica tartamudez
Bioenergetica tartamudezveropabon
 
Bioenergetica, la energia en el cuerpo
Bioenergetica, la energia en el cuerpoBioenergetica, la energia en el cuerpo
Bioenergetica, la energia en el cuerpoveropabon
 
Belt técnica vocal
Belt técnica vocalBelt técnica vocal
Belt técnica vocalveropabon
 
Becoming and being a bioenergetic analyst
Becoming and being a bioenergetic analystBecoming and being a bioenergetic analyst
Becoming and being a bioenergetic analystveropabon
 
Arteterapia resolucion conflictos
Arteterapia resolucion conflictosArteterapia resolucion conflictos
Arteterapia resolucion conflictosveropabon
 
Arteterapiaes un acompañamiento
Arteterapiaes un acompañamientoArteterapiaes un acompañamiento
Arteterapiaes un acompañamientoveropabon
 
Arteterapia y mandalas
Arteterapia y mandalasArteterapia y mandalas
Arteterapia y mandalasveropabon
 
Arteterapia en educacion
Arteterapia en educacionArteterapia en educacion
Arteterapia en educacionveropabon
 
Aaatratamientoesquizo
AaatratamientoesquizoAaatratamientoesquizo
Aaatratamientoesquizoveropabon
 
An introduction to the somatic energetic
An introduction to the somatic energeticAn introduction to the somatic energetic
An introduction to the somatic energeticveropabon
 
Narcissism and ideal ego
Narcissism and ideal egoNarcissism and ideal ego
Narcissism and ideal egoveropabon
 
Narcissism (1)
Narcissism (1)Narcissism (1)
Narcissism (1)veropabon
 
Narcisistic dilemma love
Narcisistic dilemma loveNarcisistic dilemma love
Narcisistic dilemma loveveropabon
 
Narcisismo vs yo_herido
Narcisismo vs yo_heridoNarcisismo vs yo_herido
Narcisismo vs yo_heridoveropabon
 
Mudras completo, reiki, feng shui
Mudras completo, reiki, feng shuiMudras completo, reiki, feng shui
Mudras completo, reiki, feng shuiveropabon
 
Ohashi como%2 bleer%2bel%2bcuerpo
Ohashi como%2 bleer%2bel%2bcuerpoOhashi como%2 bleer%2bel%2bcuerpo
Ohashi como%2 bleer%2bel%2bcuerpoveropabon
 
Narcissism self enchantment
Narcissism self enchantmentNarcissism self enchantment
Narcissism self enchantmentveropabon
 
Psicomotricidad y arteterapia
Psicomotricidad y arteterapiaPsicomotricidad y arteterapia
Psicomotricidad y arteterapiaveropabon
 

Mehr von veropabon (20)

Body language
Body languageBody language
Body language
 
C.g. jung lo inconsciente en la vida psiquica normal y patololica
C.g. jung   lo inconsciente en la vida psiquica normal y patololicaC.g. jung   lo inconsciente en la vida psiquica normal y patololica
C.g. jung lo inconsciente en la vida psiquica normal y patololica
 
Bioenergetica tartamudez
Bioenergetica tartamudezBioenergetica tartamudez
Bioenergetica tartamudez
 
Bioenergetica, la energia en el cuerpo
Bioenergetica, la energia en el cuerpoBioenergetica, la energia en el cuerpo
Bioenergetica, la energia en el cuerpo
 
Belt técnica vocal
Belt técnica vocalBelt técnica vocal
Belt técnica vocal
 
Becoming and being a bioenergetic analyst
Becoming and being a bioenergetic analystBecoming and being a bioenergetic analyst
Becoming and being a bioenergetic analyst
 
Arteterapia resolucion conflictos
Arteterapia resolucion conflictosArteterapia resolucion conflictos
Arteterapia resolucion conflictos
 
Arteterapiaes un acompañamiento
Arteterapiaes un acompañamientoArteterapiaes un acompañamiento
Arteterapiaes un acompañamiento
 
Arteterapia y mandalas
Arteterapia y mandalasArteterapia y mandalas
Arteterapia y mandalas
 
Arteterapia en educacion
Arteterapia en educacionArteterapia en educacion
Arteterapia en educacion
 
Aaatratamientoesquizo
AaatratamientoesquizoAaatratamientoesquizo
Aaatratamientoesquizo
 
An introduction to the somatic energetic
An introduction to the somatic energeticAn introduction to the somatic energetic
An introduction to the somatic energetic
 
Narcissism and ideal ego
Narcissism and ideal egoNarcissism and ideal ego
Narcissism and ideal ego
 
Narcissism (1)
Narcissism (1)Narcissism (1)
Narcissism (1)
 
Narcisistic dilemma love
Narcisistic dilemma loveNarcisistic dilemma love
Narcisistic dilemma love
 
Narcisismo vs yo_herido
Narcisismo vs yo_heridoNarcisismo vs yo_herido
Narcisismo vs yo_herido
 
Mudras completo, reiki, feng shui
Mudras completo, reiki, feng shuiMudras completo, reiki, feng shui
Mudras completo, reiki, feng shui
 
Ohashi como%2 bleer%2bel%2bcuerpo
Ohashi como%2 bleer%2bel%2bcuerpoOhashi como%2 bleer%2bel%2bcuerpo
Ohashi como%2 bleer%2bel%2bcuerpo
 
Narcissism self enchantment
Narcissism self enchantmentNarcissism self enchantment
Narcissism self enchantment
 
Psicomotricidad y arteterapia
Psicomotricidad y arteterapiaPsicomotricidad y arteterapia
Psicomotricidad y arteterapia
 

Narcissism and vulnerability

  • 1. PSYCHOANALYTIC PSYCHOLOGY, 12(), 115-126 Copyright © 1995, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Adult Attachment Style and Narcissistic Vulnerability M. Carole Pistole Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey Although attachment theory has traditionally emphasized adaptive responses in the child-parent relationship, researchers have more recently applied attach- ment theory to adult love relationships. Both the child and adult literature have explored individual differences in attachment behavior and identified stylistic categories of secure and insecure attachments. Although the insecure categories are characterized by overt behavior which appears quite different (i.e., clinging vs. distance), in adult relationships where attachment is reciprocal, these stylis- tic patterns may achieve a similar function. In this article, I argue that, in adult relationships, insecure attachments reflect strategies for managing a greater level of narcissistic vulnerability than exists in secure attachment. Attachment theory, which has a long-standing history and extensive liter- ature in child development, has recently been extended to investigating adults' love relationships. Both the child and adult literature have ad- dressed qualitative or stylistic differences in how relatively healthy per- sons function in attachment relationships. In the adult literature, researchers have focused on individual differences in secure and insecure categories and have not yet considered how insecure attachments, which seem different from one another, may serve a similar purpose. Such a distinction may be relevant only for adult attachment relationships, in which the partners serve as attachment figures for each other and in which the caregiving and sexual systems are also active in the relationship (Shaver, Hazan, & Bradshaw, 1988). In this article, I link attachment and narcissism to illuminate how different insecure categories of adult attach- ment may be similar despite different overt characteristics. Requests for reprints should be sent to M. Carole Pistole, Department of Educational Psy- chology, Graduate School of Education, Rutgers University, 10 Seminary Place, New Bruns- wick, NJ 08903.
  • 2. 116 PISTOLE ADULT ATTACHMENT Attachment (Bowlby, 1979, 1988) influences development, psycholog cal organization, and adult love (Shaver & Hazan, 1988; Shaver et: al., 19 !8). According to Bowlby, attachment is a behavioral system experienced . s a bond with a particular other who is sensed as a source of security and sal ;ty. As a safe base, the attachment figure facilitates exploration and the qus lity of development throughout the lifespan. More successful exploration re- duces a stronger sense of capability, and this in turn contributes to appro m- ate self-reliance, autonomy, and success in relationships and work. Behavior within attachment relationships is planned and guided b / a cognitive-affective schema or "internal working model" that is origin .11 y constructed in infancy from interactions (Bowlby, 1979, 1988). This mo lei, which also mediates the experiencing and meaning of the relationship, in- cludes (a) expectations about the other's caring and responsiveness, (b) beliefs about the self's worthiness of care and attention, and (c) rules for affect regulation (e.g., in negative or distressing situations; Kobak & Sc& rj, 1988; Mikulincer, Florian, & Tolmacz, 1990). These working models are thought to underlie individual differences in adult attachment style. Stimulated by an interest in love (Shaver & Hazan, 1988) and building on the work of Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall (1978) with infants, Ha :an and Shaver (1987) identified three forms of adult attachment relationshi DS: secure, insecure-anxious-ambivalent, and insecure-avoidant. More rec ;nt research (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), which conceptualized styles logically by crossing positive and negative models of both self and otl er, suggests that a four-style model (secure, preoccupied, avoidant-fearful, ; nd avoidant-dismissing) may more accurately describe adult attachment. I i a comparison of the three and four-style models, Brennan, Shaver, and Tot ey (1991) found systematic correspondence between the frameworks. The c is- cussion in this article is organized along a three-style model because feai : u. and dismissing are both avoidant styles with much in common.1 Briefly, the research indicates that the securely attached are more coi fi- dent and competent in their emotional interactions, more "happy, frienc ly and trusting" (Hazan & Shaver, 1987, p. 515). Preoccupied (or anxious-a n- bivalent) attachment is characterized by clinging and neediness and an intense focus on the partner (Feeney & Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 198 7). Avoidant attachment is distinguished by emotional distance and a comp il- sive self-reliance (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Hazan & Shaver, 198 7). NARCISSISM As introduced by Freud (1914/1961), the term narcissism has been used to describe a variety of clinical phenomena, including the libidinal investmi nt 'Distinctions between fearful and dismissing avoidance are articulated when meaningful.
  • 3. ATTACHMENT 11 7 of the self (Moore & Fine, 1990; Sandier, Person, & Fonagy, 1991). In current usage, the term narcissism, despite theoretical differences between authors, is often used in the context of self-esteem and refers to an aspect of personality, (i.e., of ego organization) that manifests in both healthy and pathological ways (Blanck & Blanck, 1979; Kernberg, 1985; Kohut & Wolf, 1978; Moore & Fine, 1990; Sandier et a l , 1991). Narcissism is related to the cognitive-affective patterning or structuring of the intrapsychic self (Blanck & Blanck, 1979; Kernberg, 1985; Kohut & Wolf, 1978). With a well-patterned or solid intrapsychic structure, the person is able to (a) soothe and comfort self (i.e., regulate esteem internally; Baker & Baker, 1987), (b) sustain goals and relationships (Patton & Robbins, 1982), and (c) value both self and significant others (i.e., there is an even distribution of self-esteem and other-esteem; Blanck & Blanck, 1979; Moore & Fine, 1990). If, however, the self-structure is less patterned or more nondifferentiated, positive valuing of the self and management of esteem functions depend more on others' behaving in ways that support the self— that is, provide valuing, confirming, or comforting functions (Baker & Baker, 1987; Patton & Robbins, 1982). With a more fragile self-structure, the person has more difficulty maintaining an inner sense of comfort and esteem and so is more easily wounded or hurt (i.e., more narcissistically vulnerable). ATTACHMENT AND NARCISSISM Although attachment and narcissism share some theoretical components, they target separate phenomena. Both theories involve cognitive-affective patterning, address affect regulation, and can accommodate healthy as well as pathological development and functioning (see Armstrong & Roth, 1989; Belsky & Nezworski, 1988; Bowlby, 1988; West & Sheldon, 1988). Attach- ment, however, addresses the person's "need for proximity, care, and secu- rity from another who can be experienced as separate from the s e l f (Silverman, 1991, p. 183). Although Bowlby (1988) proposed that the attach- ment system becomes integrated as an aspect of personality, the emphasis of the theory is on interpersonal behavior and its representation. In contrast, narcissism encompasses more general self-regard and undifferentiated or merged aspects of ego organization. When pathological, narcissism ad- dresses a "sense of self lacking sufficient inner resources to give meaning to life simply by living it fully" (Bromberg, 1986, p. 441). Nevertheless, look- ing at adult attachment relationships without reference to narcissism may obscure how attachment patterns are related to esteem and self-protection. In this article, I argue that insecure attachment is characterized by a greater degree of narcissistic vulnerability than secure attachment. Concomitantly, preoccupied and avoidant attachment reflect different strategies for manag- ing vulnerability and self-esteem.
  • 4. 11 8 PISTOLE ATTACHMENT AND SELF-ESTEEM MANAGEMENT All persons experience fluctuations in self-esteem (Kohut & Wolf, 19' 8), but persons with less narcissistic vulnerability are more able "to man igs feelings like inadequacy, weakness, incompetence, or guilt" (Kinston, l c 87, p. 220). Being appropriately self-reliant, experiencing competence and n as- tery in relation to internal standards and goals, is also a way of manag ng esteem (see Elson, 1987; Kernberg, 1985). In addition, self-esteem is reh ted to feelings about one's worth and value (Solomon, 1989). Although the previous components have not been examined directly in adult attachm> nt, research can be construed as supporting the notion that the securely attac led are more capable at managing esteem. In secure attachment, more compel snt affect regulation is suggested by a more frequent occurrence of posii ive emotion (Simpson, 1990), fewer symptoms of distress (e.g., anxiety, ho; til- ity, loneliness; Hazan & Shaver, 1990; Kobak & Sceery, 1988), and gre; ter ego resilience (Kobak & Hazan, 1991; Kobak & Sceery, 1988). Sec ire attachment is also associated with lower levels of self-conscious anxi ;ty (Feeney & Noller, 1990). Other research indicates that the securely attaci ed have more competency or mastery experiences with which to regulate ss- teem by living up to internal standards. Securely attached adults have a 1 ;s:> emotionally permeated approach to goals evidenced by greater satisfact on with work, less difficulty completing tasks, and less fear of failure or rej ac- tion from co-workers (Hazan & Shaver, 1990). In addition, attachment in: lu- ences college students' adjustment arid career maturity (Blustein, Walbrid *e, Friedlander, & Palladino, 1991; Kenny, 1987a, 1987b; Lapsley, Rice, & Fitzgerald, 1990). Secure attachment is also associated with more success rul relationship functioning as demonstrated through longer relationship ife (Hazan & Shaver, 1987); use of an integrating style of conflict resolut on (Pistole, 1989); and higher levels of passion, commitment, and satisfact on (Levy & Davis, 1988). Furthermore, measured in various ways in seve ra studies, self-worth is consistently higher among the securely attached (C j - lins & Read, 1990; Feeney & Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Ryar &. Lynch, 1989). Research suggests, therefore, that secure attachment is disl n- guished by more effective self-esteem management and, by implication, v, th a more solid self-structure and less narcissistic vulnerability. INTERPERSONAL EFFECTS OF NARCISSISTIC VULNERABILITY Narcissistic vulnerability affects the management of adults' love relatii n- ships (Elson, 1987; Solomon, 1989), because the person needs to obt in self-functions from the environment. Relationships that are not driven oy narcissistic vulnerability involve "a mutuality in which the focus on the s ;li
  • 5. ATTACHMENT 11 9 is balanced by recognition of another as a separate, autonomous self (Solo- mon, 1989, p. 47). The self is solid enough that the partner is intellectually and emotionally experienced as different and separate from self (i.e., with separate interests and desires). Although involvement with the partner does heighten self-esteem, esteem enhancement is provided through a sense of mastery or competence including success in the relationship and apprecia- tion of the partner as a way of fulfilling internal values and standards. Research indicates that secure attachment relationships demonstrate a sense of self and the partner as separate. For example, secure attachment has been associated with more positive views of others (Collins & Read, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987), interdependence (Simpson, 1990), intimacy (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Levy & Davis, 1988), trust (Feeney & Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987), and mutuality (Feeney & Noller, 1991). Securely attached adults have reported "being able to accept and support the partner despite the partner's faults" (Hazan & Shaver, 1987, p. 515); and in describing their relationships, they "emphasize the importance of openness and closeness . . . while at the same time seeking to retain their individual identity" (Feeney & Noller, 1991, p. 208). The picture that emerges of secure attachment includes an appreciation of both self and other as well as a capacity for openness and cooperativeness. This description is consistent with others being perceived as separate people and with esteem being distributed between self and other. Narcissistic Use of the Partner More narcissistically based relationships are characterized by the needs of the self assuming a primary importance. The self is more fragile, and esteem is more difficult to manage internally—that is, there exists a greater degree of narcissistic vulnerability (Solomon, 1989). The person is more sensitive to emotional injury, focuses attention more on personal needs than on the partner, and expects partner to behave in affirming and self-enhancing ways. Interactions with the partner are often dictated by the need to stabilize a sense of worth and to regulate feelings, especially negative feelings about self (see Kinston, 1987). More narcissistically vulnerable persons, in adapting, organize defensive structures (i.e., patterns "of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors"; Patton & Robbins, 1982, p. 880) that attempt to cover over or compensate for the vulnerability and thereby protect the self. Rather than regulating esteem needs through an internal self-confirming process, self-regard is accom- plished through a pattern of approaching (e.g., merging; Kohut & Wolf, 1978) or distancing from (Akhtar & Thomson, 1982) significant others. The other person's importance stems more from bolstering or maintaining the self and less from an appreciation of the other in his or her separateness (i.e., likeness and "differentness") from self. For example, the person phenomeno-
  • 6. 120 PISTOLE logically enhances self-esteem through fusion with a partner who is >er- ceived as possessing "all greatness, all power, all esteem, all worth ind value" (Elson, 1987, p. 40). With the partner serving as a selfobject (Kc hut & Wolf, 1978), the power and worth are experienced as belonging to s ;lf. The partner is valued as part of the self (Elson, 1987, p. 40) and "for ths internal functions and the emotional stability" (Baker & Baker, 1987, p 2) he or she augments. Moreover, as a part of the self, the partner is expectei to interact in a way that is congruent with the self's defensive strategies. If the partner does not meet the self's narcissistic needs (e.g., for ck se- ness or distance), then the person is subject to an awareness of differentr sss between self and partner. This incongruence would be experienced a > a separation threat and trigger intense separation anxiety, which would aro lse the attachment system (Bowlby, 1988). The ensuing attachment beha ior might also be contaminated by defensive behavior designed to regulate ; nd protect self (rather than regain security). That is, the needs of the attachm ;nt system would be to experience the partner as either symbolically or ph1 si- cally available. Defensive needs would be to protect the self "from expi ri- encing needs for love, understanding, and validation" (Basch, 1987, p. 31 8). Preoccupied attachment. Preoccupied attachment can be constri ed as a defensive strategy in which narcissistic vulnerability is manaj ec. through merger with the partner. In research, preoccupied romantic relati< >n- ships were characterized by high levels of idealizing the partner and ar extreme approach to love which includes obsessive preoccupation (Peer ey & Noller, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987), hypervigilance to separation, greater distress over separation (Mikulincer et al., 1990), and attending tc distress (Kobak & Sceery, 1988). These characteristics indicate more inti n- sity in attention to partner than is required for interdependency and intim; -y (see Elson, 1987), which indeed are not so well accomplished in preoccup: sd attachment. In addition, the strong clinging and idealized focus on partnei is consistent with gaining affirmation through merger—that is, experienci ig the idealized other, who contains worth and value, as if he or she were a p irt of self, were a selfobject. Moreover, although subtle, language also indicates fusion with the pa :t- ner. In one study, persons with a preoccupied attachment exhibited a higl er level of couple references ("we" vs. "I") associated with the perception of problems in the relationship (Feeney & Noller, 1991). Only when there ; re problems (i.e., incongruence between self and partner) is a "we" (two ps o- ple) versus an "I" (fusion) recognized. Other relationship characteristics also suggest that a component of nee< !i- ness directs the relationship behaviors. Studies have found that preoccupi ;d attachment is characterized by more emotional dependence, a desire i :>r more commitment (Feeney & Noller, 1990), greater reliance on the partn ;r, more use of others as a safe base (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), a id
  • 7. ATTACHMENT 121 inappropriately high levels of self-disclosure (Mikulincer & Nachshon, 1991). Further, persons with a preoccupied attachment experience more emotional ups and downs within the relationship (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). In sum, they seem to "depend on others to maintain positive self regard" (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991, p. 234). The interpretation of clinging preoccupation as a defensive strategy is supported by other research. Preoccupied attachment has been associated with lower levels of esteem (Collins & Read, 1990; Feeney & Noller, 1990), and research indicates that low self-esteem persons use interper- sonal behavior "to enhance their self-affect" (Baumgardner, Kaufman, & Levy, 1989, p. 919). Consistent with this view, Mikulincer et al. (1990) concluded that persons with preoccupied attachments "do not emphasize the caring component in close relationships and their behavior is not motivated by consideration of others' interests" (p. 278). Other research finding lower levels of friendship in their love relationships (Feeney & Noller, 1991) and a control component in their pattern of interpersonal difficulties (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991) also supports the view that preoccupied relationship behavior is motivated by self-sustaining needs. There appears to be an "overwhelming need . . . to simply be in a relation- ship, no matter what or with whom—the primary goal is emotional secu- rity" (Newcomb, 1981, p. 134). Avoidant attachment. In avoidant attachment, narcissistic vulnera- bility is managed through distancing from the partner, thereby, avoiding closeness and intimacy (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Hazan & Shaver, 1987). These relationships are associated with low levels of relying on others, using others as a safe base, romantic involvement (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991), self-disclosure (Bartholomew & Horo- witz, 1991; Mikulincer & Nachshon, 1991), intensity (Bartholomew, 1990; Feeney & Noller, 1990), and higher separation distress than the securely attached (Mikulincer et al., 1990). Further, affect is regulated through dismissing the importance of attachment (Bartholomew & Horo- witz, 1991), dismissing distress (Kobak & Sceery, 1988), directing atten- tion toward nonemotional domains (e.g., work; Hazan & Shaver, 1990), and idealizing self or other (Hazan & Shaver, 1987). The need to wall off or reject a portion of experience (e.g., intense feelings) is indicative of narcissistic vulnerability and a need for partner's cooperation in manag- ing self. Because self-regard is based "on the ability to temporarily tolerate negative affects in order to achieve mastery over threatening or frustrating situations" (Cassidy & Kobak, 1988, p. 304), the defensive function of avoidant strategies leaves the self-structure still vulnerable. An additional indication of narcissistic vulnerability in avoidant attach- ment comes from the functioning of anger in relationships. "Anger and hostility are often instigated by threats to self-esteem of an interpersonal
  • 8. 122 PISTOLE nature" (Kernis, Grannemann, & Barclay, 1989, p. 1013). Perhaps the ho: til - ity associated with avoidant attachment (Bartholomew & Horowitz, IS 31; Kobak & Sceery, 1988) is triggered as a self-protective mechanism that (a) defends against anxiety and negative feelings about self or (b) function to repair damaged self-esteem and preserve a feeling of well-being (Kerni ; et al.( 1989; Solomon, 1989). Self-defense in the relationship is also suggested by the avoidantly at- tached person's endorsement of love as friendship in the absence of a co re- sponding endorsement of romantic love, passion, commitment, or satisfaction (see Feeney & Noller, 1990, 1991; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; L ;y & Davis, 1988). The endorsement of friendship can be interpreted as a me ins of maintaining safer levels of emotional intensity, which is consistent wi h a more fragile self-structure and with using a defensive style rather t lan internal resources to regulate esteem. In a seeming contradiction to this argument, like the securely attached nd unlike fearful avoidants, dismissing avoidants have reported high self es- teem and self-acceptance (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). The uniqt sly high, positive evaluation of self was coupled with a uniquely low leve of subjective distress and with interpersonal problems characterized by ho: til- ity and coldness (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). This constellation of findings, interpreted in conjunction with directing attention away from i is- tress (Kobak & Sceery, 1988) and dismissing attachment needs (B .rt- holomew & Horowitz, 1991), can be construed as indicating a defensiv ily bolstered self. Meaningful self-worth would be accompanied by compete ice in relationship and affect management (Basch, 1988), which is associa :ed with secure but not dismissing attachment. In sum, the primary characteristic of avoidant attachment is avoidance of closeness and ensuing intimacy. The defensive strategy creates a sort of safety in the perceived "detachment" from the partner. Distance facility tes cutting off or never being "touched" by perceived criticism or the experie ice of intense emotions and, thereby, protects a fragile self from being emoti >n- ally overwhelmed with unmanageable emotion. Similarly, the stance of detachment functions to keep away from the self-structure "anything t lat would diminish it" (Akhtar & Thomson, 1982, p. 13). For instance, by distancing, fearful avoidant people hold at bay their fear of intimacy, pro )a- ble rejection, and the self's being overwhelmed with unmanageable emot on (see Bartholomew, 1990). Similarly, the high self-concept of dismiss ng avoidance can be construed as an idealization of self. Distancing ft )m emotional closeness with partner helps ensure that the facade is not pu ic- tured, self-esteem is not injured, and unmanageable emotion is not exp ri- enced. In avoidant attachment, the person protects self against he dangerousness of others (Kinston, 1987). It is as if the persons' "fragile se lse of self will disintegrate" (Modell, 1986, p. 299) or be emotionally o^ er- whelmed or swallowed up (see Kohut & Wolf, 1978) if the partners get cl >se or if feelings are intense.
  • 9. ATTACHMENT 123 CONCLUSION Looking at attachment through the lens of narcissism stimulates making a distinction between appropriate security needs and narcissistic use of the partner to manage self and avoid being hurt. In preoccupied attachment, the defensive strategy is to merge with an idealized other who bolsters feelings of worth. In avoidant attachment, the partner is distanced to maintain self through a behavioral or phenomenological response that strictly avoids closeness and any ensuing intense or negative feelings. One avoidant strat- egy keeps the self contained, closed, passive, and nonassertive; the other strategy protects through idealizing the self and discounting the importance of the attachment system. Although I explored how healthy personalities navigate narcissistic is- sues, attachment style may also be relevant to psychopathology. Fearful avoidance corresponds closely to avoidant personality disorder (Bart- holomew, 1990), and dismissing avoidance is reminiscent of narcissistic personality disorder. The high, defensive self-concept of dismissing attach- ment is similar to the idealized, narcissistic grandiose self; both patterns involve latent vulnerability, coldness, hostility, and using others (American Psychiatric Association, 1987; Kernberg, 1984). Finally, the distinction between attachment and narcissistic needs can be useful to both clinicians and researchers. With relationship issues, therapists can facilitate clients' progress by defining and validating attachment needs and also clarifying how narcissistic needs related to self-regard, self-esteem management, and ego organization are compromising autonomy and inti- macy. Further, because avoidant attachment is associated with hostility, which is in turn associated with pathological aspects of narcissism (see Raskin, Novacek, & Hogan, 1991) and with shame (Tangney, Wagner, Fletcher, & Gramzow, 1992), research investigating associations between attachment style, narcissism, and shame might be productive. Last, research designed to separate aspects of personality organized around specific attach- ment needs from more global aspects of personality organized around needs to manage esteem and defensively protect self would be useful. Investigating adults' attachment behavior under conditions of unexpected separation—that is, when proximity seeking and security needs are active and strongest (Bowlby, 1979, 1988)—may lead to distinctions between attachment and narcissistic vulnerability and thereby enrich the science and practice of psychology. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS A portion of this article was presented at the 1993 Convention of the Ameri- can Psychological Association. I appreciate the helpful comments of Richard De Lisi and the anonymous reviewers.
  • 10. 124 PISTOLE REFERENCES Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C , Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of artachme. t: A psychological study of the strange situation. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Akhtar, S., & Thomson, J. A., Jr. (1982). Overview: Narcissistic personality disorder. Amei can Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 12-20. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental d ^or- ders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author. Armstrong, J. G., & Roth, D. M. (1989). Attachment and separation difficulties in e; ling disorders: A preliminary investigation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8, 4 1 - 155. Baker, H. S., & Baker, M. N. (1987). Heinz Kohut's self psychology: An overview. Amet can Journal of Psychiatry, 144, 1-9. Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy: An attachment perspective. Journal of Si civ.l and Personal Relationships, 7, 147-178. Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test ;>f a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244. Basch, M. F. (1987). The interpersonal and the intrapsychic: Conflict or harmony'' Contei pc- rary Psychoanalysis, 23, 367-381. Basch, M. F. (1988). Understanding psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books. Baumgardner, A. H., Kaufman, C. M., & Levy, P. E. (1989). Regulating affect intcrperson Sly. When low esteem leads to greater enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psyc IOI- ogy, 56, 907-921. Belsky, ]., & Nezworski, T. (Eds.). (1988). Clinical implications of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Blanck, G., & Blanck, R. (1979). Ego psychology 11: Psychoanalytic developmental psychol <gy. New York: Columbia University Press. Blustein, D. L., Walbridge, M. M., Friedlander, M. L., & Palladino, D. E. (1991). Contribut :>ns of psychological separation and parental attachment to the career development proc ;ss. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 39-50. Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. New York: Tavistock. Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base. New York: Basic Books. Brennan, K. A., Shaver, P. R., & Tobey, A. E. (1991). Attachment styles, gender and pare ital problem drinking. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 451-466. Bromberg, P. M. (1986). The mirror and the mask: On narcissism and psychoanalytic growtl hi A. P. Morrison (Ed.), Essential papers on narcissism (pp. 438-466). New York: New ^ ark University Press. Cassidy, J., & Kobak, R. R. (1988). Avoidance and its relation to other defensive processes In J. Belsky & T. Nezworksi (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment (pp. 300-3: 3) Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship qua ity in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644-663. Elson, M. (Ed.). (1987). The Kohut seminars. New York: Norton. Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor of adult romantic relat: )n ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 281-291. Feeney, J. A., & Noller, P. (1991). Attachment style and verbal descriptions of r o m a tic partners. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 187—215. Freud, S. (1961). On narcissism: An introduction. London: Hogarth. (Original work publis iec 1914) Hazan, C , & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Jout iai of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511-524. Hazan, C , & Shaver, P. R. (1990). Love and work: An attachment-theoretical perspect ipe
  • 11. ATTACHMENT 125 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 270-280. Kenny, M. E. (1987a). The extent and function of parental attachment among first-year college students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 17-29. Kenny, M. E. (1987b). Family ties and leaving home for college: Recent findings and im- plications. Journal of College Student Personnel, 28, 438-442. Kernberg, O. F. (1984). Severe personality disorders. New Haven, CT: Yale University. Kernberg, 0 . F. (1985). Borderline conditions and pathological narcissism. Northvale, NJ: Aronson. Kernis, M. H., Grannemann, B. D., & Barclay, L. C. (1989). Stability and level of self-esteem as predictors of anger arousal and hostility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 1013-1022. Kinston, W. (1987). The shame of narcissism. In D. L. Nathanson (Ed.), The many faces of shame (pp. 214-245). New York: Guilford. Kobak, R. R., & Hazan, C. (1991). Attachment in marriage: Effects of security and accuracy of working models. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 861-869. Kobak, R. R., & Sceery, A. (1988). Attachment in late adolescence: Working models, affect regulation, and representations of self and others. Child Development, 59, 135-146. Kohut, H., & Wolf, E. S. (1978). The disorders of the self and their treatment: An outline. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 59, 413-425. Lapsley, D. K., Rice, K. G., & Fitzgerald, D. P. (1990). Adolescent attachment, identity, and adjustment to college: Implications for the continuity of adaptation hypothesis. Journal of Counseling and Development, 68, 561-565. Levy, M. C , & Davis, K. E. (1988). Lovestyles and attachment styles compared: Their relations to each other and to various relationship characteristics. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 5, 439-471. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Tolmacz, R. (1990). Attachment styles and fear of personal death: A case study of affect regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 273—280. Mikulincer, M., & Nachshon, O. (1991). Attachment styles and patterns of self-disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61. 321-331. Model], A. H. (1986). A narcissistic defence against affects and the illusion of self-sufficiency. In A. P. Morrison (Ed.), Essential papers on narcissism (pp. 293-307). New York: New York University. Moore, B. E., & Fine, B. D. (1990). Psychoanalytic terms and concepts. New Haven, CT: The American Psychoanalytic Association and Yale University Press. Newcomb, M. D. (1981). Heterosexual cohabitation relationships. In S. Duck & R. Gilmour (Eds.), Personal relationships 1: Studying personal relationships (pp. 131-164). New York: Academic. Patton, M. J., & Robbins, S. B. (1982). Kohut's self-psychology as a model for college-student counseling. Professional Psychology, 13, 876-888. Pistole, M. C. (1989). Attachment in adult romantic relationships: Style of conflict resolution and relationship satisfaction. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 6, 505-510. Raskin, R., Novacek, J., & Hogan, R. (1991). Narcissistic self-esteem management. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 911-918. Ryan, R. M., & Lynch, J. J. (1989). Emotional autonomy versus detachment: Revisiting the vicissitudes of adolescence and young adulthood. Child Development, 60, 340-356. Sandier, J., Person, E. S., & Fonagy, P. (Eds.). (1991). Freud's "On narcissism: An introduc- tion." New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Shaver, P., & Hazan, C. (1988). A biased overview of the study of love. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 5, 473-501. Shaver, P., Hazan, C , & Bradshaw, D. (1988). Love as attachment: The integration of three behavioral systems. In R. Sternberg & M. Barnes (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 68-99). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  • 12. 126 PISTOLE Silverman, D. K. (1991). Attachment patterns and Freudian theory: An integrative prop >sal. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 8, 169-193. Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on romantic relationships. Journ, I of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 971-980. Solomon, M. F. (1989). Narcissism and intimacy. New York: Norton. Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., Fletcher, C , & Gramzow, R. (1992). Shamed into anger? The reh :ion of shame and guilt to anger and self-reported aggression. Journal of Personality and S( -id Psychology, 62, 669-675. West, M., & Sheldon, A. E. R. (1988). Classification of pathological attachment patterr ; in adults. Journal of Personality Disorders, 2, 153-159.