SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 100
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
ASSESSING AN INTERGENERATIONAL HORTICULTURE THERAPY PROGRAM 
FOR ELDERLY ADULTS AND PRESCHOOL CHILDREN 
by 
Mary Lorraine Predny 
Thesis submitted to the Faculty of 
Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University 
in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of 
MASTER OF SCIENCE 
in 
HORTICULTURE 
____________________________ 
Dr. P. Diane Relf 
Committee Chair 
____________________________ ____________________________ 
Dr. J. Roger Harris Dr. Andrew J. Stremmel 
KEY WORDS: Volunteers, Staff, Video, Activities, Day care
ASSESSING AN INTERGENERATIONAL HORTICULTURE THERAPY PROGRAM 
FOR ELDERLY ADULTS AND PRESCHOOL CHILDREN 
by 
Mary Lorraine Predny 
Dr. Diane Relf, Chair 
Horticulture Department 
ABSTRACT 
The goal of this research project was to determine if introducing intergenerational interactions would 
supplement or detract from the use of horticulture as a therapeutic tool when working with elderly 
adults and preschool children. The program was set up to compare independent group activities with 
intergenerational activities. A group of elderly adults in the University Adult Day Service and a group 
of preschool children in the University Child Development Laboratory School took part in both 
separate age group and intergenerational activities. There were three sessions each week: one for the 
children’s group, a second one for the elderly adults’ group, and a third one that combined both 
groups. The same activity was done during all three sessions each week, with modifications to make 
the activity appropriate for each age group and to make it more interactive for the intergenerational 
group. These activities took place in the campus building where the day care centers are located. Four 
volunteers assisted with the activities. Two worked with the children’s group both during separate 
and intergenerational activities, and two volunteers similarly assisted with the elderly adult group. 
Video cameras were used to record each session. These videos were viewed and evaluated after the 
10-week horticulture therapy program was completed to score attendance and participation during 
separate age group activities, and attendance, participation, and interaction between the two groups 
during intergenerational activities. This data was used to determine if introducing intergenerational 
interactions affected the individual’s attendance or participation, and to determine if the interactions 
between the two groups showed any change over time.
Several variables were shown to affect the outcome of research. The first variable discussed is the 
effect of the staff, volunteers, or administration on the participants and the activities. Staff and 
volunteers can greatly affect intergenerational interactions by: 1) failing to encourage participation 
from participants of all ages, 2) lacking experience or having discomfort in working with special 
populations, 3) failure to establish adequate communication with the researcher or with each other, 
or 4) demonstrating a negative attitude towards the project. The second variable in research is the 
limitation introduced by data analysis using video. While video recording is useful in evaluating data, 
it can cause problems due to a limited viewing area, limited viewing angles, blocked screens, or 
unfamiliarity with recording equipment. 
Videos were used to assess participation and interaction. Participation scores include three categories: 
“no participation” for present but inactive participation, and “working with direct assistance” or 
“independent participation” for active participation. Participation was affected by the horticulture 
activities’ set up, difficulty level, and availability of assistance from volunteers. Children’s 
participation during separate group activities was affected mainly by the difficulty level and set up of 
activities. Elderly adult’s participation during separate age group activities was affected mainly by 
each individual’s abilities and availability of assistance. Children’s intergenerational participation 
scores show an increase in the category of “working with direct assistance”, while elderly adults’ 
intergenerational scores show an increase in the categories of “no participation” and “independent 
participation”. In part, the change in intergenerational participation was due to a decrease in the 
assistance available from volunteers for each individual. 
Lastly, the percentage of total interaction time between the generations during activities increased 
over time. However, the introduction of intergenerational interactions detracted from the use of 
horticulture as a therapeutic tool for elderly adults and preschool children. It is recommended that 
intergenerational programming may not be useful to fulfill specific horticulture therapy goals for these 
groups. At the same time, the intergenerational activities involving horticulture plant-based activities 
were more successful at increasing interactions than the craft-type activities. Therefore horticulture 
iii
may be a useful activity for intergenerational programs with a goal of increased interaction and 
relationship development. 
iv
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 
The completion of this research project and thesis could not have been possible without the assistance 
and support of many individuals. Thanks to my advisor Dr. Diane Relf for her enthusiasm, guidance, 
and constructive criticism which helped turn a good idea into a rewarding project. Her innovative 
ideas, resourcefulness, and practical solutions made it easier to face the many challenges that arose 
throughout the course of this project. I also thank my committee members Drs. Roger Harris and 
Andrew Stremmel for their advice and support throughout this study. 
I am grateful to the Virginia Tech Adult Day Service and the Child Development Lab School for 
the use of their facilities, their patient and advising staff, and most of all for allowing me the pleasure 
of working with their clients, many of whom made this research project an incredibly enjoyable and 
rewarding experience. 
I owe many thanks to the four horticulture therapy volunteers who offered their time and energy, the 
Virginia Tech Greenhouses, Floral Design Lab, and Dave Angle for donating horticultural supplies, 
the staff in the Office of Consumer Horticulture who read and revised this manuscript, and Alan 
McDaniels for his statistical consultations. Without their assistance this project would not have been 
successful. 
Lastly, I extend my infinite love and appreciation to my family and friends who offered me the 
encouragement, advice, and support that gave me the strength and courage to follow my dreams, and 
the love and happiness to enjoy life along the way. 
v
TABLE OF CONTENTS 
I. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 
II. Perspectives on Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 
Horticulture Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 
Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 
Intergenerational Programming Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 
Implementing a Program . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 
Gardening for Elderly Adults . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 
Gardening for Children . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 
Comparison . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 
Literature Cited . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 
Additional References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 
III. Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy Research Variables 
Introduced by Staff, Volunteers, and Video . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 
Working with Staff and Volunteers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 
Video . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 
Literature Cited . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21 
IV. Intergenerational Interactions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 
Materials and Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 
Results and Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 
Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 
Guidelines . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 
Recommendations for Future Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 
Literature Cited . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 
V. Horticulture Therapy Activities for Preschool Children, 
Elderly Adults, and Intergenerational Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 
Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 
Activity Goals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 
Testing of Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 
Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41 
Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 
Elderly Adult Activity References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 
Preschool Children Activity References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 
Intergenerational Activity References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 
vi
vii 
VI. Appendices 
Appendix A: Separate Age Group Activity Participation Chart - Children . . . . . . . . . . . 55 
Appendix B: Separate Age Group Activity Participation Chart - Elderly Adults . . . . . . 58 
Appendix C: Intergenerational Activity Participation Chart - Children . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 
Appendix D: Intergenerational Activity Participation Chart - Elderly Adults . . . . . . . . . 65 
Appendix E: Total Participation - Children . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 
Appendix F: Total Participation - Elderly Adults . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 
Appendix G: Total Separate Age Group Activity Participation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 
Appendix H: Total Intergenerational Activity Participation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 
Appendix I: Intergenerational Interaction Chart - Children . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 
Appendix J: Intergenerational Interaction Chart - Elderly Adults . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 
Appendix K: Total Individual and Activity Intergenerational Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . 92
INTRODUCTION 
The chapters in this thesis were written as separate articles to be submitted to different journals for 
publication. Because of this, there is some repetition among the chapters which is necessary to clarify 
the information within them. Even though each article results from the same research study, they 
address different issues and do not repeat results and conclusions. 
The first two articles, “Perspectives on Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy” and 
“Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy Research Variables Introduced by Staff, Volunteers, and 
Video” will be submitted to the Journal of the American Horticulture Therapy Association. 
The third article, “Intergenerational Interactions” will be submitted to HortTechnology. 
The last article, “Horticulture Activities for Preschool Children, Elderly Adults, and Intergenerational 
Groups” will be submitted to Activities, Adaptation, and Aging. 
1
PERSPECTIVES ON INTERGENERATIONAL HORTICULTURE THERAPY 
Horticultural Therapy 
Historically, horticulture has proven to be an effective means of therapy for many different 
populations. Horticultural Therapy (HT) generally refers to the use of horticultural activities that are 
adapted to meet specific goals of an individual with special needs in treatment (Relf, 1997). Many 
individuals benefit from these activities, including elderly adults, disabled children, mentally and 
physically disabled adults, or other individuals who could benefit from participating in horticultural 
activities, but who require special adaptations or modifications to do so (Relf and Dorn, 1995). 
Horticulture therapy programs are usually led by a professional who is trained to “tailor the use of 
plants to fit the therapy and rehabilitation needs of those individuals with whom they are working” 
(AHTA publication). Although the specific needs of any particular population and even the needs of 
several individuals within a certain population may vary, the main goals of a HT program remain the 
same. As Relf states, “the specific goals toward which a HT program is directed may differ distinctly 
from one institution to another and from one population to another. However, the ultimate goal of 
these programs is the improved physical and mental health of the individual” (Relf [Hefley], 1973). 
Horticulture has become a valuable therapeutic tool because the activities can easily be adjusted and 
adapted to meet the needs of any specific population without altering the main objectives of the 
program. 
Intergenerational Horticultural Therapy 
Recently, intergenerational HT programs have been used in order to add to a horticulture program 
the opportunity for diverse social interactions for special groups that could benefit from such 
experiences (Abbott et al., 1997; Epstein and Greenberger, 1990; Kerrigan and Stevenson, 1997). 
Intergenerational HT programs have not been widely used or studied, so the impacts of such 
programs are unknown. Non-horticultural intergenerational programs have been studied in various 
2
situations over the past several years, but the results of these studies are mostly inconclusive or 
contradictory (Dellman-Jenkins et al., 1991; Seefeldt, 1989). It is important, therefore, to study these 
interactions to determine if they could become an asset to HT programming. 
Intergenerational Programming Research 
Generations United, a group formed in the mid 1980s to promote intergenerational activities, defines 
intergenerational programming as “the purposeful bringing together of different generations in 
ongoing planned activities designed to achieve the development of new relationships as well as 
specified program goals” (in Ventura-Merkel et al., 1989). The idea of intergenerational exchanges 
emerged in the 1960s with programs such as ‘Adopt a Grandparent’ and other similar programs that 
connected young, school-aged children to elderly adults (Newman, 1989). These programs were 
mainly started in response to decreased contact between the generations. Despite the increasing 
population of elderly persons in our society, young people have infrequent or ineffectual connections 
with them due to a “breakdown of the extended family network [and] increased ageism and age 
segregation” (Cohon, 1989). Sally Newman, founder of the University of Pittsburgh’s Generations 
Together program developed to research intergenerational programs, states that “for our elderly, 
there has been (an observed) decline in self-esteem and self-worth, and an increase in feelings of 
loneliness. For our children and youth, there has been an observed loss of the traditional elder/child 
nurturing, a loss of cultural and historical connections, and an increase in their fear of aging. Age 
segregation, furthermore, seems to have resulted in an increase in myths and stereotypes between the 
young and the old” (Newman, 1989). The goal of intergenerational programs has been to alleviate 
the isolation and negative attitudes that result from a lack of contact between these groups. These 
programs also serve to expose individuals to the diversity of human life in order to promote 
understanding and acceptance of the differences that exist between the generations. “When we learn, 
work and play only with our age-peers, we begin to accept a homogenous view of the world as our 
version of reality. This leads not only to a shallow, one-dimensional view of how things are, but 
inevitably begins to limit the possibilities of how they can become” (Tice, 1985). 
3
Intergenerational contact is seen as equally important for all generations, even though the benefits for 
each group varies. Although these programs can refer to interactions between any generations, most 
studies have focused on exchanges between elderly adults and young children. These groups are 
assumed to receive the greatest benefit from intergenerational experiences due to their limited contact 
with each other in everyday life and their similar status as ‘dependent’ in society. 
Today’s elderly adult population experiences fewer social contacts and increased isolation due to the 
negative perceptions of younger populations (Cohon, 1989). Developing positive relationships with 
the younger generation are reported to increase feelings of self-esteem and life satisfaction, while 
decreasing isolation and loneliness for elderly adults in our society (Seefeldt, 1989). A study by 
Pastorello et al. “noted that institutionalized elderly reported not only less loneliness and depression 
but feelings of youthfulness following volitional interaction with preschool children” (in Kocarnik and 
Ponzetti, 1991). 
For children, changes in family structure and a loss of connection with their grandparents may cause 
a lack of continuity (Chamberlain et al., 1994) and misunderstandings or misconceptions about elderly 
adults and the aging process (Seefeldt, 1989). Exchanges with the older generation are said to 
“influence moral and personal development of the maturing child” (Cohon, 1989) and help the 
younger generation shape their value systems “by seeing their linkages to the past” (Chamberlain et 
al., 1994). Not only do relationships with elderly adults help to shape morals and values and prevent 
negative stereotypes about aging, but a study by Kerschner and Harris also indicated that the 
“children often thrive on the individualized attention the seniors can provide” (in Kocarnik and 
Ponzetti, 1991). 
Not all studies on intergenerational experiences have shown these positive effects. Seefeldt (1987) 
cites several studies of programs that had inconclusive or negative effects on the participants. In a 
later study, a group of preschoolers that visited infirm elderly adults actually showed an increase in 
their negative attitudes toward their own aging (Seefeldt, 1989). Seefeldt (1987) hypothesizes that 
“differing research methodologies, samples, and types of programs and contacts may account for 
4
these inconsistent findings.” Previous studies indicate possible reasons for these negative results and 
solutions that may prevent undesirable experiences. These include making the contact beneficial for 
both groups rather than having one group serve the other (Tice, 1985), designing projects that “have 
a definite purpose or end product” (Aday et al., 1991), establishing programs that are long-term so 
the individuals have a chance to establish relationships with one another (Seefeldt, 1989), and 
ensuring that positive attitudes develop as a result of discovered similarities between the groups 
(Chapman and Neal, 1990). Seefeldt (1987) proposed recommendations for ensuring successful 
programs that reinforce these points and include “protecting the prestige of elders as well as children; 
limiting frustration for both adults and children by arranging for contact that is intimate, not casual; 
planning for interaction that has integrity and is functional for both groups; and ensuring that contact 
between old and young is rewarding and pleasant for both groups.” 
Implementing a Program 
According to these guidelines, HT has excellent potential as a focus for intergenerational interaction 
because it provides benefits for all persons involved, a definite end product to share and discuss, a 
long-term project with intrinsic rewards, and a common interest in order to establish a connection 
between the groups. The horticultural activities could also be adapted to meet the needs of both 
groups involved. However, it is not as certain that intergenerational interactions would complement 
a HT program. For example, the specific goals of gardening with elderly adults and gardening with 
children have some conflicting objectives that may make the combination of the two groups 
inappropriate. 
Gardening for Elderly Adults 
According to past studies, gardening is one of the preferred leisure activities for elderly adults 
(Burgess, 1990; Hill and Relf, 1983; Relf, 1989). Horticulture can offer many opportunities for 
exercise and socialization for elderly persons who are learning to deal with limitations such as sensory 
loss, physical decline, and loss of status in society (Carstens, 1985). Many tools and activities can be 
5
adapted to meet the needs of individuals with disabilities such as arthritis, cerebral palsy, Alzheimer’s 
disease, and dementia (Bubel, 1990; Kerrigan and Stevenson, 1997; Whittier, 1991). Some research 
even shows that gardening can alleviate specific psychological problems (Langer and Rodin, 1976; 
Rodin and Langer, 1977). According to Haas et al. (1998), “health conditions affect an older person’s 
ability to perform a range of common activities needed for personal self-maintenance and independent 
community residence.... Rehabilitation and supervised care that includes HT can help reduce personal 
losses and restore an older person’s level of functioning.” Some possible benefits include improved 
quality of life, control and independence, a greater sense of personal responsibility and autonomy, 
social interaction, mental stimulation, sensory stimulation, decreased boredom, creative expression, 
stimulation of long- and short-term memory, and physical exercise on various levels (Burgess, 1990; 
Carstens, 1985; Haas et al., 1998; Hill and Relf, 1983; Langer and Rodin, 1976; Rodin and Langer, 
1977; Rothert and Daubert, 1981). Studies indicate that elderly adults prefer gardens that contain 
popular plants from their youth, produce that they can enjoy, and a place that is clean and well-kept 
where they can relax in peace and quiet (Mooney, 1994). 
Gardening for Children 
Children’s gardening as a means of environmental education has gained popularity in recent years and 
is now supported by many schools, botanic gardens and arboreta, cooperative extension, and other 
agencies or groups (Relf and Dorn, 1995). Many studies show that environmental attitudes are 
formed at a young age, so it is important to encourage positive views of nature early in a child’s 
development (Eberbach, 1990; Wilson, 1995, 1996). Using hands-on gardening while teaching 
principles of environmental conservation and basic science can be very effective because children 
learn mainly through physical contact and manipulation of the world around them (Eberbach, 1988, 
1990; Moore, 1996; Palmer, 1994; Straw, 1990; Wilson, 1995). Horticulture can also be used to 
teach math, art, history, language skills, social studies, and literature (DeMarco, 1997). Adapting 
horticultural activities to meet the needs and stages of development can “appeal to a child’s interest, 
encourage experimentation in the physical world, and foster perspective taking and cooperation in 
the social world” (DeVries and Zan, 1995). The outdoors can also offer a “greater sense of freedom” 
6
and “more unrestricting play activities” to support the importance of play in a child’s development 
and learning (Henniger, 1994). According to Wilson, “once children learn to respect and love the 
world of nature, they will be ready and eager to learn more about the scientific aspects of the world 
around them” (1995). Some additional goals of gardening with children of any age include developing 
patience, perseverance, reverence, responsibility, cooperation, physical health, good work habits, 
motivation for learning, confidence, empathy, a sense of wonder and excitement, and respect and 
appreciation for nature (Bunn, 1986; Green, 1994; Waters, 1993; Wilson, 1995). Children’s gardens 
should be designed to offer freedom to move and play, plants that are interesting and exotic, and 
“leftover, wild places that they have the freedom to manipulate. They tend to be wild, messy places” 
(Hart, 1993). 
Comparison 
The needs of elderly adults and children that are addressed by HT have many similarities including 
increased autonomy, a sense of wonder and excitement, physical and mental stimulation, social 
interactions, sensory stimulation, and creative expression. However, the conflicting energy levels of 
the two groups, and the opposing needs for quiet and cleanliness for elderly adults and freedom to 
explore and play for children, may result in frustrations and negative attitudes between the two 
groups. 
Literature Cited 
Abbott, G., V. Cochran, and A.A. Clair. 1997. Innovations in intergenerational programs for persons 
who are elderly: The role of horticultural therapy in a multidisciplinary approach, p. 27-38. 
In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the Older Adult. Hawthorne Press, NY. 
Aday, R.H. 1991. Youth’s attitudes toward the elderly: The impact of intergenerational partners. J. 
7 
Applied Gerontology 10(3):372-384. 
American Horticultural Therapy Association, A Career in Horticultural Therapy, unpublished. 
Bubel, N. 1990. A therapy garden. Country Journal (September/October):74-76. 
Bunn, D.E. 1986. Group cohesiveness is enhanced as children engage in plant stimulated discovery 
activities. J. Therapeutic Hort. 1:37-43. 
Burgess, C.W. 1990. Horticulture and its application to the institutionalized elderly. Activities, 
Adaptation and Aging 14(3):51-61.
Carstens, D.Y. 1985. Site Planning and Design for the Elderly: Issues, Guidelines, and Alternatives. 
8 
Van Nostrand Reinhold Co., NY. 
Chamberlain, V.M., E. Fetterman, and M. Maher. 1994. Innovation in elder and child care: An 
intergenerational experience. Educ. Gerontology 20:193-204. 
Chapman, N.J. and M.B. Neal. 1990. The effects of intergenerational experiences on adolescents and 
older adults. The Gerontologist 30(6):825-832. 
Cohon, D. 1989. Intergenerational program research to refine theory and practice. J. Children in 
Contemp. Society 20:217-230. 
Dellman-Jenkins, M., D. Lambert, and D. Fruit. 1991. Fostering preschoolers’ prosocial behaviors 
toward the elderly: The effect of an intergenerational program. Educ. Gerontology 17:21-32. 
DeVries, R. and B. Zan. 1995. Creating a constructivist classroom atmosphere. Young Children 
50(9):4-13. 
DeMarco, L.W. 1997. The Factors Affecting Elementary School Teachers’ Integration of School 
Gardening into the Curriculum, unpublished thesis. Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State 
University. 
Eberbach, C. 1988. Garden Design for Children, unpublished thesis. U. Of Delaware. 
Eberbach, C. 1990. Children’s gardens: The meaning of place, p. 80-83. In: P.D. Relf (ed.). The Role 
of Horticulture in Human Well-Being and Social Development: A National Symposium. 
Timber Press, Portland, OR. 
Epstein, S.G. and D.S. Greenberger. 1990. Nurturing plants, children, and older individuals: 
Intergenerational horticultural therapy. J. Therapeutic Hort. 5:16-19. 
Green, K. 1994. Encouraging nurturing behavior of two to seven year olds by introducing plants and 
flowers, p. 395-408. In: J. Flagler, and R. Poincelot (eds.). People-Plant Relations: Setting 
Research Priorities, A National Symposium. Hayworth Press, NY. 
Haas, K., S.P. Simson, and N.C. Stevenson. 1998. Older persons and horticultural therapy practice, 
p. 231-255. In: S.P. Simson and M.C. Straus (eds.). Horticulture as Therapy: Principles and 
Practice. Haworth Press, NY. 
Hart, R. 1993. Kids need wild places, gentle guidance. Amer. Horticulturalist 72(11):3. 
Henniger. 1994. Planning for outdoor play. Young Children 49(4):10-15. 
Hill, C.O. and P.D. Relf. 1983. Gardening as an outdoor activity in geriatric institutions. Activities, 
Adaptation and Aging 3(1):47-54. 
Kerrigan, J. and N.C. Stevenson. 1997. Behavioral study of youth and elders in an intergenerational 
horticultural therapy program, p. 141-154. In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the 
Older Adult Population. Hawthorne Press, NY. 
Kocarnik, R.A. and J.J. Ponzetti. 1991. The advantages and challenges of intergenerational programs 
in long-term care facilities. J. Gerontological Social Work 16(½):97-107. 
Langer, E.J. and J. Rodin. 1976. The effects of choice and enhanced personal responsibility for the 
aged: A field experiment in an institutional setting. J. Personality and Social Psych. 34(2):191- 
198. 
Mooney, P.F. 1994. Assessing the benefits of a therapeutic horticulture program for seniors in 
immediate care, p. 173-194. In: M. Francis, P. Lindsey, and J.S. Rice (eds.). The Healing 
Dimensions of People-Plant Relations. Center for Design Research, Davis, CA. 
Moore, R.C. 1996. Compact nature: The role of playing and learning gardens on children’s lives. J.
9 
Therapeutic Hort. 8:72-82. 
Newman, S. 1989. A history of intergenerational programs J. of Children in Contemp. Society 20:1- 
15. 
Palmer, J.A. 1994. Acquisition of environmental subject knowledge in preschool children: An 
international study. Children’s Environments 11(3):204-211. 
Relf (Hefley), P.D. 1973. Horticulture- a therapeutic tool. J. Rehab. 39(1):27-29. 
Relf, P.D. 1978. Horticulture as a recreational activity. Am. Health Care Assn. J. 4(5):68-70. 
Relf, P.D. 1989. Gardening in Raised Beds and Containers for the Elderly and Physically 
Handicapped. Virginia Cooperative Extension publication. 
Relf, P.D. 1997. Defining Horticultural Therapy, unpublished. 
Relf, P.D. and S. Dorn. 1995. Horticulture: Meeting the needs of special populations. 
HortTechnology 5(2):94-103. 
Rodin, J. and E.J. Langer. 1977. Long-term effects of a control-relevant intervention with the 
institutionalized aged. J. Personality and Social Psych. 35(12):897-902. 
Rothert, E.R. and J.R. Daubert. 1981. Horticultural Therapy for Nursing Homes, Senior Centers, 
Retirement Living. Chicago Horticultural Society, Chicago, IL. 
Scheid, D.T. 1976. An Approach to Teaching Children About the Aesthetics of Plants and Gardens, 
unpublished thesis. U. Of Delaware 
Seefeldt, C. 1987. Intergenerational programs. Childhood Educ. (October):14-18. 
Seefeldt, C. 1989. Intergenerational programs: impact on attitudes. J. of Children in Contemp. 
Society 20:185-194. 
Straw, H. 1990. The nursery garden. Early Child Development and Care 57:109-120. 
Tice, C.H. 1985. Perspectives on intergenerational initiatives: Past, present, and future. Children 
Today 14(5):6-11. 
Ventura-Merkel, C., D.S. Liederman, and J. Ossofsky. 1989. Exemplary intergenerational programs. 
J. of Children in Contemp. Society 20:173-180. 
Waters, M. 1993. Down in the dirt with kids: Tips on raising a crop of young gardeners. Horticulture 
71(3):18-22. 
Whittier, D. 1991. Horticultural activities for physical disabilities of the elderly. NCTRH Newsletter 
6(1):3-5. 
Wilson, R.A. 1995. Nature and young children: A natural connection. Young Children 
(September):4-8. 
Wilson, R.A. 1996. Environmental education programs for preschool children. J. Env. Educ. 
27(4):28-33. 
Additional References 
Angelis, J. 1992. The genesis of an intergenerational program. Educ. Gerontology 18:317-327. 
Bocian, K. and S. Newman. 1989. Evaluation of intergenerational programs: Why and how. J. 
Children in Contemp. Society 20:147-163. 
Browne, C.A. 1994. The role of nature for the promotion of well-being of the elderly, p. 75-79. In:
M. Francis, P. Lindsey, and J.S. Rice (eds.). The Healing Dimensions of People Plant 
Relations. Center for Design Research, Davis, CA. 
Brummel, S.W. 1989. Developing an intergenerational program. J. Children in Contemp. Society 
10 
20:119-133. 
Eberbach, C. 1987. Gardens from a child’s view- an interpretation of children’s art-work. J. 
Therapeutic Hort. 2:9-16. 
Ellis, W.S. 1992. The gift of gardening. National Geographic 181(5):52-81. 
Ezell, D.O., E.V. Jones, and A.P. Olson. 1981. Outdoor Gardening for the Handicapped. Clemson 
University Cooperative Extension Service publication. 
Galvin, E.S. 1994. The joy of seasons: With the children, discover the joys of nature. Young Children 
(May):4-8. 
Gardening Science Manual, New York Board of Education publication. 
Haas, K. 1996. The therapeutic quality of plants. J. Therapeutic Hort. 8:61-67. 
Hamby, A. 1996. Intergenerational Activities: An Observational Study of the Experiences of Children 
and Adults, unpublished thesis. Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University. 
Henkin, N.Z. and S. W. Sweeney. 1989. Linking systems: A systems approach to intergenerational 
programming. J. Children in Contemp. Society 20:165-172. 
Hochstein, R. 1994. Partners in growing. Parents (July):134-138. 
Hoover, R.C. 1994. Healing gardens and Alzheimer’s disease, p. 283-299. In: M. Francis, P. Lindsey, 
and J.S. Rice (eds.). The Healing Dimensions of People-Plant Relations. Center for Design 
Research, Davis, CA. 
Houseman, D. 1986. Developing links between horticultural therapy and aging. J. Therapeutic Hort. 
1:9-14. 
Howell, D.C. 1997. Statistical Methods for Psychology. Duxbury Press, NY. 
Jaus, H.H. 1994. The development and retention of environmental attitudes in elementary school 
children. J. Env. Educ. 15(3):33-36. 
Kaplan, M.J. 1994. Use of sensory stimulation with Alzheimer’s patients in a garden setting, p. 291- 
306. In: J. Flagler, and R. Poincelot (eds.). People-Plant Relations: Setting Research 
Priorities, A National Symposium. Hayworth Press, NY. 
Kuehne, V.S. 1992. Older adults in intergenerational programs: What are their experiences really like. 
Activities, Adaptation and Aging 16(4):49-67. 
Labreque, C. and L. Tremblay. 1996. The evolutive prosthetic garden: A new concept for elderly 
living in nursing facilities. J. Therapeutic Hort. 8:56-60. 
Matsuo, E. 1990. What we may learn through horticultural activity, p. 146-148. In: P.D. Relf (ed.). 
The Role of Horticulture in Human Well-Being and Social Development: A National 
Symposium. Timber Press, Portland, OR. 
Meyer, H.G. 1973. Children grow in gardens. Flower and Garden (March):50-51. 
Moore, B. 1989. Growing with Gardening. Univ. of NC Press, Chapel Hill, NC. 
Moore, R.C. 1993. Plants for Play. MIG Communications, Berkley, CA. 
Moore, S.H. 1981. Horticultural therapy and the aging client. In: NCTRH publication, The 
Comprehensive View of Horticulture and the Aging 1(2):55-59. 
National Gardening Association. 1987. Successful Senior Citizen Gardens. NGA publication. 
Neer, K. 1990. A children’s garden. The Herbalist 56:69-76.
Newman, S. and C. Ward. 1993. An observational study of intergenerational activities and behavior 
change in dementing elders at an adult day care center. Intl. J. Aging and Human 
Development 36:321-333. 
Nordvig, O.K. 1975. Horticultural therapy in public education. California Hort. Journal 36(1):36-37. 
Rae, W.A. and D.A. Stieber. 1976. Plant play therapy: Growth through growth. J. Pediatric Psych. 
11 
1(4):18-20. 
Relf, P.D. 1990. Dynamics of horticultural therapy. Rehab. Lit. 42(5-6):147-150. 
Stremmel, A.J., S.S. Travis, P. Kelley-Harrison, and A.D. Hensley. 1994. The perceived benefits and 
problems associated with intergenerational exchanges in day care settings. The Gerontologist 
34(4):513-519. 
Travis, S.S., A.J. Stremmel, and P.A. Duprey. 1993. Child and adult day care professions converging 
in the 1990s: Implications for training and research. Educ. Gerontology 19:283-293. 
Van Zandt, K. and J.R. Crace. 1981. The role of horticultural therapy in a retirement community. In: 
NCTRH publication, The Comprehensive View of Horticulture and the Aging 1(2):49-54. 
Whiren, A.P. 1995. Planning a garden from a child’s perspective. Children’s Environments 
12(2):250-255.
INTERGENERATIONAL HORTICULTURE THERAPY RESEARCH VARIABLES 
INTRODUCED BY STAFF, VOLUNTEERS, AND VIDEO 
Working with Staff and Volunteers 
Earlier this year a research project was conducted on a horticultural therapy program involving both 
elderly adults and preschool children at day care centers in adjoining facilities. This project focused 
on the interactions between the two groups and the success of horticulture in allowing for meaningful 
intergenerational activities. The design of this project allowed preschool children and elderly adults 
to be observed both during separate age group activities and similar intergenerational activities. Each 
week during the 10-week study included one day for the children’s activity, one day for the elderly 
adults’ activity, and a third day for the intergenerational activity. The horticulture activities aimed to 
meet the needs of both the children and the elderly adults, and to allow for social interaction during 
intergenerational sessions. Although participation fluctuated due to attendance and interest, there 
were an average of 11 children and 7 adults who regularly chose to participate in the horticultural 
activities. Two horticulture student volunteers assisted with the children’s group both during the 
separate and intergenerational activities, and two additional volunteers similarly assisted with the 
elderly adult group. 
During intergenerational activities, older adults and children separated into four small groups each 
led by one volunteer. This design was meant to facilitate supervision of the activities and to encourage 
casual interaction among the participants. Although it was not the intention of this project to study 
the influence of the volunteers on research, this set up allowed for observations that showed the 
success of the intergenerational activities was directly related to the volunteers’ attitudes and 
experience. The staff members at both the Adult Day Service and the Child Development Laboratory 
School also had a noticeable influence on the project although their involvement in the research was 
indirect. At the conclusion of the study several observations were made about factors that influence 
the success of intergenerational HT research projects. Many of these factors related to the staff and 
volunteers’ influence on the participants and activities. Many other HT research programs are also 
12
affected by administrators, other staff members, and volunteers that play a role in the implementation 
and success of activities. In research these interactions can alter the data collected. It is important 
therefore to recognize possible problem areas before implementing HT research so that personnel 
factors can be minimized. 
I will begin by citing several examples of these variables and the effects I observed during my study. 
From my discussions with other researchers I believe that these examples are fairly common, even 
though they have not been widely addressed in HT research. I would then like to suggest possible 
strategies that can be supported by observations made throughout this program which could prevent 
or minimize the variables introduced by other staff or volunteers. In this study the main areas of 
concern involving other staff and volunteers are: 1) their attitudes toward the intergenerational HT 
program, and 2) the interactions between the personnel and the participants. I would like to 
emphasize that although the variables introduced by the numbers and diversity of personnel involved 
in HT research can affect the success of the project, these variables may be limited in order to reduce 
possible negative effects. 
Before this project began I met with both the directors of the Adult Day Service and the Child 
Development Lab to discuss the interest and possibility of conducting HT research between these two 
groups. The general attitudes were very positive for both the horticulture program and the 
intergenerational activities. Both centers are located in the same campus building, separated only by 
a small room designed to allow for intergenerational activities and research. As both centers are part 
of the university they were designed to facilitate research and observation, with observation rooms 
and video equipment for recording activities. 
One main area of concern is the interaction between the personnel and the participants. Both the staff 
members at the Adult Day Service and the Child Development Lab were knowledgeable and 
experienced in working with their own groups and meeting their specific needs during activities. 
However, during intergenerational activities most of these staff members did not interact with the 
other group members present. A lack of experience or possible discomfort in working with a new 
13
group led to a lack of interactions between the staff and the other group present. This not only 
excluded the new group and decreased the social interactions, but the close assistance from the staff 
also reduced the independent involvement and self motivation of the participants during the HT 
activities. Without the encouragement from the staff the older adults and children would not interact 
during activities. For this study, these staff members were asked not to assist so the trained 
horticulture volunteers could conduct their intergenerational groups without distractions or 
disruptions. If time and money allow, a better solution would be offering training for working with 
both groups, or education in both gerontology and child development to alleviate these negative 
effects on intergenerational interaction (Kocarnik and Ponzetti, 1991; Seefeldt, 1987; Stremmel et 
al., 1994; Travis et al., 1993). This could also decrease the negative effects caused by a lack of 
experience. The four horticulture student volunteers who assisted in this project had varying degrees 
of experience with either elderly adults or preschool children. As each intergenerational activity was 
broken down into small groups supervised by each volunteer, it was possible to observe the effects 
of their experience or lack of it. The two volunteers who had more experience were more comfortable 
working with their groups and encouraging participation and interaction. The small groups they 
supervised were more active, and therefore the volunteers concluded that the intergenerational 
program was a success and beneficial for both groups involved. The two volunteers who lacked 
experience working with either elderly adults or children were less likely to encourage participation 
and interaction from either age participants, and therefore concluded that the intergenerational 
program was not successful, or even interesting. 
For intergenerational activities to be successful they must be thoroughly planned and organized. Good 
leadership and encouragement are essential in promoting interactions. Simply placing the two groups 
in a room together and passively observing for interactions will produce poor results. According to 
Kocarnik and Ponzetti (1991), a mediator is needed to help “make sense of the encounter..., initiate 
conversations, respond to questions, facilitate the formation of relationships..., and discuss any 
questions or concerns [the participants] might have.” The role of this mediator is crucial for success, 
as “each friendship requires nurturing and guidance if it is to benefit both the young and the old” 
(Kocarnik and Ponzetti, 1991). 
14
At the same time, this mediator (the volunteer or staff member) must remain focused on the larger 
goals of interaction and relationship development. In a study by Kerrigan and Stevenson (1997), 
intergenerational interaction was decreased when the mediator intervened to make corrections during 
the steps of the activity. The mediator must always keep in mind that achieving the larger goals is 
more important than the specific process or the end product (Kerrigan and Stevenson, 1997). 
Many of these factors that play a role in the overall perception of the research program could be 
prevented or at least minimized to have less of an effect on the outcome of the study. Because the 
staff and volunteers involved in the program are not always credited with the success and because 
human resources can be difficult to find these variables that could potentially be controlled are often 
overlooked. As a result of the large number of staff and volunteers involved in this study additional 
procedures were identified that could improve communication and facilitate research implementation 
and success. 
The first step before implementing any research program is to establish good communication with the 
administrators and staff at the facility where the research will take place. Some important concerns 
that need to be discussed are the facility’s interest and support for HT research, their knowledge of 
research and methodology, flexibility and adaptability in integrating into a research project, their 
philosophies and goals for their clients, their awareness of the constraints and requirements for 
conducting horticulture activities, the available space and resources, scheduling, and any rules or 
regulations that could affect how the research is designed or implemented. The nature of supplies and 
tools used in HT must be clearly explained to staff and administrators (Relf, 1978). 
Having the support of the facility is essential for successful research (Angelis, 1992; Brummel, 1989). 
Without their full support it would be impossible to receive approval from the clients and/or their 
families to conduct the research. As many of the staff members work closely with the participants, 
their attitude can greatly influence the participants’ desire to take part in the HT activities. At some 
facilities this could also influence the resources and funds that could be made available for activities. 
The administration and staff should have a clear understanding of research and methodologies, 
15
including the ability to be flexible and adaptable when integrating with a research program. The 
facility’s attitude toward the program will determine if the HT will be continued after the research 
has been completed. For the well-being of the participants who enjoy the horticulture program and 
for the growth of the field of HT it should always be a goal to find long-term support and interest in 
continuing the program. 
It is necessary to discuss the facility’s philosophies and their goals for the clients before designing the 
research project. The best way to gain support is to design the program to reinforce the center’s 
objectives for the clients. This way the program will be viewed as an additional ally instead of a 
threat. No program will survive if it is perceived as detrimental, counterproductive, or unnecessary 
for the clients’ health or happiness. Because horticulture is so adaptable, many activities could easily 
be designed to meet the needs of both the facility and its participants. Also, the researcher must 
carefully and sensitively adjust procedure so to be least disruptive to ongoing curriculum and 
programming at the facilities. 
The next step is to agree on the space and resources to be used and the time schedule of the project. 
At a center where there are many different programs and activities it is important to understand 
territorial areas. Whether a designated area will be allotted, or whether an area will be shared, discuss 
the requirements and intentions for the area to be used to prevent misunderstandings or frustrations 
with other staff. Scheduling both the frequency and duration of the project and receiving approval 
from all staff will also decrease possible conflicts (Hill and Relf, 1983). Anyone who feels that they 
are losing space, resources, or time to a new program will understandably become agitated. 
Disagreements about or competitions for resources could negatively influence the success of the 
research and limit the support and growth of the program. Rather than feeling that these resources 
are being taken away the staff members must feel that they are allowing them to be used and shared 
by others. This will allow them to feel more secure with their position instead of threatened and 
powerless. 
Lastly, be sure to discuss the rules and regulations of the facility that are designed to protect the 
16
participants’ safety and well-being. Breaking any of these rules, even if the purpose for them is not 
understood, will increase the perceived threat of the research on the participants. This could decrease 
support both for this research and for any future research projects. 
Other than the administration and staff, the volunteers can also affect the success of research. 
Volunteer management is best achieved by good training, communication, and respect. Regardless 
of a volunteer’s past experience, it will be necessary to offer orientation and training before the 
project begins. Screening volunteers and evaluating their personalities will allow for a match of their 
strengths and interests to the necessary tasks. Allowing volunteers to use their strengths and explore 
their interests will decrease frustration, promote initiative and responsibility, and enhance their 
enjoyment of the volunteer experience. Discuss the objectives and goals with all volunteers so that 
the importance and purpose of the research are understood. Provide information and resources for 
working with the specific population, and if possible, allow time for the volunteers to get to know 
the participants. This will increase both their self-confidence and their competence. Discuss both the 
facility’s rules and your own guidelines for working with participants to ensure safety and success. 
Establish a clear list of responsibilities and expectations of the volunteers. Ensure that all volunteers 
understand the guidelines, responsibilities, and expectations before the project begins to prevent 
misunderstandings and confusion. Having volunteers that enjoy their work and are competent with 
the tasks to which they are assigned will reduce frustration both with the volunteer and with the 
researcher. 
Establish comfortable communication with volunteers by emphasizing that their ideas and concerns 
for the project are important and appreciated. In order for any volunteer to enjoy his/her experience 
they must feel valuable. Allowing them to express their opinions will not only reinforce their 
importance to the research, but may also offer new insights and ideas to improve activities. Having 
volunteers keep a journal of the activities will encourage them to evaluate both the program and their 
own performance. Reading these journals will allow for a better understanding of the successes and 
failures that occur. Supporting volunteers with repeated reinforcement and evaluation of their 
progress will allow the volunteers to appreciate their strengths while discovering ways to work 
17
18 
around their weaknesses. 
Respect is an important factor in determining the volunteers’ enjoyment and success. In order for any 
volunteer to be successful he/she must be enthusiastic for the program and enjoy being a part of the 
research. Expressing appreciation for their time and energy and acknowledging their efforts are 
essential for promoting good relations. Lastly, despite the personal satisfaction and enjoyment HT 
can offer through volunteering, demands on time, energy, and emotions can lead to burn out. Some 
volunteers may also discover that they simply do not enjoy horticulture or the clients with whom they 
are working. If this occurs it is best to allow them to leave the program without feelings of guilt or 
resentment. Convincing unhappy volunteers to continue working with the program will be self 
defeating, as their lack of energy and enthusiasm will decrease the success of the activity and their 
participants’ enjoyment. Replacing these volunteers introduces more variables in HT research and 
according to Flagler (1992) it can also upset participants and administrators and decrease continuity 
and stability. However, it may ultimately lead to more successful activities due to the correlation 
between the volunteer’s satisfaction and their performance as a mediator. “Without a competent staff, 
a program will fall apart or stagnate in a monotonous, uninteresting routine” (Brummel, 1989). 
Many of the variables introduced by personnel who are involved in HT research can be controlled 
with careful planning. Most important, simply acknowledging these factors and their effects can 
change how data is collected and interpreted. Working with people is like working with the weather; 
as horticultural researchers must deal with environmental factors beyond their control in the field, 
horticultural therapy researchers must deal with the variables introduced by human nature. When 
working with people, as in working with the weather, one must always be prepared. By understanding 
the necessity for good communication many variables introduced by personnel can be prevented or 
minimized to allow for more successful HT research. 
Video 
The bias introduced by personal attitude and beliefs requires an unbiased method of collecting and
interpreting data. This may be achieved through the use of video recording. Using video offers both 
advantages and disadvantages that need to be considered before implementing research, as these 
obstacles may change how the program is designed. Some factors to be considered are the availability 
of video equipment, knowing how to use the equipment correctly, and the limitations of using this 
equipment. Video has many advantages in data collection and evaluation because it allows an 
unbiased observer to assess the data, it allows more time for assessment, it allows stopping and 
rewinding to clarify actions, and it can be retained for future analysis or teaching purposes. Some 
disadvantages are that it can be expensive, it can be difficult to use correctly, it has time and viewing 
screen limitations, it may not adjust to changes in the activities, and it may not offer adequate clarity 
for assessing data. 
The facility where this research took place had a video system with cameras installed in several 
rooms. A small observation room containing the operation console, viewing screen, and audio control 
was separate from the rooms where the cameras were located. Because this video system was rarely 
used, some of the cameras were blocked by props or furniture and the observation room was utilized 
as a storage closet and copy room. This caused several problems as large boxes and a copy machine 
left little space to access the video controls and almost no space for adjusting controls or observation. 
The videos were set up each day before the HT session. Because the observation room was separate 
from the activity rooms and because the research director was responsible for both the videos and the 
activities, no adjustments could be made to the video’s volume or viewing area after the activities 
began. 
This system was quite useful although not without imperfections. The quality of the videos was 
excellent, while the sound was variable. Despite the excellent quality of the videos, there were some 
problems that affected the visibility of the activity and the participants. The cameras did not cover the 
entire room where the activities took place, so if any participants moved out of the viewing field their 
activity was not recorded. The cameras could not be adjusted to record these participants without 
having additional personnel at the controls, which was not available. Another significant problem was 
created by staff or volunteers standing in front the cameras, not realizing they were blocking all or 
19
part of the room from view. Lastly, the angle of the cameras view was a problem in several 
circumstances where a participant was not facing the camera, and his/her activity and facial expression 
was not observable. 
The sound quality of the video tapes adds several more considerations. Because the volume in the 
room could be adjusted it could be raised so that quiet activities and quiet participants could still be 
heard on tape. At the same time, loud activities in other rooms could also be picked up. When several 
participants are talking at once it becomes difficult to discern one particular voice on tape. Lastly, 
because the volume controls in the activity room were separate from the volume control in the 
observation room, it was easy to forget to turn on the volume for the video resulting in a silent tape. 
As the video cameras in this system were only located in two rooms at the facility, a video camera 
on a tripod was used for outdoor activities. This set up could be used for almost any HT research 
situation. Compared to the indoor video system, the camera on a tripod was easier to operate and 
offered similar results. The disadvantages associated with limited viewing area and blocked screens 
also apply, but because the camera is located in the same room as the activities adjustments easily can 
be made. 
Before starting a research project where video will be used, a pretest of the equipment will be useful 
in indicating possible limitations that may change how the research is designed or implemented. It will 
also be important to train volunteers and staff members who assist with the research to ensure that 
the activities remain within the camera’s view and that blocked screens are minimized. 
Scoring video data can be done in many ways. This research project used participation and interaction 
scores based on charts by Kerrigan and Stevenson (1997) and another by Kuehne (1992). Several 
other charts are available for other observable behaviors. These charts allow an observer to record 
both the amount and duration of behaviors, which can be very useful in quantifying qualitative data. 
These advantages and disadvantages should be considered before choosing video for data collection. 
20
Knowing the possible problem areas and how to avoid or minimize the effects can change how the 
research project is designed or implemented. Knowing how this data will be viewed and by whom is 
also important to ensure that the necessary information is included in the video, and that other 
necessary data that is not seen on tape can be accounted for. Combining video along with journaling 
will offer more complete data assessment, and it may also decrease the severity of problems caused 
by blocked or limited viewing areas, silent or confusing audio quality, or a missed day of recording. 
Literature Cited 
Angelis, J. 1992. The genesis of an intergenerational program. Educ. Gerontology 18:317-327. 
Brummel, S.W. 1989. Developing an intergenerational program. J. Children in Contemp. Society 
21 
20:119-133. 
Flagler, J.S. 1992. Horticulture therapy: Potentials for master gardeners, p. In: P.D. Relf (ed.). The 
Role of Horticulture in Human Well-Being and Social Development: A National Symposium. 
Timber Press, Portland, OR. 
Hill, C.O. and P.D. Relf. 1983. Gardening as an outdoor activity in geriatric institutions. Activities, 
Adaptation and Aging 3(1):47-54. 
Kerrigan, J. and N.C. Stevenson. 1997. Behavioral study of youth and elders in an intergenerational 
horticultural therapy program, p. 141-154. In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the 
Older Adult Population. Hawthorne Press, NY. 
Kocarnik, R.A. and J.J. Ponzetti. 1991. The advantages and challenges of intergenerational programs 
in long-term care facilities. J. Gerontological Social Work 16(½):97-107. 
Kuehne, V.S. 1992. Older adults in intergenerational programs: What are their experiences really like. 
Activities, Adaptation and Aging 16(4):49-67. 
Relf, P.D. 1978. Horticulture as a recreational activity. Am. Health Care Assn. J. 4(5):68-70. 
Seefeldt, C. 1987. Intergenerational programs. Childhood Educ. (October):14-18. 
Stremmel, A.J., S.S. Travis, P. Kelley-Harrison, and A.D. Hensley. 1994. The perceived benefits and 
problems associated with intergenerational exchanges in day care settings. The Gerontologist 
34(4):513-519. 
Travis, S.S., A.J. Stremmel, and P.A. Duprey. 1993. Child and adult day care professions converging 
in the 1990s: Implications for training and research. Educ. Gerontology 19:283-293.
INTERGENERATIONAL INTERACTIONS 
Introduction 
The goal of this research project was to determine if introducing intergenerational interactions would 
supplement or detract from HT goals when working with elderly adults or preschool children. These 
goals included increased autonomy, physical stimulation, mental stimulation, and sensory stimulation. 
Recently, intergenerational HT programs have been used in order to add to a horticulture program 
the opportunity for diverse social interactions for special groups that could benefit from such 
experiences (Abbott et al., 1997; Epstein and Greenberger, 1990; Kerrigan and Stevenson, 1997). 
Intergenerational HT programs have not been widely used or studied, so the impacts of such 
programs are unknown. Intergenerational programs have been studied in various situations over the 
past several years, but the results of these studies are mostly inconclusive or contradictory (Dellman- 
Jenkins et al., 1991; Seefeldt, 1989). It is important, therefore, to study these interactions to 
determine if they could become an asset to HT programming, and conversely to see if horticulture 
is an appropriate activity for intergenerational programming with this population. 
Previous research indicates several guidelines that can lead to more successful intergenerational 
programming. These include making the contact beneficial for both groups rather than having one 
group serve the other (Tice, 1985), designing projects that “have a definite purpose or end product” 
(Aday et al., 1991), establishing programs that are long-term so the individuals have a chance to 
establish relationships with one another (Seefeldt, 1989), and ensuring that positive attitudes develop 
as a result of discovered similarities between the groups (Chapman and Neal, 1990). Seefeldt (1987) 
proposed recommendations for ensuring successful programs that reinforce these points and include 
“protecting the prestige of elders as well as children; limiting frustration for both adults and children 
by arranging for contact that is intimate, not casual; planning for interaction that has integrity and is 
functional for both groups; and ensuring that contact between old and young is rewarding and 
pleasant for both groups.” 
22
According to these guidelines, HT has excellent potential as a focus for intergenerational interaction 
because it provides benefits for all persons involved, a definite end product to share and discuss, a 
long-term project with intrinsic rewards, and a common interest in order to establish a connection 
between the groups. The horticultural activities could also be adapted to meet the needs of both 
groups involved. However, it is not as certain that intergenerational interactions would complement 
a HT program. For example, the specific goals of gardening with elderly adults and gardening with 
children have some different objectives that may make the combination of the two groups 
inappropriate. 
The needs of elderly adults and children that are addressed by HT have many similarities including 
increased autonomy, a sense of wonder and excitement, physical and mental stimulation, social 
interactions, sensory stimulation, and creative expression. However, the conflicting energy levels of 
the two groups, and the opposing needs for quiet and cleanliness for elderly adults and freedom to 
explore and play for children, may result in frustrations and negative attitudes between the two groups 
when focused around a plant/soil based program. 
Materials and Methods 
This research program involved 17 elderly adult clients in the University Adult Day Service and 16 
preschool children between the ages of 3 and 5 in the Child Development Laboratory School. The 
number of participants in the program varied daily due to the health of the clients, the turnover rate 
at the Adult Day Service, and the agreement that the clients should be allowed to exercise autonomy 
and decide independently if they wish to participate in each activity. Both the Adult Day Service and 
the Child Development Lab School are located at Virginia Tech in the same campus building 
separated only by a small room designed to allow for intergenerational interaction. The facilities are 
equipped with observational equipment to allow for research. The intergenerational room and the 
Adult Day Service have video cameras controlled by a central recording system located in a small 
observational room inside the Adult Day Service. The separate children’s and elderly adults’ activities 
took place in the intergenerational room, and the intergenerational activities took place in the Adult 
23
Day Service room which was larger. A video camera on a tripod was used to record activities that 
took place in the enclosed outdoor patio. 
The program was set up to compare separate age group activities with intergenerational activities. 
There were three sessions each week: one for the elderly group, a second one for the children’s 
group, and a third one that combined both groups. The same activity was completed during all three 
sessions in one week, with small modifications to make the activity appropriate for each group. 
Four volunteers assisted with the HT activities. These volunteers were part of a university HT class 
that required volunteer service. They had varying levels of experience with horticulture and working 
with children or elderly adults. Two volunteers assisted the children during separate age group 
activities, and two volunteers similarly assisted with the elderly adult activities. The same four 
volunteers also assisted with the intergenerational activities. 
Ten activities (Chapter V) were chosen based on their adaptability and interest for both the children 
and the elderly adults. Several sources (Gardening Science Manual; Moore, 1989; Moore, 1993; 
Rothert and Daubert, 1981) were consulted to find activities that would appeal to both children and 
elderly adults, and also be appropriate during intergenerational activities. The activities were then 
designed to meet the needs of both groups, making small modifications to the set up or procedures 
when necessary. The activities designed for children and elderly adults to complete separately focused 
on individual projects, while the intergenerational activity was to be completed as a small group effort 
to increase social interaction. The horticulture supplies used in the activities were either collected, 
bought, or donated by the researchers, volunteers, or the university greenhouses and floral design lab. 
Activities were chosen that required few or inexpensive materials. 
The data for each session was collected on video tape over a 10-week period. These video tapes were 
viewed by the researcher after the study was completed to collect data for analysis. Quantitative data 
was collected on the attendance and participation of each individual during both separate age group 
and intergenerational activities. Data also were collected on the interactions between elderly adults 
24
and preschool children during the intergenerational activities. At the completion of the 10-week 
project, the video tapes for all sessions were viewed to collect data. The children’s and elderly adults’ 
separate activities were scored according to attendance and participation. Participation was broken 
down into 3 levels- ‘O’ being present but inactive, ‘I’ being active with direct volunteer assistance, 
and ‘II’ being independent participation. These scores varied with the difficulty level of the activity, 
activity set up, and the individual’s ability. Participation scores were tallied in total times, so the same 
individual may have received “O”, “I”, or “II” for the same activity with varying amounts of time 
spent in each category of participation. 
Attendance percentages were calculated for the elderly adults based on the number of the total 
participants who attended the activity that day. The percentages were not available for the children 
because records of the total participants at the facility that day were not available. Average 
participation time was calculated by dividing the total time of the activity by the number of 
participants. Type of participation percentages were calculated by tallying the total participation times 
for each type of participation, then dividing these numbers by the total time of the activity. 
The video tapes were also used to collect data on interactions between the two groups during 
intergenerational activities. Interaction scores had four categories: ‘I’ being non-verbal interaction, 
‘II’ being one-directional verbal interaction (one participant addressed another without getting a 
response), ‘III’ being two-directional interaction (conversation), and ‘IV’ being two-directional 
interaction with physical assistance during the activity. 
All data were organized into charts to compare separate and intergenerational activities, and to 
determine overall trends that developed during the 10-week period. Inferential statistical analysis was 
not feasible because of the small sample size, the short period of time of the study, and the large 
number of human variables did not allow for a controlled data base. This research can not provide 
definitive answers of the success of intergenerational activities. Rather, it serves to identify variables 
that affect success, trends that might indicate potential success of intergenerational programs, and 
directions for future research. 
25
Results and Discussion 
Children’s Separate Activities 
During children’s separate activities (Table 1), few ‘O’ scores were observed as inactive children 
usually left the room where the horticulture activity took place. ‘I’ or ‘II’ scores were dependent on 
the activity and how it was set up. Activities that were difficult or unfamiliar resulted in high ‘I’ 
ratings as they required the most assistance. Activities that involved familiar tasks such as cutting, 
glueing, drawing, or putting soil in pots required little volunteer assistance and resulted in high ‘II’ 
scores. 
Table 1. Attendance, average time, and percentage of time in three levels of participation for children during separate 
activities. 
Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 
Name Design Seeds I Cuttings Seeds II Terrarium Frames Scare- Corsage Planting 
tags crow 
26 
# 
present 
13 9 15 11 10 14 8 8 11 7 
average 
time in 
minutes 
10m. 14.3m. 10.3m. 8.7m. 7.9m. 7.1m. 14.4m. 14.3m. 6m. 14.1m. 
type of 
partici-pation 
in % 
0- 1% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 
I-73.7 I-26.7 I-75.3 I-100 I-100 I-100 I-8.3 I-28.5 I-100 I-0 
II-25.3 II-73.3 II-24.7 II-0 II-0 II-0 II-91.8 II-71.5 II-0 II-100 
The average participation time for children’s separate activities depended on the set up of the activity. 
Simple activities that required less direct assistance and allowed the children to work independently 
(# 2, 7, 8, 10) also encouraged them to work longer. Activities that involved use of fine motor skills 
(#1, 3) also required longer time for completion and moderate direct assistance. The assistance 
required depended on the children’s abilities and development of fine motor skills. More difficult 
activities, specifically the ordered step activities (# 4, 5, 6, 9) had low average participation times. 
These highly structured activities where volunteers worked one-on-one with the children resulted in 
a shorter period of time needed for participants to complete the activity. 
The number of participants in children’s separate age group activities decreased after the first six
weeks of the study. During these first six weeks, the Child Development Lab School started the day 
with a group time where all the participants sat down and talked about the activities taking place that 
day. During this group time, the children showed a lot of enthusiasm for the horticulture activities and 
the participation was never lower than 70%. However, during the last four weeks of the study, the 
group time was no longer conducted each day. Without an introduction to the horticulture activities, 
it became more difficult to encourage children to take part in the horticulture activities which were 
set up in the room next door. During these last four weeks, the children’s participation was never 
lower than 50% of the children present that day. 
Elderly Adult Separate Activities 
The elderly adult activities (Table 2) had ‘O’ scores because many participants either could not leave 
the table without assistance or because they could not work on their own while the volunteer was 
assisting another participant. ‘I’ and ‘II’ scores were primarily related to the individuals’ physical 
abilities. The few participants with greater physical skills could work alone after a brief demonstration 
of the activity, and scored primarily ‘II’s. Individuals who were not able to work without direct 
assistance due to physical limitations scored mostly ‘I’s. 
Table 2. Attendance, average time, and percentage of time in three levels of participation for elderly adults during separate 
activities. 
Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 
Name Design Seeds I Cuttings Seeds II Terrarium Frames Scare- Corsage Planting 
tags crow 
27 
# 
present 
9/12 5/10 9/10 7/11 7/10 8/11 6/13 5/11 5/8 3/8 
75% 50% 90% 64% 70% 73% 46% 45% 62.5% 37.5% 
average 
time in 
minutes 
25.1m. 26.2m. 19.6m. 31.4m. 18.3m. 15.9m. 24m. 21.6m. 22.2m. 13.7m. 
type of 
partici-pation 
in % 
0-50.4 0-37.4 0-61.6 0-28.9 0-39.3 0-30.2 0-39.3 0-24 0-49.2 0-38.3 
I-36.3 I-57.6 I-36.3 I-49.3 I-54.7 I-69.8 I-51.9 I-58.4 I-50.8 I-61.7 
II-13.1 II-5 II-2.1 II-21.9 II-6 II-0 II-8.8 II-17.6 II-0 II-0 
The elderly adults’ participation was less dependent on how the activity was set up and more 
dependent on each individuals’ physical abilities. Because of this, the activities did not show as much
variation in the scores as the children’s activities. The average participation time varied with the 
amount of materials available for the activity. During activities with unlimited materials available for 
each participant (# 1, 2, 4, 7, 8) many older adults participated longer. Activities where available 
materials were limited (#3, 5, 6, 10) showed a decreased average time as participants were limited 
to the number of products they could complete. The only exception was activity #9. Materials were 
limited for this activity, but the high difficulty level increased the time spent completing the activity. 
Intergenerational Activities 
Participation scores also were used to assess individuals during the intergenerational activities. These 
data allowed for a comparison of the total participation between separate and intergenerational group 
activities to indicate the success of certain activities either in separate or combined groups. 
Intergenerational participation scores showed several changes in participation due to the joining of 
the two groups. These changes in scores appear to have been influenced primarily by a reduction in 
individual assistance available from the volunteers during the activity. 
No data were collected on week 5 because the video equipment was not properly started during that 
intergenerational activity. To compensate for this missing intergenerational data the week 5 separate 
age group activity was not included in comparison data. 
Children’s intergenerational scores (Table 3) show an initial increase in ‘O’s. This is probably due to 
the volunteers’ inexperience at encouraging participation from all participants. These “O” scores 
disappeared after the volunteers gained more experience working with their groups. Children’s scores 
also show an increase in ‘I’s and decrease in ‘II’s compared to separate group activities (Table 1) 
with only two exceptions (#8, 9). This is most likely due to the increased structure of the 
intergenerational activities. In order to encourage interactions between the two groups, activities were 
designed to promote team work and cooperation between the generations. Independent activity 
usually decreased interactions, so participants were encouraged to work together rather than on their 
own and activities were redesigned as group participation efforts. The two exceptions (#8, 9) showed 
an increase in independent activity because of the simplicity of the activities. These activities were less 
28
structured and allowed the participants to work independently but in a group setting. 
Table 3. Attendance, average time, and percentage of time in three levels of participation for children during 
intergenerational activities. 
Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 
Board Sand Seeds I Hanging Labels Terrarium Frames Concrete Arrange- Planting 
Garden Basket ments 
29 
# 
present 
14 12 9 7 ND 7 8 10 5 9 
average 
time in 
minutes 
16.8 9.1m. 15.2 10.1m. ND 9.9m. 12.1m. 18.3m. 13.6m. 15.3m. 
m. m. 
type of 
partici-pation 
in % 
0-5% 0-11.6 0-0.6 0-0 ND 0-0 0-0 0-0 0-0 0-0 
I-95 I-45.1 I-99.4 I-100 I-100 I-100 I-0 I-31.6 I-100 
II-0 II-43.3 II-0 II-0 II-0 II-0 II-100 II-68.4 II-0 
At the same time the children’s average participation time increased for all but two intergenerational 
activities. Because the children were seated at tables and less direct assistance was available, the 
children required more time completing the activities in intergenerational groups. On the days when 
intergenerational activities took place, the activity started before the scheduled group time on that 
day. These activities also took place in the Adult Day Service room, which was two rooms away from 
the Child Development Lab room. It was more difficult to encourage the children to participate in 
activities on these days. Although it is possible that fewer children chose to participate in these 
activities because they did not enjoy the intergenerational group, the high number of variables make 
it difficult to identify the exact reason. Total children’s participation comparing separate age group 
and intergenerational activities are summarized in Table 4. 
Table 4. Total children’s participation comparing cumulative time spent in different levels of participation during separate 
and intergenerational activities. 
0 I II Total Participation Average 
minutes minutes minutes minutes Occurrences minutes 
Separate 1m. 536m. 465m. 1002m. 96 10.4 m. 
Intergenerational 26m. 807m. 273m. 1106m. 81 13.7m.
Elderly Adults 
The elderly adults’ participation scores (Table 5) show an increase in ‘O’ scores in 8 of the 10 
activities compared to separate activities (Table 2), while the other 2 stay the same. This was due to 
two separate observed factors. First, in some situations, ‘O’ scores increase because less direct 
assistance was available from the volunteers, and without the needed assistance, certain of the elderly 
adults could not participate in the activity. This was often observed with elderly adults who had 
limited physical abilities, and for elderly adults with Alzheimer’s disease. The second reason for the 
increase in ‘O’ scores was observed in elderly adults with higher physical capabilities. Many of these 
participants did not join in when children were present because they did not wish to get in the way 
or prevent the children from participating. Many of these elderly adults who did not need direct 
assistance to participate simply sat back and watched the children complete in the activity. 
Table 5. Attendance, average time, and percentage of time in three levels of participation for elderly adults during 
intergenerational activities. 
Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 
Board Sand Seeds I Hanging Labels Terrarium Frames Concrete Arrange- Planting 
Garden Basket ments 
30 
# 
present 
7/11 7/10 8/8 8/9 ND 8/13 10/11 3/12 7/12 7/11 
64% 70% 100% 89% 62% 91% 25% 58% 64% 
average 
time in 
minutes 
24.6m. 16.1m. 17.1m. 10.8m. ND 13.1m. 15.1m. 16.7m. 17.3m. 28.4m. 
type of 
partici-pation 
in % 
0-70.7 0-75.3 0-48 0-70.5 ND 0-37.1 0-61 0-50.3 0-46.7 0-43.6 
I-16.4 I-10.4 I-39.5 I-17 I-50.4 I-19 I-0 I-21.3 I-16.9 
II-12.9 II-14.3 II-12.5 II-12.5 II-12.5 II-20 II-49.7 II-32 II-39.6 
Another observed problem that affected both the elderly adults with and without physical limitations 
was excessive direct assistance for the children from the staff or volunteers. During the first several 
activities, many staff or volunteers worked closely with the active children to keep them involved in 
the activity, and the adults who needed assistance or encouragement to participate were ignored or 
given limited access to the activity. Because the children were more vocal with their assistance 
requests and more willing to leave when they were unsatisfied, the staff and volunteers focused their 
attention on these children and let the elderly adults remain silent and inactive. With careful coaching
and experience in working with both populations and in meeting their needs more efficiently, the 
problems of excessive direct assistance for the children decreased, increasing the assistance and 
attention available for the elderly adults. Throughout the course of the study, the ‘O’ scores 
decreased which may be attributed to the assistance of the volunteers who encouraged the elderly 
adults to work with the children rather than watch, and with the decrease in excessive direct 
assistance for the children. Three of the four early activities (#1, 2, 4) had over 70% ‘O’s, while of 
the last five activities three (#6, 9, 10) showed no more than 47% ‘O’, with two slightly higher scores 
(#7, 8) which were a result of the activity set up. During activity #7, the participants worked 
independently rather than in groups and encouraged interaction and participation was low. During 
activity #8, most elderly adults were unable to reach the wheelbarrow that the children crowded 
around and therefore watched, instead of participated, in the activity. 
Along with the increase in ‘O’ scores, the elderly adults also show an increase in ‘II’s. This is due to 
the limited amount of direct assistance available during the activity. Thus it was observed that without 
direct assistance the ‘I’ scores decrease. The ‘O’ and ‘II’ scores increase because in response to the 
decrease in direct assistance the elderly adults would either not participate or work on their own. 
Total elderly adults’ participation comparing separate age group and intergenerational activities is 
summarized in Table 6. 
Table 6. Total elderly adults’ participation comparing cumulative time spent in different levels of participation during 
separate and intergenerational activities. 
0 I II Total Participation Average 
minutes minutes minutes minutes Occurrences minutes 
Separate 490m. 677m. 117m. 1284m. 57 22.5m. 
Intergenerational 588m. 253m. 293m. 1134m. 65 17.4m. 
Intergenerational Interactions 
During the course of this 10-week study the total percentage of interaction time between the 
generations during activities increased over time (Table 7) with two exceptions (#7, 8). Several 
factors appear to have contributed to this increase. The two groups became more familiar with each 
31
other and more comfortable interacting, and the volunteers became more comfortable working with 
both groups and encouraging participation and interaction from everyone at their table. Most of the 
intergenerational interaction occurred between the 4 and 5 year old children and the elderly adults 
with the highest cognitive and physical abilities. This may be because the 3 year old children and the 
elderly adults with Alzheimer’s disease or other limited cognitive and physical abilities needed a large 
amount of direct assistance to complete the activities, and therefore worked with the volunteers more 
than with the other participants. 
Table 7. Intergenerational interaction for elderly adults and preschool children during intergenerational activities. 
Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 
Board Sand Seeds Cuttings Seeds Terrar- Frames Concrete Arrang- Planting 
garden I II iums ing 
32 
I - non-verbal 
12m. 0m. 0m. 2m. ND 33m. 12m. 0m. 24m. 26m. 
II - one-direction 
verbal 
8m. 15m. 14m. 8m. ND 7m. 2m. 6m. 22m. 16m. 
III - two-direction 
verbal 
4m. 6m. 28m. 4m. ND 0m. 6m. 0m. 2m. 8m. 
IV - two-direction 
verbal + 
physical 
assistance 
0m. 0m. 0m. 8m. ND 0m. 0m. 0m. 2m. 62m. 
Total 24m. 21m. 42m. 22m. ND 40m. 20m. 6m. 50m. 112m. 
Total time 
407.4 221m. 264m 157m. ND 174m. 247.8m. 233m. 189m. 337m. 
for all 
m. 
partici-pants 
Percent of 
interaction 
time 
6% 10% 15% 14% ND 23% 8% 3% 26% 33% 
Several factors that appeared to reduce the amount of interaction in all activities were excessive staff 
or volunteer direct assistance for individuals rather than team or group encouragement, a lack of 
experience or discomfort in working with either elderly adults or children, or inappropriate activity 
set up and material distribution. The low interaction scores of activity #7 and 8 were probably due 
to the nature of the activities. During activity #7, participants worked individually at the same table.
Without a shared product to encourage cooperation very little interaction took place. During activity 
#8, the participants stood around a wheelbarrow to mix concrete and functioned as independent 
workers rather than a team. In addition, many of the elderly adults could not reach the wheelbarrow 
and chose not to participate in the activity. 
Of all these activities, the horticulture plant-based activities (# 3, 4, 6, 9, 10) show greater percentage 
of interaction time than the craft-type activities (#1, 2, 7, 8). This indicates that horticulture may be 
more useful than craft-type activities for intergenerational programs with a goal of increased 
interaction and relationship development. 
Conclusions 
The HT goals of working with young children or elderly adults have several similarities, but also 
several differences. The decrease in children’s attendance and elderly adults’ participation during 
intergenerational activities compared to separate activities indicates that introducing intergenerational 
interactions may detract from the ability to accomplish HT goals. This is especially seen with the 
elderly adult group. Participants with physical limitations or Alzheimer’s disease who require a high 
amount of direct assistance could not participate as well during intergenerational activities. 
The horticultural plant-based intergenerational activities showed more success than horticultural 
crafts at encouraging social interactions between the two groups as reflected by this study. The craft-type 
activities involved simple skills such as cutting, glueing, or writing, These types of tasks 
encourage individual activity more than group activity. The horticulture plant-based activities 
involved less individualized tasks and more group effort and team work for completion. For 
intergenerational programs that aim to increase social interaction and relationship development 
between generations, horticulture with live plants may help attain these goals more effectively than 
craft-type activities. 
Because most interaction was observed between the older children and the elderly adults with more 
33
physical and cognitive abilities, this research may indicate that two groups in this study may not be 
the best combination for intergenerational programs in this type of program. The younger children 
and elderly adults with limited physical and cognitive abilities who had higher demands for attention 
from the staff and volunteers were not as strongly or positively influenced by the intergenerational 
interactions. The age and activity level of the participants influenced their type of interaction during 
intergenerational activities. 
The decision to implement intergenerational HT programming will depend upon the age, 
developmental or ability level, and goals for the individuals involved. Individuals with high demands 
for physical assistance during activities may not benefit from intergenerational activities that demand 
high levels attention and support from volunteers or staff. 
The high level of variables and compounding factors that are introduced when working with young 
children, elderly adults, staff, and volunteers limits the ability to reach clear conclusions and simply 
provides direction for conducting programs and future research. Because of the limited resources 
available for this study, the results of this data are not conclusive evidence of the desirable or 
undesirable outcome of the addition of intergenerational experiences to the HT program. Rather, the 
results of this study serve to make recommendations for program implementation and further 
research. More conclusive results could be obtained if a source of participants could be identified that 
would provide a larger and more random sample population, the use of a separate control group for 
comparison, and a longer duration of study. 
Intergenerational research involves many variables that affect the outcome of the study. Allowing 
more time to train volunteers and gain experience in working with both elderly adults and children 
will increase the success of activities. Use of professionals with training, experience, and a long-term 
position would provide the greatest reduction in variance due to personnel. Using preliminary tests 
to gain experience when using video to collect data will decrease the effects of video limitations. 
Video can also allow for data analysis by an unbiased observer who is ignorant of the research 
hypothesis. 
34
Guidelines 
Based on this research several guidelines were found to be useful in encouraging successful 
intergenerational interactions during HT activities. 
Activities: 1. Appeal to both age groups’ interest 
2. Are developmentally and functionally appropriate for the groups involved 
3. Group activities are preferred to individual activities done in groups 
4. Have a definite start and end 
5. Have a definite shared goal 
Set up: 1. Small groups allow for easy, more intimate contact 
2. Have all participants seated at table, interspersed by age 
3. Introduce participants or have them introduce themselves 
4. Have a definite task for each person; emphasize importance of all participation 
5. Encourage 1-on-1 interaction between participants 
6. Prevent distractions from noise, discomfort, etc. 
35 
Staff/Volunteers: 
1. Have a suitable ratio of staff/volunteers to participants 
(Recommended maximum of 4 participants for 1 volunteer) 
2. Be familiar with needs of both groups, with appropriate expectations according to 
the developmental or functional level of the individuals 
3. Avoid over-direction or excessive involvement with participants 
4. Direct activity with positive attitude 
5. Encourage interactions among participants 
6. Keep a journal for activity- and self-evaluation 
7. Establish good communication for feedback, input, etc. 
Interactions: 1. Do not force interactions or participation
2. Best when either elderly adult or child can assist each other or work together in 
groups equally 
3. Best for individuals that do not require constant direct assistance from 
staff/volunteer 
Recommendations for Future Research 
Research is in its initial stages both for HT and for intergenerational programming. This research 
project identified several areas that need to be addressed in future studies. 
36 
Activities 
& How the set up affects the ability to reach the activity goals 
& How plant-based activities compare to craft-type activities for different populations 
Volunteers 
& What training will produce the best results from volunteers 
& What time and input expectations are appropriate from volunteers 
& How volunteer turnover could affect the research 
& What the suitable ratio of staff/volunteers to participants is for groups with different abilities, 
needs, and limitations 
Data Collection 
& What the effectiveness is of different data collection techniques such as journaling, video tape, 
or unbiased and uninvolved observers 
& How the data collection methods can alter the results or conclusions 
Intergenerational Groups 
& What the effects are of intergenerational HT interactions involving different combinations of 
ages and abilities of children and elderly adults
& How alterations to the activity, tools, or environment may increase participation and 
interaction from elderly adults with severe physical limitations or Alzheimer’s disease 
Literature Cited 
Abbott, G., V. Cochran, and A.A. Clair. 1997. Innovations in intergenerational programs for persons 
who are elderly: The role of horticultural therapy in a multidisciplinary approach, p. 27-38. 
In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the Older Adult. Hawthorne Press, NY. 
Aday, R.H. 1991. Youth’s attitudes toward the elderly: The impact of intergenerational partners. J. 
37 
Applied Gerontology 10(3):372-384. 
Chapman, N.J. and M.B. Neal. 1990. The effects of intergenerational experiences on adolescents and 
older adults. The Gerontologist 30(6):825-832. 
Dellman-Jenkins, M., D. Lambert, and D. Fruit. 1991. Fostering preschoolers’ prosocial behaviors 
toward the elderly: The effect of an intergenerational program. Educ. Gerontology 17:21-32. 
Epstein, S.G. and D.S. Greenberger. 1990. Nurturing plants, children, and older individuals: 
Intergenerational horticultural therapy. J. Therapeutic Hort. 5:16-19. 
Gardening Science Manual, New York Board of Education publication. 
Kerrigan, J. and N.C. Stevenson. 1997. Behavioral study of youth and elders in an intergenerational 
horticultural therapy program, p. 141-154. In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the 
Older Adult Population. Hawthorne Press, NY. 
Moore, B. 1989. Growing with Gardening. Univ. of NC Press, Chapel Hill, NC. 
Moore, R.C. 1993. Plants for Play. MIG Communications, Berkley, CA. 
Rothert, E.R. and J.R. Daubert. 1981. Horticultural Therapy for Nursing Homes, Senior Centers, 
Retirement Living. Chicago Horticultural Society, Chicago, IL. 
Seefeldt, C. 1987. Intergenerational programs. Childhood Educ. (October):14-18. 
Seefeldt, C. 1989. Intergenerational programs: Impact on attitudes. J. of Children in Contemp. 
Society 20:185-194. 
Tice, C.H. 1985. Perspectives on intergenerational initiatives: Past, present, and future. Children 
Today 14(5):6-11.
HORTICULTURE THERAPY ACTIVITIES FOR PRESCHOOL CHILDREN, 
ELDERLY ADULTS, AND INTERGENERATIONAL GROUPS 
Introduction 
These activities were designed as part of an intergenerational research project. They consist of 10 
different HT activities that can be adapted for preschool children, elderly adults, and intergenerational 
groups. The design of the research allowed preschool children and elderly adults to be observed both 
during separate age group activities and during similar intergenerational activities. Each week during 
the 10-week study involved one day for the children’s activity, a second day for the elderly adults’ 
activity, and a third day for the intergenerational activity. The same horticulture activity was used for 
each group’s session during one week, with slight modifications to make the activity appropriate for 
each group. As a result of this study (Chapter IV), 10 activities have been tested for use by children, 
elderly adults, and intergenerational groups. 
The children’s group testing these activities consisted of 16 children between the ages of 3 and 5. The 
children had similar skill levels with some small variation in tasks such as spelling or using fine motor 
skills (cutting with scissors, planting seeds, etc.). The children were part of an afternoon day care 
program at the University Child Development Lab School which met for three hours three days a 
week. The elderly adult group consisted of 12 regular attending participants age 65 or older. The 
elderly adults had various levels of speech, cognitive, and physical abilities. The elderly adults were 
part of the University Adult Day Service day care which met for 9 hours every weekday. These same 
two groups of children and adults attended the intergenerational activities. Participation in the 
horticulture activities was optional for all the participants. The actual number of children and elderly 
adults that participated in each activity varied due to absences or preference for other activities at the 
centers. 
38
Activity Goals 
The goals of HT for children and elderly adults have several similarities including increased 
autonomy, physical and mental stimulation, social interaction, sensory stimulation, creative 
expression, and a sense of wonder and excitement. However, conflicting energy levels of the two 
groups and the opposing needs for quiet and cleanliness for elderly adults and freedom to explore and 
play for children could potentially result in frustrations and negative attitudes between the two groups 
during intergenerational activities. The goals of intergenerational programs include increased social 
contact and interaction, and relationship development between different generations. 
The specific goals for all activities were participation, self-motivation, and fun. Additional goals 
included education and skill enhancement for the children’s group, exercise and skill enhancement for 
the elderly adult group, and social interaction for the intergenerational group. 
Testing of Activities 
The Child Development Lab School and the Adult Day Service are located in the same campus 
building separated only by a small room designed to allow for intergenerational activities. An outdoor 
enclosed patio was used for activities during warm, sunny weather. The intergenerational room and 
the Adult Day Service were equipped with video cameras to record activities. A camera on a tripod 
was used to record outdoor activities for data collection and analysis. 
Four volunteers assisted with the HT activities. These volunteers were part of a university HT class 
that required volunteer service. They had varying levels of experience with horticulture and working 
with children or elderly adults. Two volunteers assisted with the children’s activities both during 
separate and intergenerational activities, and two volunteers similarly assisted with the elderly adult 
activities. 
The activities were chosen to be appropriate and interesting for both groups involved. These activities 
were arranged in a progressive order, starting with getting to know each other (Making Nametags), 
39
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children
Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Ähnlich wie Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children

Mental health 175 page programtami teachersall
Mental health 175 page programtami teachersallMental health 175 page programtami teachersall
Mental health 175 page programtami teachersallpattystewardson
 
Child Neglect Risk & Protective Factors Guide
Child Neglect Risk & Protective Factors GuideChild Neglect Risk & Protective Factors Guide
Child Neglect Risk & Protective Factors GuideUniversity of New Mexico
 
AIDSTAR-One Caring for Children Living with HIV in Africa
AIDSTAR-One Caring for Children Living with HIV in AfricaAIDSTAR-One Caring for Children Living with HIV in Africa
AIDSTAR-One Caring for Children Living with HIV in AfricaAIDSTAROne
 
Global Medical Cures™ | Emerging & Re-Emerging Infectious Diseases
 Global Medical Cures™ | Emerging & Re-Emerging Infectious Diseases Global Medical Cures™ | Emerging & Re-Emerging Infectious Diseases
Global Medical Cures™ | Emerging & Re-Emerging Infectious DiseasesGlobal Medical Cures™
 
World health organization international study on the prevalence of fetal alco...
World health organization international study on the prevalence of fetal alco...World health organization international study on the prevalence of fetal alco...
World health organization international study on the prevalence of fetal alco...BARRY STANLEY 2 fasd
 
A Literature Review On Factors Influencing Abortion In Nepal
A Literature Review On Factors Influencing Abortion In NepalA Literature Review On Factors Influencing Abortion In Nepal
A Literature Review On Factors Influencing Abortion In NepalFinni Rice
 
Identifying and prioritizing stakeholder needs in neurodevelopmental conditio...
Identifying and prioritizing stakeholder needs in neurodevelopmental conditio...Identifying and prioritizing stakeholder needs in neurodevelopmental conditio...
Identifying and prioritizing stakeholder needs in neurodevelopmental conditio...KBHN KT
 
Recommendations for a public health approach
Recommendations for a public health approachRecommendations for a public health approach
Recommendations for a public health approachclac.cab
 
Global Medical Cures™ | Sleep, Sleep Disorders and Biological Rhythms
Global Medical Cures™ | Sleep, Sleep Disorders and Biological RhythmsGlobal Medical Cures™ | Sleep, Sleep Disorders and Biological Rhythms
Global Medical Cures™ | Sleep, Sleep Disorders and Biological RhythmsGlobal Medical Cures™
 
5065946.pdf
5065946.pdf5065946.pdf
5065946.pdfshinco1
 
Dissertation
DissertationDissertation
DissertationAmy Duff
 
Provision of Agriculture related information to rural farmers in Nigeria usin...
Provision of Agriculture related information to rural farmers in Nigeria usin...Provision of Agriculture related information to rural farmers in Nigeria usin...
Provision of Agriculture related information to rural farmers in Nigeria usin...Oyindeinbofa Nicholas.Ere
 
Scoping study student wellbeing study 2008
Scoping study   student wellbeing study 2008Scoping study   student wellbeing study 2008
Scoping study student wellbeing study 2008i4ppis
 
Is psychotherapy effective for reducing suicide attempt and non suicidal sel...
Is psychotherapy effective for reducing suicide attempt and non  suicidal sel...Is psychotherapy effective for reducing suicide attempt and non  suicidal sel...
Is psychotherapy effective for reducing suicide attempt and non suicidal sel...Daryl Chow
 

Ähnlich wie Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children (20)

Mental health 175 page programtami teachersall
Mental health 175 page programtami teachersallMental health 175 page programtami teachersall
Mental health 175 page programtami teachersall
 
Child Neglect Risk & Protective Factors Guide
Child Neglect Risk & Protective Factors GuideChild Neglect Risk & Protective Factors Guide
Child Neglect Risk & Protective Factors Guide
 
Breanne.dissertation final version (1)
Breanne.dissertation final version (1)Breanne.dissertation final version (1)
Breanne.dissertation final version (1)
 
AIDSTAR-One Caring for Children Living with HIV in Africa
AIDSTAR-One Caring for Children Living with HIV in AfricaAIDSTAR-One Caring for Children Living with HIV in Africa
AIDSTAR-One Caring for Children Living with HIV in Africa
 
Global Medical Cures™ | Emerging & Re-Emerging Infectious Diseases
 Global Medical Cures™ | Emerging & Re-Emerging Infectious Diseases Global Medical Cures™ | Emerging & Re-Emerging Infectious Diseases
Global Medical Cures™ | Emerging & Re-Emerging Infectious Diseases
 
World health organization international study on the prevalence of fetal alco...
World health organization international study on the prevalence of fetal alco...World health organization international study on the prevalence of fetal alco...
World health organization international study on the prevalence of fetal alco...
 
A Literature Review On Factors Influencing Abortion In Nepal
A Literature Review On Factors Influencing Abortion In NepalA Literature Review On Factors Influencing Abortion In Nepal
A Literature Review On Factors Influencing Abortion In Nepal
 
Identifying and prioritizing stakeholder needs in neurodevelopmental conditio...
Identifying and prioritizing stakeholder needs in neurodevelopmental conditio...Identifying and prioritizing stakeholder needs in neurodevelopmental conditio...
Identifying and prioritizing stakeholder needs in neurodevelopmental conditio...
 
Wiggins dissertationfinal
Wiggins dissertationfinalWiggins dissertationfinal
Wiggins dissertationfinal
 
An Assessment of a Vocational Horticulture Training Program at a School for J...
An Assessment of a Vocational Horticulture Training Program at a School for J...An Assessment of a Vocational Horticulture Training Program at a School for J...
An Assessment of a Vocational Horticulture Training Program at a School for J...
 
Recommendations for a public health approach
Recommendations for a public health approachRecommendations for a public health approach
Recommendations for a public health approach
 
Global Medical Cures™ | Sleep, Sleep Disorders and Biological Rhythms
Global Medical Cures™ | Sleep, Sleep Disorders and Biological RhythmsGlobal Medical Cures™ | Sleep, Sleep Disorders and Biological Rhythms
Global Medical Cures™ | Sleep, Sleep Disorders and Biological Rhythms
 
5065946.pdf
5065946.pdf5065946.pdf
5065946.pdf
 
Out 6
Out 6Out 6
Out 6
 
phd thesis
phd thesisphd thesis
phd thesis
 
Dissertation 2.1
Dissertation 2.1Dissertation 2.1
Dissertation 2.1
 
Dissertation
DissertationDissertation
Dissertation
 
Provision of Agriculture related information to rural farmers in Nigeria usin...
Provision of Agriculture related information to rural farmers in Nigeria usin...Provision of Agriculture related information to rural farmers in Nigeria usin...
Provision of Agriculture related information to rural farmers in Nigeria usin...
 
Scoping study student wellbeing study 2008
Scoping study   student wellbeing study 2008Scoping study   student wellbeing study 2008
Scoping study student wellbeing study 2008
 
Is psychotherapy effective for reducing suicide attempt and non suicidal sel...
Is psychotherapy effective for reducing suicide attempt and non  suicidal sel...Is psychotherapy effective for reducing suicide attempt and non  suicidal sel...
Is psychotherapy effective for reducing suicide attempt and non suicidal sel...
 

Mehr von School Vegetable Gardening - Victory Gardens

Mehr von School Vegetable Gardening - Victory Gardens (20)

Behind Enemy Lines - Marthe Cohn - One Woman against Nazi Germany
Behind Enemy Lines - Marthe Cohn - One Woman against Nazi GermanyBehind Enemy Lines - Marthe Cohn - One Woman against Nazi Germany
Behind Enemy Lines - Marthe Cohn - One Woman against Nazi Germany
 
Coconut Oil helps Heal Children's ADHD - ADD Disease, Autism & Alzheimer Disease
Coconut Oil helps Heal Children's ADHD - ADD Disease, Autism & Alzheimer DiseaseCoconut Oil helps Heal Children's ADHD - ADD Disease, Autism & Alzheimer Disease
Coconut Oil helps Heal Children's ADHD - ADD Disease, Autism & Alzheimer Disease
 
One Teacher Makes Students into Champions
One Teacher Makes Students into ChampionsOne Teacher Makes Students into Champions
One Teacher Makes Students into Champions
 
Good Books help Students Excel in Life & School
Good Books help Students Excel in Life & SchoolGood Books help Students Excel in Life & School
Good Books help Students Excel in Life & School
 
Greening & Restoring the Sahara Desert with the Groasis Waterboxx
Greening & Restoring the Sahara Desert with the Groasis WaterboxxGreening & Restoring the Sahara Desert with the Groasis Waterboxx
Greening & Restoring the Sahara Desert with the Groasis Waterboxx
 
Groasis Waterboxx Lets Trees Grow Up in Unfriendly Places
Groasis Waterboxx Lets Trees Grow Up in Unfriendly PlacesGroasis Waterboxx Lets Trees Grow Up in Unfriendly Places
Groasis Waterboxx Lets Trees Grow Up in Unfriendly Places
 
Explanation of the Groasis Technology for Growing Food in Desert Regions
Explanation of the Groasis Technology for Growing Food in Desert RegionsExplanation of the Groasis Technology for Growing Food in Desert Regions
Explanation of the Groasis Technology for Growing Food in Desert Regions
 
Groasis Waterboxx & the Agua, Vida Naturaleza Project for Growing Food in Des...
Groasis Waterboxx & the Agua, Vida Naturaleza Project for Growing Food in Des...Groasis Waterboxx & the Agua, Vida Naturaleza Project for Growing Food in Des...
Groasis Waterboxx & the Agua, Vida Naturaleza Project for Growing Food in Des...
 
Groasis Waterboxx Handbook on Planting Instructions for Trees & Crops in Dese...
Groasis Waterboxx Handbook on Planting Instructions for Trees & Crops in Dese...Groasis Waterboxx Handbook on Planting Instructions for Trees & Crops in Dese...
Groasis Waterboxx Handbook on Planting Instructions for Trees & Crops in Dese...
 
Groasis Waterboxx Manual for Growing Vegetables in Arid Lands
Groasis Waterboxx Manual for Growing Vegetables in Arid LandsGroasis Waterboxx Manual for Growing Vegetables in Arid Lands
Groasis Waterboxx Manual for Growing Vegetables in Arid Lands
 
Water Saving Measures of Using the Groasis Waterboxx in Organic Gardening in ...
Water Saving Measures of Using the Groasis Waterboxx in Organic Gardening in ...Water Saving Measures of Using the Groasis Waterboxx in Organic Gardening in ...
Water Saving Measures of Using the Groasis Waterboxx in Organic Gardening in ...
 
Making a Week’s Worth of Rain Last the Whole Year
Making a Week’s Worth of Rain Last the Whole YearMaking a Week’s Worth of Rain Last the Whole Year
Making a Week’s Worth of Rain Last the Whole Year
 
Using the Groasis Waterboxx to Plant New Trees in Desert Regions
Using the Groasis Waterboxx to Plant New Trees in Desert RegionsUsing the Groasis Waterboxx to Plant New Trees in Desert Regions
Using the Groasis Waterboxx to Plant New Trees in Desert Regions
 
Greening the World - Desert Restoration, Reduce CO2, Feed the People & Create...
Greening the World - Desert Restoration, Reduce CO2, Feed the People & Create...Greening the World - Desert Restoration, Reduce CO2, Feed the People & Create...
Greening the World - Desert Restoration, Reduce CO2, Feed the People & Create...
 
Groasis Technology Compared to Drip Irrigation
Groasis Technology Compared to Drip IrrigationGroasis Technology Compared to Drip Irrigation
Groasis Technology Compared to Drip Irrigation
 
Groasis Waterboxx - Palm Springs Students Test New Planter Designed to Fight ...
Groasis Waterboxx - Palm Springs Students Test New Planter Designed to Fight ...Groasis Waterboxx - Palm Springs Students Test New Planter Designed to Fight ...
Groasis Waterboxx - Palm Springs Students Test New Planter Designed to Fight ...
 
Groasis Waterboxx Handbook for Planting Methods & Sample of Crop Test Results...
Groasis Waterboxx Handbook for Planting Methods & Sample of Crop Test Results...Groasis Waterboxx Handbook for Planting Methods & Sample of Crop Test Results...
Groasis Waterboxx Handbook for Planting Methods & Sample of Crop Test Results...
 
Groasis Waterboxx Technology Offers Possible Cure for the Deserts
Groasis Waterboxx Technology Offers Possible Cure for the DesertsGroasis Waterboxx Technology Offers Possible Cure for the Deserts
Groasis Waterboxx Technology Offers Possible Cure for the Deserts
 
Austin Sustainable Neighborhoods uses Groasis Waterboxx to Successfully Plant...
Austin Sustainable Neighborhoods uses Groasis Waterboxx to Successfully Plant...Austin Sustainable Neighborhoods uses Groasis Waterboxx to Successfully Plant...
Austin Sustainable Neighborhoods uses Groasis Waterboxx to Successfully Plant...
 
Box Harvest Water in the Driest Place
Box Harvest Water in the Driest PlaceBox Harvest Water in the Driest Place
Box Harvest Water in the Driest Place
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...christianmathematics
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfAn Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfSanaAli374401
 
SECOND SEMESTER TOPIC COVERAGE SY 2023-2024 Trends, Networks, and Critical Th...
SECOND SEMESTER TOPIC COVERAGE SY 2023-2024 Trends, Networks, and Critical Th...SECOND SEMESTER TOPIC COVERAGE SY 2023-2024 Trends, Networks, and Critical Th...
SECOND SEMESTER TOPIC COVERAGE SY 2023-2024 Trends, Networks, and Critical Th...KokoStevan
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDThiyagu K
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptRamjanShidvankar
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxAreebaZafar22
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfagholdier
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdfAn Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
An Overview of Mutual Funds Bcom Project.pdf
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
SECOND SEMESTER TOPIC COVERAGE SY 2023-2024 Trends, Networks, and Critical Th...
SECOND SEMESTER TOPIC COVERAGE SY 2023-2024 Trends, Networks, and Critical Th...SECOND SEMESTER TOPIC COVERAGE SY 2023-2024 Trends, Networks, and Critical Th...
SECOND SEMESTER TOPIC COVERAGE SY 2023-2024 Trends, Networks, and Critical Th...
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptxICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
ICT Role in 21st Century Education & its Challenges.pptx
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 

Assessng an Intergeneratonal Horticulture Therapy Program for Elderly Adults and Preschool Children

  • 1. ASSESSING AN INTERGENERATIONAL HORTICULTURE THERAPY PROGRAM FOR ELDERLY ADULTS AND PRESCHOOL CHILDREN by Mary Lorraine Predny Thesis submitted to the Faculty of Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE in HORTICULTURE ____________________________ Dr. P. Diane Relf Committee Chair ____________________________ ____________________________ Dr. J. Roger Harris Dr. Andrew J. Stremmel KEY WORDS: Volunteers, Staff, Video, Activities, Day care
  • 2. ASSESSING AN INTERGENERATIONAL HORTICULTURE THERAPY PROGRAM FOR ELDERLY ADULTS AND PRESCHOOL CHILDREN by Mary Lorraine Predny Dr. Diane Relf, Chair Horticulture Department ABSTRACT The goal of this research project was to determine if introducing intergenerational interactions would supplement or detract from the use of horticulture as a therapeutic tool when working with elderly adults and preschool children. The program was set up to compare independent group activities with intergenerational activities. A group of elderly adults in the University Adult Day Service and a group of preschool children in the University Child Development Laboratory School took part in both separate age group and intergenerational activities. There were three sessions each week: one for the children’s group, a second one for the elderly adults’ group, and a third one that combined both groups. The same activity was done during all three sessions each week, with modifications to make the activity appropriate for each age group and to make it more interactive for the intergenerational group. These activities took place in the campus building where the day care centers are located. Four volunteers assisted with the activities. Two worked with the children’s group both during separate and intergenerational activities, and two volunteers similarly assisted with the elderly adult group. Video cameras were used to record each session. These videos were viewed and evaluated after the 10-week horticulture therapy program was completed to score attendance and participation during separate age group activities, and attendance, participation, and interaction between the two groups during intergenerational activities. This data was used to determine if introducing intergenerational interactions affected the individual’s attendance or participation, and to determine if the interactions between the two groups showed any change over time.
  • 3. Several variables were shown to affect the outcome of research. The first variable discussed is the effect of the staff, volunteers, or administration on the participants and the activities. Staff and volunteers can greatly affect intergenerational interactions by: 1) failing to encourage participation from participants of all ages, 2) lacking experience or having discomfort in working with special populations, 3) failure to establish adequate communication with the researcher or with each other, or 4) demonstrating a negative attitude towards the project. The second variable in research is the limitation introduced by data analysis using video. While video recording is useful in evaluating data, it can cause problems due to a limited viewing area, limited viewing angles, blocked screens, or unfamiliarity with recording equipment. Videos were used to assess participation and interaction. Participation scores include three categories: “no participation” for present but inactive participation, and “working with direct assistance” or “independent participation” for active participation. Participation was affected by the horticulture activities’ set up, difficulty level, and availability of assistance from volunteers. Children’s participation during separate group activities was affected mainly by the difficulty level and set up of activities. Elderly adult’s participation during separate age group activities was affected mainly by each individual’s abilities and availability of assistance. Children’s intergenerational participation scores show an increase in the category of “working with direct assistance”, while elderly adults’ intergenerational scores show an increase in the categories of “no participation” and “independent participation”. In part, the change in intergenerational participation was due to a decrease in the assistance available from volunteers for each individual. Lastly, the percentage of total interaction time between the generations during activities increased over time. However, the introduction of intergenerational interactions detracted from the use of horticulture as a therapeutic tool for elderly adults and preschool children. It is recommended that intergenerational programming may not be useful to fulfill specific horticulture therapy goals for these groups. At the same time, the intergenerational activities involving horticulture plant-based activities were more successful at increasing interactions than the craft-type activities. Therefore horticulture iii
  • 4. may be a useful activity for intergenerational programs with a goal of increased interaction and relationship development. iv
  • 5. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The completion of this research project and thesis could not have been possible without the assistance and support of many individuals. Thanks to my advisor Dr. Diane Relf for her enthusiasm, guidance, and constructive criticism which helped turn a good idea into a rewarding project. Her innovative ideas, resourcefulness, and practical solutions made it easier to face the many challenges that arose throughout the course of this project. I also thank my committee members Drs. Roger Harris and Andrew Stremmel for their advice and support throughout this study. I am grateful to the Virginia Tech Adult Day Service and the Child Development Lab School for the use of their facilities, their patient and advising staff, and most of all for allowing me the pleasure of working with their clients, many of whom made this research project an incredibly enjoyable and rewarding experience. I owe many thanks to the four horticulture therapy volunteers who offered their time and energy, the Virginia Tech Greenhouses, Floral Design Lab, and Dave Angle for donating horticultural supplies, the staff in the Office of Consumer Horticulture who read and revised this manuscript, and Alan McDaniels for his statistical consultations. Without their assistance this project would not have been successful. Lastly, I extend my infinite love and appreciation to my family and friends who offered me the encouragement, advice, and support that gave me the strength and courage to follow my dreams, and the love and happiness to enjoy life along the way. v
  • 6. TABLE OF CONTENTS I. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1 II. Perspectives on Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Horticulture Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Intergenerational Programming Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 Implementing a Program . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 Gardening for Elderly Adults . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 Gardening for Children . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6 Comparison . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Literature Cited . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7 Additional References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 III. Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy Research Variables Introduced by Staff, Volunteers, and Video . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 Working with Staff and Volunteers . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 12 Video . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 Literature Cited . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21 IV. Intergenerational Interactions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22 Materials and Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 23 Results and Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33 Guidelines . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35 Recommendations for Future Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 Literature Cited . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 37 V. Horticulture Therapy Activities for Preschool Children, Elderly Adults, and Intergenerational Groups . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38 Activity Goals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 Testing of Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 39 Activities . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51 Elderly Adult Activity References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52 Preschool Children Activity References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 Intergenerational Activity References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 vi
  • 7. vii VI. Appendices Appendix A: Separate Age Group Activity Participation Chart - Children . . . . . . . . . . . 55 Appendix B: Separate Age Group Activity Participation Chart - Elderly Adults . . . . . . 58 Appendix C: Intergenerational Activity Participation Chart - Children . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 Appendix D: Intergenerational Activity Participation Chart - Elderly Adults . . . . . . . . . 65 Appendix E: Total Participation - Children . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 Appendix F: Total Participation - Elderly Adults . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 Appendix G: Total Separate Age Group Activity Participation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 76 Appendix H: Total Intergenerational Activity Participation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79 Appendix I: Intergenerational Interaction Chart - Children . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82 Appendix J: Intergenerational Interaction Chart - Elderly Adults . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 Appendix K: Total Individual and Activity Intergenerational Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . 92
  • 8. INTRODUCTION The chapters in this thesis were written as separate articles to be submitted to different journals for publication. Because of this, there is some repetition among the chapters which is necessary to clarify the information within them. Even though each article results from the same research study, they address different issues and do not repeat results and conclusions. The first two articles, “Perspectives on Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy” and “Intergenerational Horticulture Therapy Research Variables Introduced by Staff, Volunteers, and Video” will be submitted to the Journal of the American Horticulture Therapy Association. The third article, “Intergenerational Interactions” will be submitted to HortTechnology. The last article, “Horticulture Activities for Preschool Children, Elderly Adults, and Intergenerational Groups” will be submitted to Activities, Adaptation, and Aging. 1
  • 9. PERSPECTIVES ON INTERGENERATIONAL HORTICULTURE THERAPY Horticultural Therapy Historically, horticulture has proven to be an effective means of therapy for many different populations. Horticultural Therapy (HT) generally refers to the use of horticultural activities that are adapted to meet specific goals of an individual with special needs in treatment (Relf, 1997). Many individuals benefit from these activities, including elderly adults, disabled children, mentally and physically disabled adults, or other individuals who could benefit from participating in horticultural activities, but who require special adaptations or modifications to do so (Relf and Dorn, 1995). Horticulture therapy programs are usually led by a professional who is trained to “tailor the use of plants to fit the therapy and rehabilitation needs of those individuals with whom they are working” (AHTA publication). Although the specific needs of any particular population and even the needs of several individuals within a certain population may vary, the main goals of a HT program remain the same. As Relf states, “the specific goals toward which a HT program is directed may differ distinctly from one institution to another and from one population to another. However, the ultimate goal of these programs is the improved physical and mental health of the individual” (Relf [Hefley], 1973). Horticulture has become a valuable therapeutic tool because the activities can easily be adjusted and adapted to meet the needs of any specific population without altering the main objectives of the program. Intergenerational Horticultural Therapy Recently, intergenerational HT programs have been used in order to add to a horticulture program the opportunity for diverse social interactions for special groups that could benefit from such experiences (Abbott et al., 1997; Epstein and Greenberger, 1990; Kerrigan and Stevenson, 1997). Intergenerational HT programs have not been widely used or studied, so the impacts of such programs are unknown. Non-horticultural intergenerational programs have been studied in various 2
  • 10. situations over the past several years, but the results of these studies are mostly inconclusive or contradictory (Dellman-Jenkins et al., 1991; Seefeldt, 1989). It is important, therefore, to study these interactions to determine if they could become an asset to HT programming. Intergenerational Programming Research Generations United, a group formed in the mid 1980s to promote intergenerational activities, defines intergenerational programming as “the purposeful bringing together of different generations in ongoing planned activities designed to achieve the development of new relationships as well as specified program goals” (in Ventura-Merkel et al., 1989). The idea of intergenerational exchanges emerged in the 1960s with programs such as ‘Adopt a Grandparent’ and other similar programs that connected young, school-aged children to elderly adults (Newman, 1989). These programs were mainly started in response to decreased contact between the generations. Despite the increasing population of elderly persons in our society, young people have infrequent or ineffectual connections with them due to a “breakdown of the extended family network [and] increased ageism and age segregation” (Cohon, 1989). Sally Newman, founder of the University of Pittsburgh’s Generations Together program developed to research intergenerational programs, states that “for our elderly, there has been (an observed) decline in self-esteem and self-worth, and an increase in feelings of loneliness. For our children and youth, there has been an observed loss of the traditional elder/child nurturing, a loss of cultural and historical connections, and an increase in their fear of aging. Age segregation, furthermore, seems to have resulted in an increase in myths and stereotypes between the young and the old” (Newman, 1989). The goal of intergenerational programs has been to alleviate the isolation and negative attitudes that result from a lack of contact between these groups. These programs also serve to expose individuals to the diversity of human life in order to promote understanding and acceptance of the differences that exist between the generations. “When we learn, work and play only with our age-peers, we begin to accept a homogenous view of the world as our version of reality. This leads not only to a shallow, one-dimensional view of how things are, but inevitably begins to limit the possibilities of how they can become” (Tice, 1985). 3
  • 11. Intergenerational contact is seen as equally important for all generations, even though the benefits for each group varies. Although these programs can refer to interactions between any generations, most studies have focused on exchanges between elderly adults and young children. These groups are assumed to receive the greatest benefit from intergenerational experiences due to their limited contact with each other in everyday life and their similar status as ‘dependent’ in society. Today’s elderly adult population experiences fewer social contacts and increased isolation due to the negative perceptions of younger populations (Cohon, 1989). Developing positive relationships with the younger generation are reported to increase feelings of self-esteem and life satisfaction, while decreasing isolation and loneliness for elderly adults in our society (Seefeldt, 1989). A study by Pastorello et al. “noted that institutionalized elderly reported not only less loneliness and depression but feelings of youthfulness following volitional interaction with preschool children” (in Kocarnik and Ponzetti, 1991). For children, changes in family structure and a loss of connection with their grandparents may cause a lack of continuity (Chamberlain et al., 1994) and misunderstandings or misconceptions about elderly adults and the aging process (Seefeldt, 1989). Exchanges with the older generation are said to “influence moral and personal development of the maturing child” (Cohon, 1989) and help the younger generation shape their value systems “by seeing their linkages to the past” (Chamberlain et al., 1994). Not only do relationships with elderly adults help to shape morals and values and prevent negative stereotypes about aging, but a study by Kerschner and Harris also indicated that the “children often thrive on the individualized attention the seniors can provide” (in Kocarnik and Ponzetti, 1991). Not all studies on intergenerational experiences have shown these positive effects. Seefeldt (1987) cites several studies of programs that had inconclusive or negative effects on the participants. In a later study, a group of preschoolers that visited infirm elderly adults actually showed an increase in their negative attitudes toward their own aging (Seefeldt, 1989). Seefeldt (1987) hypothesizes that “differing research methodologies, samples, and types of programs and contacts may account for 4
  • 12. these inconsistent findings.” Previous studies indicate possible reasons for these negative results and solutions that may prevent undesirable experiences. These include making the contact beneficial for both groups rather than having one group serve the other (Tice, 1985), designing projects that “have a definite purpose or end product” (Aday et al., 1991), establishing programs that are long-term so the individuals have a chance to establish relationships with one another (Seefeldt, 1989), and ensuring that positive attitudes develop as a result of discovered similarities between the groups (Chapman and Neal, 1990). Seefeldt (1987) proposed recommendations for ensuring successful programs that reinforce these points and include “protecting the prestige of elders as well as children; limiting frustration for both adults and children by arranging for contact that is intimate, not casual; planning for interaction that has integrity and is functional for both groups; and ensuring that contact between old and young is rewarding and pleasant for both groups.” Implementing a Program According to these guidelines, HT has excellent potential as a focus for intergenerational interaction because it provides benefits for all persons involved, a definite end product to share and discuss, a long-term project with intrinsic rewards, and a common interest in order to establish a connection between the groups. The horticultural activities could also be adapted to meet the needs of both groups involved. However, it is not as certain that intergenerational interactions would complement a HT program. For example, the specific goals of gardening with elderly adults and gardening with children have some conflicting objectives that may make the combination of the two groups inappropriate. Gardening for Elderly Adults According to past studies, gardening is one of the preferred leisure activities for elderly adults (Burgess, 1990; Hill and Relf, 1983; Relf, 1989). Horticulture can offer many opportunities for exercise and socialization for elderly persons who are learning to deal with limitations such as sensory loss, physical decline, and loss of status in society (Carstens, 1985). Many tools and activities can be 5
  • 13. adapted to meet the needs of individuals with disabilities such as arthritis, cerebral palsy, Alzheimer’s disease, and dementia (Bubel, 1990; Kerrigan and Stevenson, 1997; Whittier, 1991). Some research even shows that gardening can alleviate specific psychological problems (Langer and Rodin, 1976; Rodin and Langer, 1977). According to Haas et al. (1998), “health conditions affect an older person’s ability to perform a range of common activities needed for personal self-maintenance and independent community residence.... Rehabilitation and supervised care that includes HT can help reduce personal losses and restore an older person’s level of functioning.” Some possible benefits include improved quality of life, control and independence, a greater sense of personal responsibility and autonomy, social interaction, mental stimulation, sensory stimulation, decreased boredom, creative expression, stimulation of long- and short-term memory, and physical exercise on various levels (Burgess, 1990; Carstens, 1985; Haas et al., 1998; Hill and Relf, 1983; Langer and Rodin, 1976; Rodin and Langer, 1977; Rothert and Daubert, 1981). Studies indicate that elderly adults prefer gardens that contain popular plants from their youth, produce that they can enjoy, and a place that is clean and well-kept where they can relax in peace and quiet (Mooney, 1994). Gardening for Children Children’s gardening as a means of environmental education has gained popularity in recent years and is now supported by many schools, botanic gardens and arboreta, cooperative extension, and other agencies or groups (Relf and Dorn, 1995). Many studies show that environmental attitudes are formed at a young age, so it is important to encourage positive views of nature early in a child’s development (Eberbach, 1990; Wilson, 1995, 1996). Using hands-on gardening while teaching principles of environmental conservation and basic science can be very effective because children learn mainly through physical contact and manipulation of the world around them (Eberbach, 1988, 1990; Moore, 1996; Palmer, 1994; Straw, 1990; Wilson, 1995). Horticulture can also be used to teach math, art, history, language skills, social studies, and literature (DeMarco, 1997). Adapting horticultural activities to meet the needs and stages of development can “appeal to a child’s interest, encourage experimentation in the physical world, and foster perspective taking and cooperation in the social world” (DeVries and Zan, 1995). The outdoors can also offer a “greater sense of freedom” 6
  • 14. and “more unrestricting play activities” to support the importance of play in a child’s development and learning (Henniger, 1994). According to Wilson, “once children learn to respect and love the world of nature, they will be ready and eager to learn more about the scientific aspects of the world around them” (1995). Some additional goals of gardening with children of any age include developing patience, perseverance, reverence, responsibility, cooperation, physical health, good work habits, motivation for learning, confidence, empathy, a sense of wonder and excitement, and respect and appreciation for nature (Bunn, 1986; Green, 1994; Waters, 1993; Wilson, 1995). Children’s gardens should be designed to offer freedom to move and play, plants that are interesting and exotic, and “leftover, wild places that they have the freedom to manipulate. They tend to be wild, messy places” (Hart, 1993). Comparison The needs of elderly adults and children that are addressed by HT have many similarities including increased autonomy, a sense of wonder and excitement, physical and mental stimulation, social interactions, sensory stimulation, and creative expression. However, the conflicting energy levels of the two groups, and the opposing needs for quiet and cleanliness for elderly adults and freedom to explore and play for children, may result in frustrations and negative attitudes between the two groups. Literature Cited Abbott, G., V. Cochran, and A.A. Clair. 1997. Innovations in intergenerational programs for persons who are elderly: The role of horticultural therapy in a multidisciplinary approach, p. 27-38. In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the Older Adult. Hawthorne Press, NY. Aday, R.H. 1991. Youth’s attitudes toward the elderly: The impact of intergenerational partners. J. 7 Applied Gerontology 10(3):372-384. American Horticultural Therapy Association, A Career in Horticultural Therapy, unpublished. Bubel, N. 1990. A therapy garden. Country Journal (September/October):74-76. Bunn, D.E. 1986. Group cohesiveness is enhanced as children engage in plant stimulated discovery activities. J. Therapeutic Hort. 1:37-43. Burgess, C.W. 1990. Horticulture and its application to the institutionalized elderly. Activities, Adaptation and Aging 14(3):51-61.
  • 15. Carstens, D.Y. 1985. Site Planning and Design for the Elderly: Issues, Guidelines, and Alternatives. 8 Van Nostrand Reinhold Co., NY. Chamberlain, V.M., E. Fetterman, and M. Maher. 1994. Innovation in elder and child care: An intergenerational experience. Educ. Gerontology 20:193-204. Chapman, N.J. and M.B. Neal. 1990. The effects of intergenerational experiences on adolescents and older adults. The Gerontologist 30(6):825-832. Cohon, D. 1989. Intergenerational program research to refine theory and practice. J. Children in Contemp. Society 20:217-230. Dellman-Jenkins, M., D. Lambert, and D. Fruit. 1991. Fostering preschoolers’ prosocial behaviors toward the elderly: The effect of an intergenerational program. Educ. Gerontology 17:21-32. DeVries, R. and B. Zan. 1995. Creating a constructivist classroom atmosphere. Young Children 50(9):4-13. DeMarco, L.W. 1997. The Factors Affecting Elementary School Teachers’ Integration of School Gardening into the Curriculum, unpublished thesis. Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University. Eberbach, C. 1988. Garden Design for Children, unpublished thesis. U. Of Delaware. Eberbach, C. 1990. Children’s gardens: The meaning of place, p. 80-83. In: P.D. Relf (ed.). The Role of Horticulture in Human Well-Being and Social Development: A National Symposium. Timber Press, Portland, OR. Epstein, S.G. and D.S. Greenberger. 1990. Nurturing plants, children, and older individuals: Intergenerational horticultural therapy. J. Therapeutic Hort. 5:16-19. Green, K. 1994. Encouraging nurturing behavior of two to seven year olds by introducing plants and flowers, p. 395-408. In: J. Flagler, and R. Poincelot (eds.). People-Plant Relations: Setting Research Priorities, A National Symposium. Hayworth Press, NY. Haas, K., S.P. Simson, and N.C. Stevenson. 1998. Older persons and horticultural therapy practice, p. 231-255. In: S.P. Simson and M.C. Straus (eds.). Horticulture as Therapy: Principles and Practice. Haworth Press, NY. Hart, R. 1993. Kids need wild places, gentle guidance. Amer. Horticulturalist 72(11):3. Henniger. 1994. Planning for outdoor play. Young Children 49(4):10-15. Hill, C.O. and P.D. Relf. 1983. Gardening as an outdoor activity in geriatric institutions. Activities, Adaptation and Aging 3(1):47-54. Kerrigan, J. and N.C. Stevenson. 1997. Behavioral study of youth and elders in an intergenerational horticultural therapy program, p. 141-154. In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the Older Adult Population. Hawthorne Press, NY. Kocarnik, R.A. and J.J. Ponzetti. 1991. The advantages and challenges of intergenerational programs in long-term care facilities. J. Gerontological Social Work 16(½):97-107. Langer, E.J. and J. Rodin. 1976. The effects of choice and enhanced personal responsibility for the aged: A field experiment in an institutional setting. J. Personality and Social Psych. 34(2):191- 198. Mooney, P.F. 1994. Assessing the benefits of a therapeutic horticulture program for seniors in immediate care, p. 173-194. In: M. Francis, P. Lindsey, and J.S. Rice (eds.). The Healing Dimensions of People-Plant Relations. Center for Design Research, Davis, CA. Moore, R.C. 1996. Compact nature: The role of playing and learning gardens on children’s lives. J.
  • 16. 9 Therapeutic Hort. 8:72-82. Newman, S. 1989. A history of intergenerational programs J. of Children in Contemp. Society 20:1- 15. Palmer, J.A. 1994. Acquisition of environmental subject knowledge in preschool children: An international study. Children’s Environments 11(3):204-211. Relf (Hefley), P.D. 1973. Horticulture- a therapeutic tool. J. Rehab. 39(1):27-29. Relf, P.D. 1978. Horticulture as a recreational activity. Am. Health Care Assn. J. 4(5):68-70. Relf, P.D. 1989. Gardening in Raised Beds and Containers for the Elderly and Physically Handicapped. Virginia Cooperative Extension publication. Relf, P.D. 1997. Defining Horticultural Therapy, unpublished. Relf, P.D. and S. Dorn. 1995. Horticulture: Meeting the needs of special populations. HortTechnology 5(2):94-103. Rodin, J. and E.J. Langer. 1977. Long-term effects of a control-relevant intervention with the institutionalized aged. J. Personality and Social Psych. 35(12):897-902. Rothert, E.R. and J.R. Daubert. 1981. Horticultural Therapy for Nursing Homes, Senior Centers, Retirement Living. Chicago Horticultural Society, Chicago, IL. Scheid, D.T. 1976. An Approach to Teaching Children About the Aesthetics of Plants and Gardens, unpublished thesis. U. Of Delaware Seefeldt, C. 1987. Intergenerational programs. Childhood Educ. (October):14-18. Seefeldt, C. 1989. Intergenerational programs: impact on attitudes. J. of Children in Contemp. Society 20:185-194. Straw, H. 1990. The nursery garden. Early Child Development and Care 57:109-120. Tice, C.H. 1985. Perspectives on intergenerational initiatives: Past, present, and future. Children Today 14(5):6-11. Ventura-Merkel, C., D.S. Liederman, and J. Ossofsky. 1989. Exemplary intergenerational programs. J. of Children in Contemp. Society 20:173-180. Waters, M. 1993. Down in the dirt with kids: Tips on raising a crop of young gardeners. Horticulture 71(3):18-22. Whittier, D. 1991. Horticultural activities for physical disabilities of the elderly. NCTRH Newsletter 6(1):3-5. Wilson, R.A. 1995. Nature and young children: A natural connection. Young Children (September):4-8. Wilson, R.A. 1996. Environmental education programs for preschool children. J. Env. Educ. 27(4):28-33. Additional References Angelis, J. 1992. The genesis of an intergenerational program. Educ. Gerontology 18:317-327. Bocian, K. and S. Newman. 1989. Evaluation of intergenerational programs: Why and how. J. Children in Contemp. Society 20:147-163. Browne, C.A. 1994. The role of nature for the promotion of well-being of the elderly, p. 75-79. In:
  • 17. M. Francis, P. Lindsey, and J.S. Rice (eds.). The Healing Dimensions of People Plant Relations. Center for Design Research, Davis, CA. Brummel, S.W. 1989. Developing an intergenerational program. J. Children in Contemp. Society 10 20:119-133. Eberbach, C. 1987. Gardens from a child’s view- an interpretation of children’s art-work. J. Therapeutic Hort. 2:9-16. Ellis, W.S. 1992. The gift of gardening. National Geographic 181(5):52-81. Ezell, D.O., E.V. Jones, and A.P. Olson. 1981. Outdoor Gardening for the Handicapped. Clemson University Cooperative Extension Service publication. Galvin, E.S. 1994. The joy of seasons: With the children, discover the joys of nature. Young Children (May):4-8. Gardening Science Manual, New York Board of Education publication. Haas, K. 1996. The therapeutic quality of plants. J. Therapeutic Hort. 8:61-67. Hamby, A. 1996. Intergenerational Activities: An Observational Study of the Experiences of Children and Adults, unpublished thesis. Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University. Henkin, N.Z. and S. W. Sweeney. 1989. Linking systems: A systems approach to intergenerational programming. J. Children in Contemp. Society 20:165-172. Hochstein, R. 1994. Partners in growing. Parents (July):134-138. Hoover, R.C. 1994. Healing gardens and Alzheimer’s disease, p. 283-299. In: M. Francis, P. Lindsey, and J.S. Rice (eds.). The Healing Dimensions of People-Plant Relations. Center for Design Research, Davis, CA. Houseman, D. 1986. Developing links between horticultural therapy and aging. J. Therapeutic Hort. 1:9-14. Howell, D.C. 1997. Statistical Methods for Psychology. Duxbury Press, NY. Jaus, H.H. 1994. The development and retention of environmental attitudes in elementary school children. J. Env. Educ. 15(3):33-36. Kaplan, M.J. 1994. Use of sensory stimulation with Alzheimer’s patients in a garden setting, p. 291- 306. In: J. Flagler, and R. Poincelot (eds.). People-Plant Relations: Setting Research Priorities, A National Symposium. Hayworth Press, NY. Kuehne, V.S. 1992. Older adults in intergenerational programs: What are their experiences really like. Activities, Adaptation and Aging 16(4):49-67. Labreque, C. and L. Tremblay. 1996. The evolutive prosthetic garden: A new concept for elderly living in nursing facilities. J. Therapeutic Hort. 8:56-60. Matsuo, E. 1990. What we may learn through horticultural activity, p. 146-148. In: P.D. Relf (ed.). The Role of Horticulture in Human Well-Being and Social Development: A National Symposium. Timber Press, Portland, OR. Meyer, H.G. 1973. Children grow in gardens. Flower and Garden (March):50-51. Moore, B. 1989. Growing with Gardening. Univ. of NC Press, Chapel Hill, NC. Moore, R.C. 1993. Plants for Play. MIG Communications, Berkley, CA. Moore, S.H. 1981. Horticultural therapy and the aging client. In: NCTRH publication, The Comprehensive View of Horticulture and the Aging 1(2):55-59. National Gardening Association. 1987. Successful Senior Citizen Gardens. NGA publication. Neer, K. 1990. A children’s garden. The Herbalist 56:69-76.
  • 18. Newman, S. and C. Ward. 1993. An observational study of intergenerational activities and behavior change in dementing elders at an adult day care center. Intl. J. Aging and Human Development 36:321-333. Nordvig, O.K. 1975. Horticultural therapy in public education. California Hort. Journal 36(1):36-37. Rae, W.A. and D.A. Stieber. 1976. Plant play therapy: Growth through growth. J. Pediatric Psych. 11 1(4):18-20. Relf, P.D. 1990. Dynamics of horticultural therapy. Rehab. Lit. 42(5-6):147-150. Stremmel, A.J., S.S. Travis, P. Kelley-Harrison, and A.D. Hensley. 1994. The perceived benefits and problems associated with intergenerational exchanges in day care settings. The Gerontologist 34(4):513-519. Travis, S.S., A.J. Stremmel, and P.A. Duprey. 1993. Child and adult day care professions converging in the 1990s: Implications for training and research. Educ. Gerontology 19:283-293. Van Zandt, K. and J.R. Crace. 1981. The role of horticultural therapy in a retirement community. In: NCTRH publication, The Comprehensive View of Horticulture and the Aging 1(2):49-54. Whiren, A.P. 1995. Planning a garden from a child’s perspective. Children’s Environments 12(2):250-255.
  • 19. INTERGENERATIONAL HORTICULTURE THERAPY RESEARCH VARIABLES INTRODUCED BY STAFF, VOLUNTEERS, AND VIDEO Working with Staff and Volunteers Earlier this year a research project was conducted on a horticultural therapy program involving both elderly adults and preschool children at day care centers in adjoining facilities. This project focused on the interactions between the two groups and the success of horticulture in allowing for meaningful intergenerational activities. The design of this project allowed preschool children and elderly adults to be observed both during separate age group activities and similar intergenerational activities. Each week during the 10-week study included one day for the children’s activity, one day for the elderly adults’ activity, and a third day for the intergenerational activity. The horticulture activities aimed to meet the needs of both the children and the elderly adults, and to allow for social interaction during intergenerational sessions. Although participation fluctuated due to attendance and interest, there were an average of 11 children and 7 adults who regularly chose to participate in the horticultural activities. Two horticulture student volunteers assisted with the children’s group both during the separate and intergenerational activities, and two additional volunteers similarly assisted with the elderly adult group. During intergenerational activities, older adults and children separated into four small groups each led by one volunteer. This design was meant to facilitate supervision of the activities and to encourage casual interaction among the participants. Although it was not the intention of this project to study the influence of the volunteers on research, this set up allowed for observations that showed the success of the intergenerational activities was directly related to the volunteers’ attitudes and experience. The staff members at both the Adult Day Service and the Child Development Laboratory School also had a noticeable influence on the project although their involvement in the research was indirect. At the conclusion of the study several observations were made about factors that influence the success of intergenerational HT research projects. Many of these factors related to the staff and volunteers’ influence on the participants and activities. Many other HT research programs are also 12
  • 20. affected by administrators, other staff members, and volunteers that play a role in the implementation and success of activities. In research these interactions can alter the data collected. It is important therefore to recognize possible problem areas before implementing HT research so that personnel factors can be minimized. I will begin by citing several examples of these variables and the effects I observed during my study. From my discussions with other researchers I believe that these examples are fairly common, even though they have not been widely addressed in HT research. I would then like to suggest possible strategies that can be supported by observations made throughout this program which could prevent or minimize the variables introduced by other staff or volunteers. In this study the main areas of concern involving other staff and volunteers are: 1) their attitudes toward the intergenerational HT program, and 2) the interactions between the personnel and the participants. I would like to emphasize that although the variables introduced by the numbers and diversity of personnel involved in HT research can affect the success of the project, these variables may be limited in order to reduce possible negative effects. Before this project began I met with both the directors of the Adult Day Service and the Child Development Lab to discuss the interest and possibility of conducting HT research between these two groups. The general attitudes were very positive for both the horticulture program and the intergenerational activities. Both centers are located in the same campus building, separated only by a small room designed to allow for intergenerational activities and research. As both centers are part of the university they were designed to facilitate research and observation, with observation rooms and video equipment for recording activities. One main area of concern is the interaction between the personnel and the participants. Both the staff members at the Adult Day Service and the Child Development Lab were knowledgeable and experienced in working with their own groups and meeting their specific needs during activities. However, during intergenerational activities most of these staff members did not interact with the other group members present. A lack of experience or possible discomfort in working with a new 13
  • 21. group led to a lack of interactions between the staff and the other group present. This not only excluded the new group and decreased the social interactions, but the close assistance from the staff also reduced the independent involvement and self motivation of the participants during the HT activities. Without the encouragement from the staff the older adults and children would not interact during activities. For this study, these staff members were asked not to assist so the trained horticulture volunteers could conduct their intergenerational groups without distractions or disruptions. If time and money allow, a better solution would be offering training for working with both groups, or education in both gerontology and child development to alleviate these negative effects on intergenerational interaction (Kocarnik and Ponzetti, 1991; Seefeldt, 1987; Stremmel et al., 1994; Travis et al., 1993). This could also decrease the negative effects caused by a lack of experience. The four horticulture student volunteers who assisted in this project had varying degrees of experience with either elderly adults or preschool children. As each intergenerational activity was broken down into small groups supervised by each volunteer, it was possible to observe the effects of their experience or lack of it. The two volunteers who had more experience were more comfortable working with their groups and encouraging participation and interaction. The small groups they supervised were more active, and therefore the volunteers concluded that the intergenerational program was a success and beneficial for both groups involved. The two volunteers who lacked experience working with either elderly adults or children were less likely to encourage participation and interaction from either age participants, and therefore concluded that the intergenerational program was not successful, or even interesting. For intergenerational activities to be successful they must be thoroughly planned and organized. Good leadership and encouragement are essential in promoting interactions. Simply placing the two groups in a room together and passively observing for interactions will produce poor results. According to Kocarnik and Ponzetti (1991), a mediator is needed to help “make sense of the encounter..., initiate conversations, respond to questions, facilitate the formation of relationships..., and discuss any questions or concerns [the participants] might have.” The role of this mediator is crucial for success, as “each friendship requires nurturing and guidance if it is to benefit both the young and the old” (Kocarnik and Ponzetti, 1991). 14
  • 22. At the same time, this mediator (the volunteer or staff member) must remain focused on the larger goals of interaction and relationship development. In a study by Kerrigan and Stevenson (1997), intergenerational interaction was decreased when the mediator intervened to make corrections during the steps of the activity. The mediator must always keep in mind that achieving the larger goals is more important than the specific process or the end product (Kerrigan and Stevenson, 1997). Many of these factors that play a role in the overall perception of the research program could be prevented or at least minimized to have less of an effect on the outcome of the study. Because the staff and volunteers involved in the program are not always credited with the success and because human resources can be difficult to find these variables that could potentially be controlled are often overlooked. As a result of the large number of staff and volunteers involved in this study additional procedures were identified that could improve communication and facilitate research implementation and success. The first step before implementing any research program is to establish good communication with the administrators and staff at the facility where the research will take place. Some important concerns that need to be discussed are the facility’s interest and support for HT research, their knowledge of research and methodology, flexibility and adaptability in integrating into a research project, their philosophies and goals for their clients, their awareness of the constraints and requirements for conducting horticulture activities, the available space and resources, scheduling, and any rules or regulations that could affect how the research is designed or implemented. The nature of supplies and tools used in HT must be clearly explained to staff and administrators (Relf, 1978). Having the support of the facility is essential for successful research (Angelis, 1992; Brummel, 1989). Without their full support it would be impossible to receive approval from the clients and/or their families to conduct the research. As many of the staff members work closely with the participants, their attitude can greatly influence the participants’ desire to take part in the HT activities. At some facilities this could also influence the resources and funds that could be made available for activities. The administration and staff should have a clear understanding of research and methodologies, 15
  • 23. including the ability to be flexible and adaptable when integrating with a research program. The facility’s attitude toward the program will determine if the HT will be continued after the research has been completed. For the well-being of the participants who enjoy the horticulture program and for the growth of the field of HT it should always be a goal to find long-term support and interest in continuing the program. It is necessary to discuss the facility’s philosophies and their goals for the clients before designing the research project. The best way to gain support is to design the program to reinforce the center’s objectives for the clients. This way the program will be viewed as an additional ally instead of a threat. No program will survive if it is perceived as detrimental, counterproductive, or unnecessary for the clients’ health or happiness. Because horticulture is so adaptable, many activities could easily be designed to meet the needs of both the facility and its participants. Also, the researcher must carefully and sensitively adjust procedure so to be least disruptive to ongoing curriculum and programming at the facilities. The next step is to agree on the space and resources to be used and the time schedule of the project. At a center where there are many different programs and activities it is important to understand territorial areas. Whether a designated area will be allotted, or whether an area will be shared, discuss the requirements and intentions for the area to be used to prevent misunderstandings or frustrations with other staff. Scheduling both the frequency and duration of the project and receiving approval from all staff will also decrease possible conflicts (Hill and Relf, 1983). Anyone who feels that they are losing space, resources, or time to a new program will understandably become agitated. Disagreements about or competitions for resources could negatively influence the success of the research and limit the support and growth of the program. Rather than feeling that these resources are being taken away the staff members must feel that they are allowing them to be used and shared by others. This will allow them to feel more secure with their position instead of threatened and powerless. Lastly, be sure to discuss the rules and regulations of the facility that are designed to protect the 16
  • 24. participants’ safety and well-being. Breaking any of these rules, even if the purpose for them is not understood, will increase the perceived threat of the research on the participants. This could decrease support both for this research and for any future research projects. Other than the administration and staff, the volunteers can also affect the success of research. Volunteer management is best achieved by good training, communication, and respect. Regardless of a volunteer’s past experience, it will be necessary to offer orientation and training before the project begins. Screening volunteers and evaluating their personalities will allow for a match of their strengths and interests to the necessary tasks. Allowing volunteers to use their strengths and explore their interests will decrease frustration, promote initiative and responsibility, and enhance their enjoyment of the volunteer experience. Discuss the objectives and goals with all volunteers so that the importance and purpose of the research are understood. Provide information and resources for working with the specific population, and if possible, allow time for the volunteers to get to know the participants. This will increase both their self-confidence and their competence. Discuss both the facility’s rules and your own guidelines for working with participants to ensure safety and success. Establish a clear list of responsibilities and expectations of the volunteers. Ensure that all volunteers understand the guidelines, responsibilities, and expectations before the project begins to prevent misunderstandings and confusion. Having volunteers that enjoy their work and are competent with the tasks to which they are assigned will reduce frustration both with the volunteer and with the researcher. Establish comfortable communication with volunteers by emphasizing that their ideas and concerns for the project are important and appreciated. In order for any volunteer to enjoy his/her experience they must feel valuable. Allowing them to express their opinions will not only reinforce their importance to the research, but may also offer new insights and ideas to improve activities. Having volunteers keep a journal of the activities will encourage them to evaluate both the program and their own performance. Reading these journals will allow for a better understanding of the successes and failures that occur. Supporting volunteers with repeated reinforcement and evaluation of their progress will allow the volunteers to appreciate their strengths while discovering ways to work 17
  • 25. 18 around their weaknesses. Respect is an important factor in determining the volunteers’ enjoyment and success. In order for any volunteer to be successful he/she must be enthusiastic for the program and enjoy being a part of the research. Expressing appreciation for their time and energy and acknowledging their efforts are essential for promoting good relations. Lastly, despite the personal satisfaction and enjoyment HT can offer through volunteering, demands on time, energy, and emotions can lead to burn out. Some volunteers may also discover that they simply do not enjoy horticulture or the clients with whom they are working. If this occurs it is best to allow them to leave the program without feelings of guilt or resentment. Convincing unhappy volunteers to continue working with the program will be self defeating, as their lack of energy and enthusiasm will decrease the success of the activity and their participants’ enjoyment. Replacing these volunteers introduces more variables in HT research and according to Flagler (1992) it can also upset participants and administrators and decrease continuity and stability. However, it may ultimately lead to more successful activities due to the correlation between the volunteer’s satisfaction and their performance as a mediator. “Without a competent staff, a program will fall apart or stagnate in a monotonous, uninteresting routine” (Brummel, 1989). Many of the variables introduced by personnel who are involved in HT research can be controlled with careful planning. Most important, simply acknowledging these factors and their effects can change how data is collected and interpreted. Working with people is like working with the weather; as horticultural researchers must deal with environmental factors beyond their control in the field, horticultural therapy researchers must deal with the variables introduced by human nature. When working with people, as in working with the weather, one must always be prepared. By understanding the necessity for good communication many variables introduced by personnel can be prevented or minimized to allow for more successful HT research. Video The bias introduced by personal attitude and beliefs requires an unbiased method of collecting and
  • 26. interpreting data. This may be achieved through the use of video recording. Using video offers both advantages and disadvantages that need to be considered before implementing research, as these obstacles may change how the program is designed. Some factors to be considered are the availability of video equipment, knowing how to use the equipment correctly, and the limitations of using this equipment. Video has many advantages in data collection and evaluation because it allows an unbiased observer to assess the data, it allows more time for assessment, it allows stopping and rewinding to clarify actions, and it can be retained for future analysis or teaching purposes. Some disadvantages are that it can be expensive, it can be difficult to use correctly, it has time and viewing screen limitations, it may not adjust to changes in the activities, and it may not offer adequate clarity for assessing data. The facility where this research took place had a video system with cameras installed in several rooms. A small observation room containing the operation console, viewing screen, and audio control was separate from the rooms where the cameras were located. Because this video system was rarely used, some of the cameras were blocked by props or furniture and the observation room was utilized as a storage closet and copy room. This caused several problems as large boxes and a copy machine left little space to access the video controls and almost no space for adjusting controls or observation. The videos were set up each day before the HT session. Because the observation room was separate from the activity rooms and because the research director was responsible for both the videos and the activities, no adjustments could be made to the video’s volume or viewing area after the activities began. This system was quite useful although not without imperfections. The quality of the videos was excellent, while the sound was variable. Despite the excellent quality of the videos, there were some problems that affected the visibility of the activity and the participants. The cameras did not cover the entire room where the activities took place, so if any participants moved out of the viewing field their activity was not recorded. The cameras could not be adjusted to record these participants without having additional personnel at the controls, which was not available. Another significant problem was created by staff or volunteers standing in front the cameras, not realizing they were blocking all or 19
  • 27. part of the room from view. Lastly, the angle of the cameras view was a problem in several circumstances where a participant was not facing the camera, and his/her activity and facial expression was not observable. The sound quality of the video tapes adds several more considerations. Because the volume in the room could be adjusted it could be raised so that quiet activities and quiet participants could still be heard on tape. At the same time, loud activities in other rooms could also be picked up. When several participants are talking at once it becomes difficult to discern one particular voice on tape. Lastly, because the volume controls in the activity room were separate from the volume control in the observation room, it was easy to forget to turn on the volume for the video resulting in a silent tape. As the video cameras in this system were only located in two rooms at the facility, a video camera on a tripod was used for outdoor activities. This set up could be used for almost any HT research situation. Compared to the indoor video system, the camera on a tripod was easier to operate and offered similar results. The disadvantages associated with limited viewing area and blocked screens also apply, but because the camera is located in the same room as the activities adjustments easily can be made. Before starting a research project where video will be used, a pretest of the equipment will be useful in indicating possible limitations that may change how the research is designed or implemented. It will also be important to train volunteers and staff members who assist with the research to ensure that the activities remain within the camera’s view and that blocked screens are minimized. Scoring video data can be done in many ways. This research project used participation and interaction scores based on charts by Kerrigan and Stevenson (1997) and another by Kuehne (1992). Several other charts are available for other observable behaviors. These charts allow an observer to record both the amount and duration of behaviors, which can be very useful in quantifying qualitative data. These advantages and disadvantages should be considered before choosing video for data collection. 20
  • 28. Knowing the possible problem areas and how to avoid or minimize the effects can change how the research project is designed or implemented. Knowing how this data will be viewed and by whom is also important to ensure that the necessary information is included in the video, and that other necessary data that is not seen on tape can be accounted for. Combining video along with journaling will offer more complete data assessment, and it may also decrease the severity of problems caused by blocked or limited viewing areas, silent or confusing audio quality, or a missed day of recording. Literature Cited Angelis, J. 1992. The genesis of an intergenerational program. Educ. Gerontology 18:317-327. Brummel, S.W. 1989. Developing an intergenerational program. J. Children in Contemp. Society 21 20:119-133. Flagler, J.S. 1992. Horticulture therapy: Potentials for master gardeners, p. In: P.D. Relf (ed.). The Role of Horticulture in Human Well-Being and Social Development: A National Symposium. Timber Press, Portland, OR. Hill, C.O. and P.D. Relf. 1983. Gardening as an outdoor activity in geriatric institutions. Activities, Adaptation and Aging 3(1):47-54. Kerrigan, J. and N.C. Stevenson. 1997. Behavioral study of youth and elders in an intergenerational horticultural therapy program, p. 141-154. In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the Older Adult Population. Hawthorne Press, NY. Kocarnik, R.A. and J.J. Ponzetti. 1991. The advantages and challenges of intergenerational programs in long-term care facilities. J. Gerontological Social Work 16(½):97-107. Kuehne, V.S. 1992. Older adults in intergenerational programs: What are their experiences really like. Activities, Adaptation and Aging 16(4):49-67. Relf, P.D. 1978. Horticulture as a recreational activity. Am. Health Care Assn. J. 4(5):68-70. Seefeldt, C. 1987. Intergenerational programs. Childhood Educ. (October):14-18. Stremmel, A.J., S.S. Travis, P. Kelley-Harrison, and A.D. Hensley. 1994. The perceived benefits and problems associated with intergenerational exchanges in day care settings. The Gerontologist 34(4):513-519. Travis, S.S., A.J. Stremmel, and P.A. Duprey. 1993. Child and adult day care professions converging in the 1990s: Implications for training and research. Educ. Gerontology 19:283-293.
  • 29. INTERGENERATIONAL INTERACTIONS Introduction The goal of this research project was to determine if introducing intergenerational interactions would supplement or detract from HT goals when working with elderly adults or preschool children. These goals included increased autonomy, physical stimulation, mental stimulation, and sensory stimulation. Recently, intergenerational HT programs have been used in order to add to a horticulture program the opportunity for diverse social interactions for special groups that could benefit from such experiences (Abbott et al., 1997; Epstein and Greenberger, 1990; Kerrigan and Stevenson, 1997). Intergenerational HT programs have not been widely used or studied, so the impacts of such programs are unknown. Intergenerational programs have been studied in various situations over the past several years, but the results of these studies are mostly inconclusive or contradictory (Dellman- Jenkins et al., 1991; Seefeldt, 1989). It is important, therefore, to study these interactions to determine if they could become an asset to HT programming, and conversely to see if horticulture is an appropriate activity for intergenerational programming with this population. Previous research indicates several guidelines that can lead to more successful intergenerational programming. These include making the contact beneficial for both groups rather than having one group serve the other (Tice, 1985), designing projects that “have a definite purpose or end product” (Aday et al., 1991), establishing programs that are long-term so the individuals have a chance to establish relationships with one another (Seefeldt, 1989), and ensuring that positive attitudes develop as a result of discovered similarities between the groups (Chapman and Neal, 1990). Seefeldt (1987) proposed recommendations for ensuring successful programs that reinforce these points and include “protecting the prestige of elders as well as children; limiting frustration for both adults and children by arranging for contact that is intimate, not casual; planning for interaction that has integrity and is functional for both groups; and ensuring that contact between old and young is rewarding and pleasant for both groups.” 22
  • 30. According to these guidelines, HT has excellent potential as a focus for intergenerational interaction because it provides benefits for all persons involved, a definite end product to share and discuss, a long-term project with intrinsic rewards, and a common interest in order to establish a connection between the groups. The horticultural activities could also be adapted to meet the needs of both groups involved. However, it is not as certain that intergenerational interactions would complement a HT program. For example, the specific goals of gardening with elderly adults and gardening with children have some different objectives that may make the combination of the two groups inappropriate. The needs of elderly adults and children that are addressed by HT have many similarities including increased autonomy, a sense of wonder and excitement, physical and mental stimulation, social interactions, sensory stimulation, and creative expression. However, the conflicting energy levels of the two groups, and the opposing needs for quiet and cleanliness for elderly adults and freedom to explore and play for children, may result in frustrations and negative attitudes between the two groups when focused around a plant/soil based program. Materials and Methods This research program involved 17 elderly adult clients in the University Adult Day Service and 16 preschool children between the ages of 3 and 5 in the Child Development Laboratory School. The number of participants in the program varied daily due to the health of the clients, the turnover rate at the Adult Day Service, and the agreement that the clients should be allowed to exercise autonomy and decide independently if they wish to participate in each activity. Both the Adult Day Service and the Child Development Lab School are located at Virginia Tech in the same campus building separated only by a small room designed to allow for intergenerational interaction. The facilities are equipped with observational equipment to allow for research. The intergenerational room and the Adult Day Service have video cameras controlled by a central recording system located in a small observational room inside the Adult Day Service. The separate children’s and elderly adults’ activities took place in the intergenerational room, and the intergenerational activities took place in the Adult 23
  • 31. Day Service room which was larger. A video camera on a tripod was used to record activities that took place in the enclosed outdoor patio. The program was set up to compare separate age group activities with intergenerational activities. There were three sessions each week: one for the elderly group, a second one for the children’s group, and a third one that combined both groups. The same activity was completed during all three sessions in one week, with small modifications to make the activity appropriate for each group. Four volunteers assisted with the HT activities. These volunteers were part of a university HT class that required volunteer service. They had varying levels of experience with horticulture and working with children or elderly adults. Two volunteers assisted the children during separate age group activities, and two volunteers similarly assisted with the elderly adult activities. The same four volunteers also assisted with the intergenerational activities. Ten activities (Chapter V) were chosen based on their adaptability and interest for both the children and the elderly adults. Several sources (Gardening Science Manual; Moore, 1989; Moore, 1993; Rothert and Daubert, 1981) were consulted to find activities that would appeal to both children and elderly adults, and also be appropriate during intergenerational activities. The activities were then designed to meet the needs of both groups, making small modifications to the set up or procedures when necessary. The activities designed for children and elderly adults to complete separately focused on individual projects, while the intergenerational activity was to be completed as a small group effort to increase social interaction. The horticulture supplies used in the activities were either collected, bought, or donated by the researchers, volunteers, or the university greenhouses and floral design lab. Activities were chosen that required few or inexpensive materials. The data for each session was collected on video tape over a 10-week period. These video tapes were viewed by the researcher after the study was completed to collect data for analysis. Quantitative data was collected on the attendance and participation of each individual during both separate age group and intergenerational activities. Data also were collected on the interactions between elderly adults 24
  • 32. and preschool children during the intergenerational activities. At the completion of the 10-week project, the video tapes for all sessions were viewed to collect data. The children’s and elderly adults’ separate activities were scored according to attendance and participation. Participation was broken down into 3 levels- ‘O’ being present but inactive, ‘I’ being active with direct volunteer assistance, and ‘II’ being independent participation. These scores varied with the difficulty level of the activity, activity set up, and the individual’s ability. Participation scores were tallied in total times, so the same individual may have received “O”, “I”, or “II” for the same activity with varying amounts of time spent in each category of participation. Attendance percentages were calculated for the elderly adults based on the number of the total participants who attended the activity that day. The percentages were not available for the children because records of the total participants at the facility that day were not available. Average participation time was calculated by dividing the total time of the activity by the number of participants. Type of participation percentages were calculated by tallying the total participation times for each type of participation, then dividing these numbers by the total time of the activity. The video tapes were also used to collect data on interactions between the two groups during intergenerational activities. Interaction scores had four categories: ‘I’ being non-verbal interaction, ‘II’ being one-directional verbal interaction (one participant addressed another without getting a response), ‘III’ being two-directional interaction (conversation), and ‘IV’ being two-directional interaction with physical assistance during the activity. All data were organized into charts to compare separate and intergenerational activities, and to determine overall trends that developed during the 10-week period. Inferential statistical analysis was not feasible because of the small sample size, the short period of time of the study, and the large number of human variables did not allow for a controlled data base. This research can not provide definitive answers of the success of intergenerational activities. Rather, it serves to identify variables that affect success, trends that might indicate potential success of intergenerational programs, and directions for future research. 25
  • 33. Results and Discussion Children’s Separate Activities During children’s separate activities (Table 1), few ‘O’ scores were observed as inactive children usually left the room where the horticulture activity took place. ‘I’ or ‘II’ scores were dependent on the activity and how it was set up. Activities that were difficult or unfamiliar resulted in high ‘I’ ratings as they required the most assistance. Activities that involved familiar tasks such as cutting, glueing, drawing, or putting soil in pots required little volunteer assistance and resulted in high ‘II’ scores. Table 1. Attendance, average time, and percentage of time in three levels of participation for children during separate activities. Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Name Design Seeds I Cuttings Seeds II Terrarium Frames Scare- Corsage Planting tags crow 26 # present 13 9 15 11 10 14 8 8 11 7 average time in minutes 10m. 14.3m. 10.3m. 8.7m. 7.9m. 7.1m. 14.4m. 14.3m. 6m. 14.1m. type of partici-pation in % 0- 1% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% 0- 0% I-73.7 I-26.7 I-75.3 I-100 I-100 I-100 I-8.3 I-28.5 I-100 I-0 II-25.3 II-73.3 II-24.7 II-0 II-0 II-0 II-91.8 II-71.5 II-0 II-100 The average participation time for children’s separate activities depended on the set up of the activity. Simple activities that required less direct assistance and allowed the children to work independently (# 2, 7, 8, 10) also encouraged them to work longer. Activities that involved use of fine motor skills (#1, 3) also required longer time for completion and moderate direct assistance. The assistance required depended on the children’s abilities and development of fine motor skills. More difficult activities, specifically the ordered step activities (# 4, 5, 6, 9) had low average participation times. These highly structured activities where volunteers worked one-on-one with the children resulted in a shorter period of time needed for participants to complete the activity. The number of participants in children’s separate age group activities decreased after the first six
  • 34. weeks of the study. During these first six weeks, the Child Development Lab School started the day with a group time where all the participants sat down and talked about the activities taking place that day. During this group time, the children showed a lot of enthusiasm for the horticulture activities and the participation was never lower than 70%. However, during the last four weeks of the study, the group time was no longer conducted each day. Without an introduction to the horticulture activities, it became more difficult to encourage children to take part in the horticulture activities which were set up in the room next door. During these last four weeks, the children’s participation was never lower than 50% of the children present that day. Elderly Adult Separate Activities The elderly adult activities (Table 2) had ‘O’ scores because many participants either could not leave the table without assistance or because they could not work on their own while the volunteer was assisting another participant. ‘I’ and ‘II’ scores were primarily related to the individuals’ physical abilities. The few participants with greater physical skills could work alone after a brief demonstration of the activity, and scored primarily ‘II’s. Individuals who were not able to work without direct assistance due to physical limitations scored mostly ‘I’s. Table 2. Attendance, average time, and percentage of time in three levels of participation for elderly adults during separate activities. Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Name Design Seeds I Cuttings Seeds II Terrarium Frames Scare- Corsage Planting tags crow 27 # present 9/12 5/10 9/10 7/11 7/10 8/11 6/13 5/11 5/8 3/8 75% 50% 90% 64% 70% 73% 46% 45% 62.5% 37.5% average time in minutes 25.1m. 26.2m. 19.6m. 31.4m. 18.3m. 15.9m. 24m. 21.6m. 22.2m. 13.7m. type of partici-pation in % 0-50.4 0-37.4 0-61.6 0-28.9 0-39.3 0-30.2 0-39.3 0-24 0-49.2 0-38.3 I-36.3 I-57.6 I-36.3 I-49.3 I-54.7 I-69.8 I-51.9 I-58.4 I-50.8 I-61.7 II-13.1 II-5 II-2.1 II-21.9 II-6 II-0 II-8.8 II-17.6 II-0 II-0 The elderly adults’ participation was less dependent on how the activity was set up and more dependent on each individuals’ physical abilities. Because of this, the activities did not show as much
  • 35. variation in the scores as the children’s activities. The average participation time varied with the amount of materials available for the activity. During activities with unlimited materials available for each participant (# 1, 2, 4, 7, 8) many older adults participated longer. Activities where available materials were limited (#3, 5, 6, 10) showed a decreased average time as participants were limited to the number of products they could complete. The only exception was activity #9. Materials were limited for this activity, but the high difficulty level increased the time spent completing the activity. Intergenerational Activities Participation scores also were used to assess individuals during the intergenerational activities. These data allowed for a comparison of the total participation between separate and intergenerational group activities to indicate the success of certain activities either in separate or combined groups. Intergenerational participation scores showed several changes in participation due to the joining of the two groups. These changes in scores appear to have been influenced primarily by a reduction in individual assistance available from the volunteers during the activity. No data were collected on week 5 because the video equipment was not properly started during that intergenerational activity. To compensate for this missing intergenerational data the week 5 separate age group activity was not included in comparison data. Children’s intergenerational scores (Table 3) show an initial increase in ‘O’s. This is probably due to the volunteers’ inexperience at encouraging participation from all participants. These “O” scores disappeared after the volunteers gained more experience working with their groups. Children’s scores also show an increase in ‘I’s and decrease in ‘II’s compared to separate group activities (Table 1) with only two exceptions (#8, 9). This is most likely due to the increased structure of the intergenerational activities. In order to encourage interactions between the two groups, activities were designed to promote team work and cooperation between the generations. Independent activity usually decreased interactions, so participants were encouraged to work together rather than on their own and activities were redesigned as group participation efforts. The two exceptions (#8, 9) showed an increase in independent activity because of the simplicity of the activities. These activities were less 28
  • 36. structured and allowed the participants to work independently but in a group setting. Table 3. Attendance, average time, and percentage of time in three levels of participation for children during intergenerational activities. Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Board Sand Seeds I Hanging Labels Terrarium Frames Concrete Arrange- Planting Garden Basket ments 29 # present 14 12 9 7 ND 7 8 10 5 9 average time in minutes 16.8 9.1m. 15.2 10.1m. ND 9.9m. 12.1m. 18.3m. 13.6m. 15.3m. m. m. type of partici-pation in % 0-5% 0-11.6 0-0.6 0-0 ND 0-0 0-0 0-0 0-0 0-0 I-95 I-45.1 I-99.4 I-100 I-100 I-100 I-0 I-31.6 I-100 II-0 II-43.3 II-0 II-0 II-0 II-0 II-100 II-68.4 II-0 At the same time the children’s average participation time increased for all but two intergenerational activities. Because the children were seated at tables and less direct assistance was available, the children required more time completing the activities in intergenerational groups. On the days when intergenerational activities took place, the activity started before the scheduled group time on that day. These activities also took place in the Adult Day Service room, which was two rooms away from the Child Development Lab room. It was more difficult to encourage the children to participate in activities on these days. Although it is possible that fewer children chose to participate in these activities because they did not enjoy the intergenerational group, the high number of variables make it difficult to identify the exact reason. Total children’s participation comparing separate age group and intergenerational activities are summarized in Table 4. Table 4. Total children’s participation comparing cumulative time spent in different levels of participation during separate and intergenerational activities. 0 I II Total Participation Average minutes minutes minutes minutes Occurrences minutes Separate 1m. 536m. 465m. 1002m. 96 10.4 m. Intergenerational 26m. 807m. 273m. 1106m. 81 13.7m.
  • 37. Elderly Adults The elderly adults’ participation scores (Table 5) show an increase in ‘O’ scores in 8 of the 10 activities compared to separate activities (Table 2), while the other 2 stay the same. This was due to two separate observed factors. First, in some situations, ‘O’ scores increase because less direct assistance was available from the volunteers, and without the needed assistance, certain of the elderly adults could not participate in the activity. This was often observed with elderly adults who had limited physical abilities, and for elderly adults with Alzheimer’s disease. The second reason for the increase in ‘O’ scores was observed in elderly adults with higher physical capabilities. Many of these participants did not join in when children were present because they did not wish to get in the way or prevent the children from participating. Many of these elderly adults who did not need direct assistance to participate simply sat back and watched the children complete in the activity. Table 5. Attendance, average time, and percentage of time in three levels of participation for elderly adults during intergenerational activities. Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Board Sand Seeds I Hanging Labels Terrarium Frames Concrete Arrange- Planting Garden Basket ments 30 # present 7/11 7/10 8/8 8/9 ND 8/13 10/11 3/12 7/12 7/11 64% 70% 100% 89% 62% 91% 25% 58% 64% average time in minutes 24.6m. 16.1m. 17.1m. 10.8m. ND 13.1m. 15.1m. 16.7m. 17.3m. 28.4m. type of partici-pation in % 0-70.7 0-75.3 0-48 0-70.5 ND 0-37.1 0-61 0-50.3 0-46.7 0-43.6 I-16.4 I-10.4 I-39.5 I-17 I-50.4 I-19 I-0 I-21.3 I-16.9 II-12.9 II-14.3 II-12.5 II-12.5 II-12.5 II-20 II-49.7 II-32 II-39.6 Another observed problem that affected both the elderly adults with and without physical limitations was excessive direct assistance for the children from the staff or volunteers. During the first several activities, many staff or volunteers worked closely with the active children to keep them involved in the activity, and the adults who needed assistance or encouragement to participate were ignored or given limited access to the activity. Because the children were more vocal with their assistance requests and more willing to leave when they were unsatisfied, the staff and volunteers focused their attention on these children and let the elderly adults remain silent and inactive. With careful coaching
  • 38. and experience in working with both populations and in meeting their needs more efficiently, the problems of excessive direct assistance for the children decreased, increasing the assistance and attention available for the elderly adults. Throughout the course of the study, the ‘O’ scores decreased which may be attributed to the assistance of the volunteers who encouraged the elderly adults to work with the children rather than watch, and with the decrease in excessive direct assistance for the children. Three of the four early activities (#1, 2, 4) had over 70% ‘O’s, while of the last five activities three (#6, 9, 10) showed no more than 47% ‘O’, with two slightly higher scores (#7, 8) which were a result of the activity set up. During activity #7, the participants worked independently rather than in groups and encouraged interaction and participation was low. During activity #8, most elderly adults were unable to reach the wheelbarrow that the children crowded around and therefore watched, instead of participated, in the activity. Along with the increase in ‘O’ scores, the elderly adults also show an increase in ‘II’s. This is due to the limited amount of direct assistance available during the activity. Thus it was observed that without direct assistance the ‘I’ scores decrease. The ‘O’ and ‘II’ scores increase because in response to the decrease in direct assistance the elderly adults would either not participate or work on their own. Total elderly adults’ participation comparing separate age group and intergenerational activities is summarized in Table 6. Table 6. Total elderly adults’ participation comparing cumulative time spent in different levels of participation during separate and intergenerational activities. 0 I II Total Participation Average minutes minutes minutes minutes Occurrences minutes Separate 490m. 677m. 117m. 1284m. 57 22.5m. Intergenerational 588m. 253m. 293m. 1134m. 65 17.4m. Intergenerational Interactions During the course of this 10-week study the total percentage of interaction time between the generations during activities increased over time (Table 7) with two exceptions (#7, 8). Several factors appear to have contributed to this increase. The two groups became more familiar with each 31
  • 39. other and more comfortable interacting, and the volunteers became more comfortable working with both groups and encouraging participation and interaction from everyone at their table. Most of the intergenerational interaction occurred between the 4 and 5 year old children and the elderly adults with the highest cognitive and physical abilities. This may be because the 3 year old children and the elderly adults with Alzheimer’s disease or other limited cognitive and physical abilities needed a large amount of direct assistance to complete the activities, and therefore worked with the volunteers more than with the other participants. Table 7. Intergenerational interaction for elderly adults and preschool children during intergenerational activities. Activity 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Board Sand Seeds Cuttings Seeds Terrar- Frames Concrete Arrang- Planting garden I II iums ing 32 I - non-verbal 12m. 0m. 0m. 2m. ND 33m. 12m. 0m. 24m. 26m. II - one-direction verbal 8m. 15m. 14m. 8m. ND 7m. 2m. 6m. 22m. 16m. III - two-direction verbal 4m. 6m. 28m. 4m. ND 0m. 6m. 0m. 2m. 8m. IV - two-direction verbal + physical assistance 0m. 0m. 0m. 8m. ND 0m. 0m. 0m. 2m. 62m. Total 24m. 21m. 42m. 22m. ND 40m. 20m. 6m. 50m. 112m. Total time 407.4 221m. 264m 157m. ND 174m. 247.8m. 233m. 189m. 337m. for all m. partici-pants Percent of interaction time 6% 10% 15% 14% ND 23% 8% 3% 26% 33% Several factors that appeared to reduce the amount of interaction in all activities were excessive staff or volunteer direct assistance for individuals rather than team or group encouragement, a lack of experience or discomfort in working with either elderly adults or children, or inappropriate activity set up and material distribution. The low interaction scores of activity #7 and 8 were probably due to the nature of the activities. During activity #7, participants worked individually at the same table.
  • 40. Without a shared product to encourage cooperation very little interaction took place. During activity #8, the participants stood around a wheelbarrow to mix concrete and functioned as independent workers rather than a team. In addition, many of the elderly adults could not reach the wheelbarrow and chose not to participate in the activity. Of all these activities, the horticulture plant-based activities (# 3, 4, 6, 9, 10) show greater percentage of interaction time than the craft-type activities (#1, 2, 7, 8). This indicates that horticulture may be more useful than craft-type activities for intergenerational programs with a goal of increased interaction and relationship development. Conclusions The HT goals of working with young children or elderly adults have several similarities, but also several differences. The decrease in children’s attendance and elderly adults’ participation during intergenerational activities compared to separate activities indicates that introducing intergenerational interactions may detract from the ability to accomplish HT goals. This is especially seen with the elderly adult group. Participants with physical limitations or Alzheimer’s disease who require a high amount of direct assistance could not participate as well during intergenerational activities. The horticultural plant-based intergenerational activities showed more success than horticultural crafts at encouraging social interactions between the two groups as reflected by this study. The craft-type activities involved simple skills such as cutting, glueing, or writing, These types of tasks encourage individual activity more than group activity. The horticulture plant-based activities involved less individualized tasks and more group effort and team work for completion. For intergenerational programs that aim to increase social interaction and relationship development between generations, horticulture with live plants may help attain these goals more effectively than craft-type activities. Because most interaction was observed between the older children and the elderly adults with more 33
  • 41. physical and cognitive abilities, this research may indicate that two groups in this study may not be the best combination for intergenerational programs in this type of program. The younger children and elderly adults with limited physical and cognitive abilities who had higher demands for attention from the staff and volunteers were not as strongly or positively influenced by the intergenerational interactions. The age and activity level of the participants influenced their type of interaction during intergenerational activities. The decision to implement intergenerational HT programming will depend upon the age, developmental or ability level, and goals for the individuals involved. Individuals with high demands for physical assistance during activities may not benefit from intergenerational activities that demand high levels attention and support from volunteers or staff. The high level of variables and compounding factors that are introduced when working with young children, elderly adults, staff, and volunteers limits the ability to reach clear conclusions and simply provides direction for conducting programs and future research. Because of the limited resources available for this study, the results of this data are not conclusive evidence of the desirable or undesirable outcome of the addition of intergenerational experiences to the HT program. Rather, the results of this study serve to make recommendations for program implementation and further research. More conclusive results could be obtained if a source of participants could be identified that would provide a larger and more random sample population, the use of a separate control group for comparison, and a longer duration of study. Intergenerational research involves many variables that affect the outcome of the study. Allowing more time to train volunteers and gain experience in working with both elderly adults and children will increase the success of activities. Use of professionals with training, experience, and a long-term position would provide the greatest reduction in variance due to personnel. Using preliminary tests to gain experience when using video to collect data will decrease the effects of video limitations. Video can also allow for data analysis by an unbiased observer who is ignorant of the research hypothesis. 34
  • 42. Guidelines Based on this research several guidelines were found to be useful in encouraging successful intergenerational interactions during HT activities. Activities: 1. Appeal to both age groups’ interest 2. Are developmentally and functionally appropriate for the groups involved 3. Group activities are preferred to individual activities done in groups 4. Have a definite start and end 5. Have a definite shared goal Set up: 1. Small groups allow for easy, more intimate contact 2. Have all participants seated at table, interspersed by age 3. Introduce participants or have them introduce themselves 4. Have a definite task for each person; emphasize importance of all participation 5. Encourage 1-on-1 interaction between participants 6. Prevent distractions from noise, discomfort, etc. 35 Staff/Volunteers: 1. Have a suitable ratio of staff/volunteers to participants (Recommended maximum of 4 participants for 1 volunteer) 2. Be familiar with needs of both groups, with appropriate expectations according to the developmental or functional level of the individuals 3. Avoid over-direction or excessive involvement with participants 4. Direct activity with positive attitude 5. Encourage interactions among participants 6. Keep a journal for activity- and self-evaluation 7. Establish good communication for feedback, input, etc. Interactions: 1. Do not force interactions or participation
  • 43. 2. Best when either elderly adult or child can assist each other or work together in groups equally 3. Best for individuals that do not require constant direct assistance from staff/volunteer Recommendations for Future Research Research is in its initial stages both for HT and for intergenerational programming. This research project identified several areas that need to be addressed in future studies. 36 Activities & How the set up affects the ability to reach the activity goals & How plant-based activities compare to craft-type activities for different populations Volunteers & What training will produce the best results from volunteers & What time and input expectations are appropriate from volunteers & How volunteer turnover could affect the research & What the suitable ratio of staff/volunteers to participants is for groups with different abilities, needs, and limitations Data Collection & What the effectiveness is of different data collection techniques such as journaling, video tape, or unbiased and uninvolved observers & How the data collection methods can alter the results or conclusions Intergenerational Groups & What the effects are of intergenerational HT interactions involving different combinations of ages and abilities of children and elderly adults
  • 44. & How alterations to the activity, tools, or environment may increase participation and interaction from elderly adults with severe physical limitations or Alzheimer’s disease Literature Cited Abbott, G., V. Cochran, and A.A. Clair. 1997. Innovations in intergenerational programs for persons who are elderly: The role of horticultural therapy in a multidisciplinary approach, p. 27-38. In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the Older Adult. Hawthorne Press, NY. Aday, R.H. 1991. Youth’s attitudes toward the elderly: The impact of intergenerational partners. J. 37 Applied Gerontology 10(3):372-384. Chapman, N.J. and M.B. Neal. 1990. The effects of intergenerational experiences on adolescents and older adults. The Gerontologist 30(6):825-832. Dellman-Jenkins, M., D. Lambert, and D. Fruit. 1991. Fostering preschoolers’ prosocial behaviors toward the elderly: The effect of an intergenerational program. Educ. Gerontology 17:21-32. Epstein, S.G. and D.S. Greenberger. 1990. Nurturing plants, children, and older individuals: Intergenerational horticultural therapy. J. Therapeutic Hort. 5:16-19. Gardening Science Manual, New York Board of Education publication. Kerrigan, J. and N.C. Stevenson. 1997. Behavioral study of youth and elders in an intergenerational horticultural therapy program, p. 141-154. In: Wells (ed.). Horticultural Therapy and the Older Adult Population. Hawthorne Press, NY. Moore, B. 1989. Growing with Gardening. Univ. of NC Press, Chapel Hill, NC. Moore, R.C. 1993. Plants for Play. MIG Communications, Berkley, CA. Rothert, E.R. and J.R. Daubert. 1981. Horticultural Therapy for Nursing Homes, Senior Centers, Retirement Living. Chicago Horticultural Society, Chicago, IL. Seefeldt, C. 1987. Intergenerational programs. Childhood Educ. (October):14-18. Seefeldt, C. 1989. Intergenerational programs: Impact on attitudes. J. of Children in Contemp. Society 20:185-194. Tice, C.H. 1985. Perspectives on intergenerational initiatives: Past, present, and future. Children Today 14(5):6-11.
  • 45. HORTICULTURE THERAPY ACTIVITIES FOR PRESCHOOL CHILDREN, ELDERLY ADULTS, AND INTERGENERATIONAL GROUPS Introduction These activities were designed as part of an intergenerational research project. They consist of 10 different HT activities that can be adapted for preschool children, elderly adults, and intergenerational groups. The design of the research allowed preschool children and elderly adults to be observed both during separate age group activities and during similar intergenerational activities. Each week during the 10-week study involved one day for the children’s activity, a second day for the elderly adults’ activity, and a third day for the intergenerational activity. The same horticulture activity was used for each group’s session during one week, with slight modifications to make the activity appropriate for each group. As a result of this study (Chapter IV), 10 activities have been tested for use by children, elderly adults, and intergenerational groups. The children’s group testing these activities consisted of 16 children between the ages of 3 and 5. The children had similar skill levels with some small variation in tasks such as spelling or using fine motor skills (cutting with scissors, planting seeds, etc.). The children were part of an afternoon day care program at the University Child Development Lab School which met for three hours three days a week. The elderly adult group consisted of 12 regular attending participants age 65 or older. The elderly adults had various levels of speech, cognitive, and physical abilities. The elderly adults were part of the University Adult Day Service day care which met for 9 hours every weekday. These same two groups of children and adults attended the intergenerational activities. Participation in the horticulture activities was optional for all the participants. The actual number of children and elderly adults that participated in each activity varied due to absences or preference for other activities at the centers. 38
  • 46. Activity Goals The goals of HT for children and elderly adults have several similarities including increased autonomy, physical and mental stimulation, social interaction, sensory stimulation, creative expression, and a sense of wonder and excitement. However, conflicting energy levels of the two groups and the opposing needs for quiet and cleanliness for elderly adults and freedom to explore and play for children could potentially result in frustrations and negative attitudes between the two groups during intergenerational activities. The goals of intergenerational programs include increased social contact and interaction, and relationship development between different generations. The specific goals for all activities were participation, self-motivation, and fun. Additional goals included education and skill enhancement for the children’s group, exercise and skill enhancement for the elderly adult group, and social interaction for the intergenerational group. Testing of Activities The Child Development Lab School and the Adult Day Service are located in the same campus building separated only by a small room designed to allow for intergenerational activities. An outdoor enclosed patio was used for activities during warm, sunny weather. The intergenerational room and the Adult Day Service were equipped with video cameras to record activities. A camera on a tripod was used to record outdoor activities for data collection and analysis. Four volunteers assisted with the HT activities. These volunteers were part of a university HT class that required volunteer service. They had varying levels of experience with horticulture and working with children or elderly adults. Two volunteers assisted with the children’s activities both during separate and intergenerational activities, and two volunteers similarly assisted with the elderly adult activities. The activities were chosen to be appropriate and interesting for both groups involved. These activities were arranged in a progressive order, starting with getting to know each other (Making Nametags), 39