SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 10
Start with What theStudent Knows or withWhat the Student
CanImagine?
It is almost universally accepted among educators that the best
way to present new material to students is to connect it to what
they already know. Mr. Egan questions this principle and
suggests that students' imaginations might offer a much more
fertile starting point for learning.
By Kieran Egan
IT IS COMMONLY argued that one of the securest findings of
educational research is that new information, to be best
understood, must be attached to knowledge the student already
has. Formulations of this finding have been various, but it has
been a staple of educational thinking from the time of John
Dewey -- and before him of Herbert Spencer -- to the recently
published National Research Council monograph How People
Learn: Bridging Research and Practice.1
I wish to suggest that the common principle of "starting where
the student is" may be both inadequate and restrictive in ways
not often discussed. In its place, I suggest we might sensibly
adopt a second principle of "asking what the student can
imagine." We can pose this question at any point in the learning
process as a starting point for further inquiry.
To show the long influence of the first principle, consider its
early articulation in the work of Herbert Spencer and its use by
John Dewey and others in shaping the curriculum -- in
particular in giving a foundation for the social studies
curriculum. Spencer argued that children's early and simple
experience had to form the basis for all future learning and that
there must be a regular and orderly progression from what is
already familiar to what is slightly less familiar -- an expansion
"by slow degrees to impressions most nearly allied."2 This
principle is believed by nearly every teacher and professor of
education I have encountered. Most people assume that it is so
obviously true that even to question it suggests a degree of
nuttiness.
The education reformers of the late 19th and early 20th
centuries encouraged teachers to rethink their ways of
presenting new material to students in their particular
disciplines in accordance with this principle. Thus elementary
mathematics would begin from the experience that children had
with fruit or with games such as marbles, and language studies
would begin with the forms of expression with which children
would already be familiar, rather than with a topic like abstract
grammar.
But subjects such as mathematics and language were still not
fully articulated with the meaningful daily interactions of children
with their local environments -- "what the student knows." What
was further proposed was a new, central curriculum area -- the
social studies. This was to be a subject that would begin with
the material of children's everyday experience -- with
themselves and their families and with their neighborhoods and
communities. Gradually, student learning would expand from
this meaningful core of personal experience to less familiar
knowledge, until, in the end, the whole universe of knowledge
could be understood as an expansion from what was most vivid
and meaningful to the child. The social studies curriculum was
designed to tie all the knowledge being learned in other
curriculum areas to the child's experience.
So we must start with what is most profoundly known by the
student and build new knowledge on that basis. David Ausubel
declared, "If I had to reduce all of educational psychology to
just one principle, I would say this: The most important single
factor influencing learning is what the learner already knows.
Ascertain this and teach him accordingly."3 Apart from this
slightly odd way of putting Spencer's principle, I think there are
four reasons why we might be wary of accepting it.
• First, if this is a fundamental principle of human learning,
there is no way the process can begin.
• Second, if novelty is a problem for human learners,
reducing the amount of the novelty doesn't solve the problem.
And if we can manage some novelty, why can't we manage
more? Perhaps Spencer's "most nearly allied" is far too
conservative.
• The third objection is less directed at the principle than at
the way it has been invariably interpreted in education and
particularly in the construction of the elementary social studies
curriculum. It is assumed that what children know first and best
will be the details of their everyday social lives. That is, it is
assumed that children's thinking is simple, concrete, and
engaged with their local experience. But, as I will elaborate
below, children also have imaginations and emotions, and
these, too, connect with the world. If children's minds are
supposed to be restricted to the everyday details of their social
lives, why are they full of monsters, talking middle-class rabbits,
and titanic emotions? We cannot sensibly explain Peter
Rabbit's appeal in terms of its "familiar family setting"4 when it
involves a safe forest, a dangerous cultivated garden, death so
close, and so on.
• Fourth, and this appeals to old-fashioned intuition, a few
moments of reflection should make clear that no one's
understanding of the world expands according to this principle
of gradual content association.
Now, given the almost universal acceptance of this principle
and the fact that these four reasons will have little impact on
most of those who believe it, I should add what seems to me
the reason such ideas survive so tenaciously. The principle
survives because, like many such knowledge claims in
education, it is a mixture of analytic truth and empirical
generalization. That is, at some level, the principle is true
simply because people define its terms to mean something that
can't be other than true. It is understood to mean something
like "You don't know whatever you don't know, and if you learn
something new, it has to fit in with what you can comprehend."
If you don't speak Chinese and are told the solution to the three
pagodas puzzle in Chinese, you will be unable to understand it.
If you do speak Chinese and know what pagodas are and
understand the puzzle and fulfill other prerequisites, you will be
able to understand the solution. At this level, the principle is
certainly true, but it isn't very helpful. What would make it
interesting are reliable empirical generalizations. That is, there
should be research showing conditions that constrain learning
that are something other than logical truths, and these are not
thick on the ground.
Well, that's a bit clotted, trying to say too much too briefly. But it
suggests why one might come to question the common
interpretation of starting where the student is. If we mean it as
simply an analytic truth, it is of no particular interest to
educators. If we mean it as an empirical claim, as it has been
taken at least since Spencer's time, then it is inadequate for the
reasons given above.
Now let us consider what might be implied by taking seriously
the principle of what the student can imagine. For obvious
reasons, there has not been much research on students'
imaginations, and yet they are clearly central to students'
learning. But ignoring the imagination because our research
methods have difficulty coming to grips with it is somewhat self-
defeating.
First, I want to argue that the imagination is the ability to think
of things as possibly being so.5 This odd ability, which seems
to be most energetically active in our early years, is clearly not
some casual, frothy part of our mind's functioning, to be blown
away with the growth of rationality. It is a hard-working core of
children's thinking.6
Second, I want to look in some detail at just three features of
children's imaginative engagement with the world and show
how they challenge the principle of "starting with what the
student knows," which has had so significant an impact on
teaching and curriculum thinking. The first feature concerns the
story form, which children engage so enthusiastically very early
in life and in all cultures and in all times that we know about.
The second concerns the kinds of abstract and affective
concepts that give structure to the fantasy stories common
across the world -- such concepts as good/bad,
brave/cowardly, rich/poor, anxiety/security, and so on, with all
the freight they carry for human cultures. The third follows from
the content of children's fantasy stories and why they are so full
of creatures like Peter Rabbit, a talking, middle-class animal
that cannot exist.
Clearly, storytellers don't pay much attention to the principle of
starting with what children know, in the way that the designers
and defenders of the elementary social studies curriculum do.
Storytellers might begin boldly with galaxies far, far away and
long, long ago, if it suits them, confident that their audience will
make sense of the content. They can introduce strange
characters and weird situations, like Harry Potter's or Frodo's,
as long as they build their narratives on the kind of abstract
concepts students are familiar with: good/bad, brave/cowardly,
anxiety/security, and so on.
Consider how children learn about the temperature continuum
conceptually. They begin with the concepts "hot" and "cold," as
these are both logically and empirically the first concepts that
can be meaningful -- "hot" being hotter than the child's body
and "cold" being colder. Then they can elaborate or mediate
these concepts by learning the concept "warm," for example.
Then they can learn an array of other temperature terms in the
context created by the first terms that form the ends of the
continuum. Later, they will learn to use abstract terms, like
thermometer numbers, to refer to temperature.
This procedure, which is very effective in dealing with the
material world, creates problems when applied to concepts that
have no mediating categories, such as life and death or nature
and culture or human and animal. Nevertheless, the world of
children's imaginative lives is filled with generated mediations
between these logically discrete concepts. If there is nothing
between life and death, we invent ghosts that are to "life" and
"death" as "warm" is to "hot" and "cold." Between nature and
culture, we invent that menagerie of talking middle-class
animals that fill the fantasy stories of children and the mythic
stories of the world.
Something serious is going on here that has little to do with
starting where the student is and gradually building on the
knowledge the student has -- except if we define these
conditions as analytic truths. The connections are made by
metaphorical leaps, not by logical connections. This article is a
meditation on what aspects of our assumptions about learning
and curriculum we might need to give up if we acknowledge the
importance of students' imaginations. For teaching, we need
not be constrained by trying to make content associations with
knowledge students already have, for there are other ways of
expanding knowledge. For the curriculum, we need no longer
be constrained to tie knowledge to the everyday experience of
students, which can be very dreary for them, but can recognize
that their imaginations allow much freedom in how they can go
about grasping the universe of knowledge.
Let me conclude by pointing out the obvious -- that I am not
arguing for ignoring students' prior knowledge and everyday
experience. Rather, I am arguing that these have been taken
as implying greater restrictions on children's learning and
curriculum possibilities than is warranted when we consider
their imaginative lives. We can start with what they can
imagine.
________________________________________
1. Suzanne M. Donovan, John D. Bransford, and James W.
Pellegrino, eds., How People Learn: Bridging Research and
Practice (Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press, 1999).
2. Herbert Spencer, Education: Intellectual, Moral, and Physical
(London: G. Manwaring, 1861), p. 82.
3. David P. Ausubel, Educational Psychology: A Cognitive View
(London: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1968), p. 18.
4. Arthur N. Applebee, The Child's Concept of Story (Chicago:
University of Chicago Press, 1978), p. 75.
5. Alan R. White, The Language of Imagination (Oxford:
Blackwell, 1990).
6. Mary Warnock, Imagination (London: Faber, 1976).

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Ähnlich wie Start with what the student knows or with what the student can imagine?

Dr. Muhsinah L Morris Teaching Philosophy 2015
Dr. Muhsinah L Morris Teaching Philosophy 2015Dr. Muhsinah L Morris Teaching Philosophy 2015
Dr. Muhsinah L Morris Teaching Philosophy 2015
Muhsinah Morris, Ph.D
 
Why Dont Students Like School: Part 1
Why Dont Students Like School: Part 1Why Dont Students Like School: Part 1
Why Dont Students Like School: Part 1
mark.richardson
 
ProPhilo presentation
ProPhilo presentationProPhilo presentation
ProPhilo presentation
Isabel Laza
 
Science & Elementary Education
Science & Elementary EducationScience & Elementary Education
Science & Elementary Education
guestfd7e92
 
Guided Response Criteria (respond to a minimum of 2 classmates)1..docx
Guided Response Criteria (respond to a minimum of 2 classmates)1..docxGuided Response Criteria (respond to a minimum of 2 classmates)1..docx
Guided Response Criteria (respond to a minimum of 2 classmates)1..docx
isaachwrensch
 
5. constructivist learning theories 6. constructivist teaching principles m...
5. constructivist learning theories 6. constructivist teaching principles   m...5. constructivist learning theories 6. constructivist teaching principles   m...
5. constructivist learning theories 6. constructivist teaching principles m...
AhL'Dn Daliva
 
Basic Assumptions and Theoretical Framework of Inquiry as a Teaching strategy...
Basic Assumptions and Theoretical Framework of Inquiry as a Teaching strategy...Basic Assumptions and Theoretical Framework of Inquiry as a Teaching strategy...
Basic Assumptions and Theoretical Framework of Inquiry as a Teaching strategy...
gillenpugs
 
Comm1609WebSupplement_TB.PDF
Comm1609WebSupplement_TB.PDFComm1609WebSupplement_TB.PDF
Comm1609WebSupplement_TB.PDF
Tom Briggs
 

Ähnlich wie Start with what the student knows or with what the student can imagine? (20)

WHY JUST TEACH ART: THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE HIPPOCAMPUS
WHY JUST TEACH ART: THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE HIPPOCAMPUSWHY JUST TEACH ART: THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE HIPPOCAMPUS
WHY JUST TEACH ART: THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE HIPPOCAMPUS
 
WHY JUST TEACH ART: THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE HIPPOCAMPUS
WHY JUST TEACH ART: THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE HIPPOCAMPUSWHY JUST TEACH ART: THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE HIPPOCAMPUS
WHY JUST TEACH ART: THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE HIPPOCAMPUS
 
Use this one-time passcode to verify your identity on Scribd. The passcode wi...
Use this one-time passcode to verify your identity on Scribd. The passcode wi...Use this one-time passcode to verify your identity on Scribd. The passcode wi...
Use this one-time passcode to verify your identity on Scribd. The passcode wi...
 
Dr. Muhsinah L Morris Teaching Philosophy 2015
Dr. Muhsinah L Morris Teaching Philosophy 2015Dr. Muhsinah L Morris Teaching Philosophy 2015
Dr. Muhsinah L Morris Teaching Philosophy 2015
 
What We Owe Children by Caleb Gattegno
What We Owe Children by Caleb GattegnoWhat We Owe Children by Caleb Gattegno
What We Owe Children by Caleb Gattegno
 
Why Dont Students Like School: Part 1
Why Dont Students Like School: Part 1Why Dont Students Like School: Part 1
Why Dont Students Like School: Part 1
 
Learning beyon d the classroom
Learning beyon d the classroomLearning beyon d the classroom
Learning beyon d the classroom
 
ProPhilo presentation
ProPhilo presentationProPhilo presentation
ProPhilo presentation
 
Nusrath online asmt
Nusrath online asmtNusrath online asmt
Nusrath online asmt
 
Aep assignment
Aep assignmentAep assignment
Aep assignment
 
Module 1 lecture
Module 1 lectureModule 1 lecture
Module 1 lecture
 
Science & Elementary Education
Science & Elementary EducationScience & Elementary Education
Science & Elementary Education
 
The 15 most influential learning theories in education (a complete summary)
The 15 most influential learning theories in education (a complete summary)The 15 most influential learning theories in education (a complete summary)
The 15 most influential learning theories in education (a complete summary)
 
Theories of Child Development session 2.pptx
Theories of Child Development session 2.pptxTheories of Child Development session 2.pptx
Theories of Child Development session 2.pptx
 
(2012) Pedagogy as an undisputed social good?
(2012) Pedagogy as an undisputed social good? (2012) Pedagogy as an undisputed social good?
(2012) Pedagogy as an undisputed social good?
 
Guided Response Criteria (respond to a minimum of 2 classmates)1..docx
Guided Response Criteria (respond to a minimum of 2 classmates)1..docxGuided Response Criteria (respond to a minimum of 2 classmates)1..docx
Guided Response Criteria (respond to a minimum of 2 classmates)1..docx
 
5. constructivist learning theories 6. constructivist teaching principles m...
5. constructivist learning theories 6. constructivist teaching principles   m...5. constructivist learning theories 6. constructivist teaching principles   m...
5. constructivist learning theories 6. constructivist teaching principles m...
 
Basic Assumptions and Theoretical Framework of Inquiry as a Teaching strategy...
Basic Assumptions and Theoretical Framework of Inquiry as a Teaching strategy...Basic Assumptions and Theoretical Framework of Inquiry as a Teaching strategy...
Basic Assumptions and Theoretical Framework of Inquiry as a Teaching strategy...
 
Ea.piaget papert
Ea.piaget   papertEa.piaget   papert
Ea.piaget papert
 
Comm1609WebSupplement_TB.PDF
Comm1609WebSupplement_TB.PDFComm1609WebSupplement_TB.PDF
Comm1609WebSupplement_TB.PDF
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
negromaestrong
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
QucHHunhnh
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
heathfieldcps1
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
PECB
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptxSeal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
Seal of Good Local Governance (SGLG) 2024Final.pptx
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptxUnit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
Unit-V; Pricing (Pharma Marketing Management).pptx
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
 
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
 
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptxAsian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural ResourcesEnergy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
 
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
 
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docxPython Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
Python Notes for mca i year students osmania university.docx
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
 

Start with what the student knows or with what the student can imagine?

  • 1. Start with What theStudent Knows or withWhat the Student CanImagine? It is almost universally accepted among educators that the best way to present new material to students is to connect it to what they already know. Mr. Egan questions this principle and suggests that students' imaginations might offer a much more fertile starting point for learning. By Kieran Egan IT IS COMMONLY argued that one of the securest findings of educational research is that new information, to be best understood, must be attached to knowledge the student already has. Formulations of this finding have been various, but it has been a staple of educational thinking from the time of John Dewey -- and before him of Herbert Spencer -- to the recently published National Research Council monograph How People Learn: Bridging Research and Practice.1 I wish to suggest that the common principle of "starting where the student is" may be both inadequate and restrictive in ways not often discussed. In its place, I suggest we might sensibly adopt a second principle of "asking what the student can
  • 2. imagine." We can pose this question at any point in the learning process as a starting point for further inquiry. To show the long influence of the first principle, consider its early articulation in the work of Herbert Spencer and its use by John Dewey and others in shaping the curriculum -- in particular in giving a foundation for the social studies curriculum. Spencer argued that children's early and simple experience had to form the basis for all future learning and that there must be a regular and orderly progression from what is already familiar to what is slightly less familiar -- an expansion "by slow degrees to impressions most nearly allied."2 This principle is believed by nearly every teacher and professor of education I have encountered. Most people assume that it is so obviously true that even to question it suggests a degree of nuttiness. The education reformers of the late 19th and early 20th centuries encouraged teachers to rethink their ways of presenting new material to students in their particular disciplines in accordance with this principle. Thus elementary mathematics would begin from the experience that children had with fruit or with games such as marbles, and language studies would begin with the forms of expression with which children
  • 3. would already be familiar, rather than with a topic like abstract grammar. But subjects such as mathematics and language were still not fully articulated with the meaningful daily interactions of children with their local environments -- "what the student knows." What was further proposed was a new, central curriculum area -- the social studies. This was to be a subject that would begin with the material of children's everyday experience -- with themselves and their families and with their neighborhoods and communities. Gradually, student learning would expand from this meaningful core of personal experience to less familiar knowledge, until, in the end, the whole universe of knowledge could be understood as an expansion from what was most vivid and meaningful to the child. The social studies curriculum was designed to tie all the knowledge being learned in other curriculum areas to the child's experience. So we must start with what is most profoundly known by the student and build new knowledge on that basis. David Ausubel declared, "If I had to reduce all of educational psychology to just one principle, I would say this: The most important single factor influencing learning is what the learner already knows. Ascertain this and teach him accordingly."3 Apart from this
  • 4. slightly odd way of putting Spencer's principle, I think there are four reasons why we might be wary of accepting it. • First, if this is a fundamental principle of human learning, there is no way the process can begin. • Second, if novelty is a problem for human learners, reducing the amount of the novelty doesn't solve the problem. And if we can manage some novelty, why can't we manage more? Perhaps Spencer's "most nearly allied" is far too conservative. • The third objection is less directed at the principle than at the way it has been invariably interpreted in education and particularly in the construction of the elementary social studies curriculum. It is assumed that what children know first and best will be the details of their everyday social lives. That is, it is assumed that children's thinking is simple, concrete, and engaged with their local experience. But, as I will elaborate below, children also have imaginations and emotions, and these, too, connect with the world. If children's minds are supposed to be restricted to the everyday details of their social lives, why are they full of monsters, talking middle-class rabbits, and titanic emotions? We cannot sensibly explain Peter Rabbit's appeal in terms of its "familiar family setting"4 when it
  • 5. involves a safe forest, a dangerous cultivated garden, death so close, and so on. • Fourth, and this appeals to old-fashioned intuition, a few moments of reflection should make clear that no one's understanding of the world expands according to this principle of gradual content association. Now, given the almost universal acceptance of this principle and the fact that these four reasons will have little impact on most of those who believe it, I should add what seems to me the reason such ideas survive so tenaciously. The principle survives because, like many such knowledge claims in education, it is a mixture of analytic truth and empirical generalization. That is, at some level, the principle is true simply because people define its terms to mean something that can't be other than true. It is understood to mean something like "You don't know whatever you don't know, and if you learn something new, it has to fit in with what you can comprehend." If you don't speak Chinese and are told the solution to the three pagodas puzzle in Chinese, you will be unable to understand it. If you do speak Chinese and know what pagodas are and understand the puzzle and fulfill other prerequisites, you will be able to understand the solution. At this level, the principle is
  • 6. certainly true, but it isn't very helpful. What would make it interesting are reliable empirical generalizations. That is, there should be research showing conditions that constrain learning that are something other than logical truths, and these are not thick on the ground. Well, that's a bit clotted, trying to say too much too briefly. But it suggests why one might come to question the common interpretation of starting where the student is. If we mean it as simply an analytic truth, it is of no particular interest to educators. If we mean it as an empirical claim, as it has been taken at least since Spencer's time, then it is inadequate for the reasons given above. Now let us consider what might be implied by taking seriously the principle of what the student can imagine. For obvious reasons, there has not been much research on students' imaginations, and yet they are clearly central to students' learning. But ignoring the imagination because our research methods have difficulty coming to grips with it is somewhat self- defeating. First, I want to argue that the imagination is the ability to think of things as possibly being so.5 This odd ability, which seems to be most energetically active in our early years, is clearly not
  • 7. some casual, frothy part of our mind's functioning, to be blown away with the growth of rationality. It is a hard-working core of children's thinking.6 Second, I want to look in some detail at just three features of children's imaginative engagement with the world and show how they challenge the principle of "starting with what the student knows," which has had so significant an impact on teaching and curriculum thinking. The first feature concerns the story form, which children engage so enthusiastically very early in life and in all cultures and in all times that we know about. The second concerns the kinds of abstract and affective concepts that give structure to the fantasy stories common across the world -- such concepts as good/bad, brave/cowardly, rich/poor, anxiety/security, and so on, with all the freight they carry for human cultures. The third follows from the content of children's fantasy stories and why they are so full of creatures like Peter Rabbit, a talking, middle-class animal that cannot exist. Clearly, storytellers don't pay much attention to the principle of starting with what children know, in the way that the designers and defenders of the elementary social studies curriculum do. Storytellers might begin boldly with galaxies far, far away and
  • 8. long, long ago, if it suits them, confident that their audience will make sense of the content. They can introduce strange characters and weird situations, like Harry Potter's or Frodo's, as long as they build their narratives on the kind of abstract concepts students are familiar with: good/bad, brave/cowardly, anxiety/security, and so on. Consider how children learn about the temperature continuum conceptually. They begin with the concepts "hot" and "cold," as these are both logically and empirically the first concepts that can be meaningful -- "hot" being hotter than the child's body and "cold" being colder. Then they can elaborate or mediate these concepts by learning the concept "warm," for example. Then they can learn an array of other temperature terms in the context created by the first terms that form the ends of the continuum. Later, they will learn to use abstract terms, like thermometer numbers, to refer to temperature. This procedure, which is very effective in dealing with the material world, creates problems when applied to concepts that have no mediating categories, such as life and death or nature and culture or human and animal. Nevertheless, the world of children's imaginative lives is filled with generated mediations between these logically discrete concepts. If there is nothing
  • 9. between life and death, we invent ghosts that are to "life" and "death" as "warm" is to "hot" and "cold." Between nature and culture, we invent that menagerie of talking middle-class animals that fill the fantasy stories of children and the mythic stories of the world. Something serious is going on here that has little to do with starting where the student is and gradually building on the knowledge the student has -- except if we define these conditions as analytic truths. The connections are made by metaphorical leaps, not by logical connections. This article is a meditation on what aspects of our assumptions about learning and curriculum we might need to give up if we acknowledge the importance of students' imaginations. For teaching, we need not be constrained by trying to make content associations with knowledge students already have, for there are other ways of expanding knowledge. For the curriculum, we need no longer be constrained to tie knowledge to the everyday experience of students, which can be very dreary for them, but can recognize that their imaginations allow much freedom in how they can go about grasping the universe of knowledge. Let me conclude by pointing out the obvious -- that I am not arguing for ignoring students' prior knowledge and everyday
  • 10. experience. Rather, I am arguing that these have been taken as implying greater restrictions on children's learning and curriculum possibilities than is warranted when we consider their imaginative lives. We can start with what they can imagine. ________________________________________ 1. Suzanne M. Donovan, John D. Bransford, and James W. Pellegrino, eds., How People Learn: Bridging Research and Practice (Washington, D.C.: National Academy Press, 1999). 2. Herbert Spencer, Education: Intellectual, Moral, and Physical (London: G. Manwaring, 1861), p. 82. 3. David P. Ausubel, Educational Psychology: A Cognitive View (London: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1968), p. 18. 4. Arthur N. Applebee, The Child's Concept of Story (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1978), p. 75. 5. Alan R. White, The Language of Imagination (Oxford: Blackwell, 1990). 6. Mary Warnock, Imagination (London: Faber, 1976).