SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 19
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology
                 http://jcc.sagepub.com/




Local Culture's Responses to Globalization : Exemplary Persons and Their Attendant
                                      Values
                               Jeanne Ho-Ying Fu and Chi-Yue Chiu
                         Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 2007 38: 636
                                 DOI: 10.1177/0022022107305244

                           The online version of this article can be found at:
                               http://jcc.sagepub.com/content/38/5/636


                                                                 Published by:

                                              http://www.sagepublications.com

                                                                 On behalf of:



                         International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology




      Additional services and information for Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology can be found at:

                                      Email Alerts: http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

                                 Subscriptions: http://jcc.sagepub.com/subscriptions

                             Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

                       Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

                          Citations: http://jcc.sagepub.com/content/38/5/636.refs.html




                     Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
LOCAL CULTURE’S RESPONSES TO GLOBALIZATION
                Exemplary Persons and Their Attendant Values

                                                 JEANNE HO-YING FU
                                        Nanyang Technological University, Singapore
                                                       CHI-YUE CHIU
                                         University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign



    Taking a social identity perspective, the authors predict that when responding to the dominating influence of
    the global culture brought in by the Western economic powers, Hong Kong Chinese will recognize the global
    culture’s superiority in status attributes (e.g., competence, achievement), while at the same time maintaining
    positive evaluations of Chinese culture on solidarity attributes (traditional moral values). The authors test this
    hypothesis by examining the Chinese and Western exemplary persons listed spontaneously by Hong Kong
    Chinese undergraduates and the kind of values carried by these exemplary persons. In three studies, partici-
    pants associate traditional Chinese exemplary persons with both solidarity and status values and traditional
    Western exemplary persons with status values only. Additionally, participants also associate contemporary
    Western exemplary persons with Western rights-based moral values, suggesting that contemporary Western
    exemplary persons could be important drivers of changes in the moral values in Hong Kong.


    Keywords:      culture; exemplary persons; values; globalization



The possible effects of globalization on local cultures have generated heated debates in the
social sciences. One critical issue in the debate is whether globalization ultimately leads to
homogenization of cultures via global hegemony. Researchers from many social science disci-
plines have approached this question from their disciplinary perspective. However, despite
some recent calls for psychological analyses of globalization (Arnett, 2002), psychologists have
not been enthusiastic participants in this dialogue. Yet any answer to the prospect of cultural
homogenization will be incomplete without systematic analyses of the psychological responses
to globalization. In this article, we examine how Hong Kong Chinese respond to Western and
global cultures by examining the cultural values carried by exemplary persons in Hong Kong.
In the following section, we review different views of local culture’s responses to globalization
in the social science literature and discuss the psychological responses of Hong Kong Chinese
to globalization from the perspective of the social identity theory. Next, we introduce a method
we developed to examine psychological responses to globalization and present the results of
three studies that looked at how Hong Kong Chinese maintain their Chinese cultural heritage
in response to globalization.

             GLOBALIZATION AND HOMOGENIZATION OF CULTURES

   Globalization involves the spread of the global economy to regional economies around
the world. It requires integration of local economies into the global market. Culturally, the

AUTHORS’ NOTE: We would like to thank Harry Triandis, Michael Morris, Yoshi Kashima, and Chung-Fang Yang for their
insightful comments on the earlier drafts of this article. Correspondences concerning this article should be sent to Jeanne
Ho-Ying Fu, Nanyang Business School, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 639798; e-mail: hyfu@pmail.ntu.edu.sg.

JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY, Vol. 38 No. 5, September 2007 636-653
DOI: 10.1177/0022022107305244
© 2007 Sage Publications
636



               Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES                       637


rapid growth of global linkages and global consciousness has led to reorganization of social
life on a global scale. As Robertson (1992) noted, globalization involves “the compression of
the world and the intensification of consciousness of the world as a whole” (p. 8). Some con-
straints that once bound different knowledge traditions have disintegrated. For example, in
China, it is common to find commercial messages promoting individualism and modern life
styles in popular magazines targeting the younger generation (Zhang & Shavitt, 2003).
    There are two different views on how local culture responds to globalization. The first
view predicts that globalization would inevitably lead to the demise of local cultures and
homogenization of world cultures. According to this view, Western nations, representatives
of the global culture, are not only perceived to be more economically advanced than non-
Western nations, but they are often seen as reference nations in the realm of cultural restruc-
turing. The global culture, which privileges consumerism, individualism, competition, and
efficiency, has been characterized in some non-Western countries as new, modern, scientific,
and results oriented (Lam, Lau, Chiu, Hong, & Peng, 1999; Pilkington & Johnson, 2003).
    In addition, the global market emphasizes the use of scientific knowledge and technol-
ogy to achieve controllable and predictable results. To compete in the global market,
instead of relying on traditional knowledge and experiences, local economies need scien-
tific knowledge and professional training in business administration. It is not surprising
that globalization in many developing countries is often accompanied by a vast demand for
science education and MBA programs (Chiu & Hong, 2006; Fischer, 1999; Tzeng &
Henderson, 1999).
    Globalization has also transformed the consumption patterns in local economies, as evi-
dent in the spread of global “brand-name” goods and restaurant chains (Daniels, 2003) and
the proliferation of global advertising that exploits similar basic material desires and cre-
ates similar lifestyles (Parameswaran, 2002). Indeed, the global success of global brands
such as McDonald’s and Starbucks has led some writers to predict an inevitable coloniza-
tion of world cultures by international corporate brands (Falk, 1999).
    However, other writers hold a different view, believing that some aspects of the local
culture would survive the erosive effects of globalization. For example, Lal (2000) believes
that although the material beliefs (beliefs pertinent to making a living) in the global cul-
ture will gain popularity as a country is integrated into the global market, beliefs pertinent
to social relationship and morality in local cultures are relatively resistant to the influence
of globalization. Additionally, multinational survey data revealed that although economic
development is accompanied by increased adherence to values that emphasize secular-
ism, scientific rationality, and individualism, the broad knowledge tradition that defines
personhood and sociality in a society (e.g., Protestantism, Confucianism) is relatively
resistant to the erosive effects of globalization (Inglehart & Baker, 2000). Likewise, ethno-
graphic studies of mobile phone use in South Korea (Yoon, 2003) and MTV in East Asia
(Santana, 2003) found that proliferation of global products and services in East Asia,
instead of destroying local cultures, has played a crucial role in reinforcing and reinvent-
ing traditional moral values in local communities.


                             THE SOCIAL IDENTITY PERSPECTIVE

   The social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) offers a theoretical perspective to
understand how a local culture may accept the values embedded in the global culture with-
out giving up the core values in the local culture. In the face of globalization, a central



            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
638   JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY


issue confronting the local culture is how it is possible to acknowledge the competitive
advantages of the capitalist logic and its attendant values in the global market and at the
same time affirm the positive distinctiveness of the heritage culture. Based on the assump-
tion that group memberships constitute an integral part of an individual’s self-definition,
social identity theorists have expounded on the different cognitive and behavioral strate-
gies a low-status group would adopt to protect its collective self-esteem when confronting
the dominating influence of another group (Hogg & Abrams, 1988). One such strategy
involves differential evaluations of the in-group and the dominating group on different
dimensions.
   Early empirical demonstrations of this strategy made use of the fact that language vari-
ations within and between speech communities are important markers of the language
user’s social group memberships. As such, perceivers can infer a speaker’s group mem-
berships from his or her voice, and the characteristics attributed to the speaker may reflect
the perceivers’ attitudes toward the speaker’s inferred membership. In these studies, the
participants listened to recordings of bilingual speakers reading a passage in one of the tar-
get languages and to a translation of the same passage in the second target language.
Unaware that they were listening to two readings of each of several bilinguals, the partic-
ipants rated the speakers’ personalities (see Krauss & Chiu, 1998; Ryan & Giles, 1982, for
reviews). Typically, ratings of speakers’ personalities can be organized into two evaluative
dimensions: status and solidarity (Ryan, Giles, & Sebastian, 1982). The status dimension,
including such competence-related attributes as successful, competent, and intelligent, is
positively correlated with sociopolitical power. Subordinate linguistic groups usually give
more favorable ratings to the dominant linguistic groups than to their own group on this
dimension (e.g., Callan, Gallois, & Forbes, 1983; Giles, Henwood, Coupland, Harriman,
& Coupland, 1992; Lyczak, Fu, & Ho, 1976; Sebastian & Ryan, 1985).
   The solidarity dimension includes social, moral attributes such as trustworthy, friendly,
kind, and benevolent. Minority group members typically rate speakers of their own lan-
guage more favorably than speakers of the standard dominant group language on the soli-
darity dimension (e.g., Bond, 1985; Giles et al., 1992; Hogg, Joyce, & Abrams, 1984;
Lyczak et al., 1976; Tong, Hong, Lee, & Chiu, 1999). This pattern of evaluation may
reflect an attempt by the less powerful group to maintain positive evaluation of the group
while acknowledging the power difference between its group and the dominant group.
   Extending this finding to local cultures’ responses to the global culture, we may
expect individuals in a local culture to acknowledge the dominating global culture’s
superiority on the status dimension, while maintaining a positive perception of the local
culture on the solidarity dimension. Individuals adopting this strategy would distinguish
between modernization and Westernization, with modernization involving acquisition of
specific skills and competencies that have fueled the economic development in the West
and Westernization involving adoption of the Western social–moral values. With this strat-
egy, people in the local community can accept modernization and resist Westernization at
the same time. As a result, they may not see modernization as a threat to the fundamental
social and moral values in the local culture.
   Bond and King (1985) have applied the social identity perspective to understand Hong
Kong Chinese’s responses to the global culture, which was brought in through coloniza-
tion and then institutionalized through rapid industrialization. In a survey, Bond and King
found that most Hong Kong Chinese respondents (about 70%) believed that modernization
involved technology, behavior, or material progress, whereas Westernization involved val-
ues, thinking, or Western cultural traditions. Almost two thirds of respondents (64%) also



            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES                       639


believed that modernization could proceed in Hong Kong without much cultural implica-
tions. About half of them claimed that they managed to preserve their Chineseness by
holding onto basic Chinese moral values, such as filial piety and respect for teachers.


      METHODOLOGICAL APPROACH USING EXEMPLARY PERSONS

   As the preliminary findings from Bond and King (1985) indicated, in Hong Kong, the
global culture is generally perceived to have originated in the West. Accordingly, in our
study of responses to the global culture in Hong Kong, we focus on the extent to which
status and solidarity values are exemplified by the individuals whom Hong Kong Chinese
consider to be exemplary persons in China and the West.
   We chose this analytic strategy for two reasons. First, exemplary persons are important
carriers of culture. Exemplary persons are real or fictional figures who possess qualities
that are so positively evaluated in the culture that others would do well to copy.1 Exemplary
persons may be persons with outstanding achievements or persons who have endured mis-
fortunes or met with failures in the pursuit of socially desirable goals. Both successful and
unsuccessful exemplary persons can incite imitation among individuals in the culture
(Lockwood, Marshall, & Sadler, 2005). Although successful exemplary persons inspire
individuals to pursue similar excellence, unsuccessful exemplary persons encourage indi-
viduals to learn from the exemplary persons’ mistakes and from their persistence in pur-
suing socially desirable goals.
   Widely recognized exemplary persons are usually idealized figures that embody certain
highly valued virtues in the culture (Chiu & Hong, 2006). As such, they are public repre-
sentations of cultural ideals and important vehicles for transmitting cultural values. In
Chinese societies, young children often learn cultural values by reading or listening to sto-
ries about exemplary persons whose behaviors exemplify these values. For example, the
value of “filial piety” is introduced to children through the well-known Twenty-Four
Parables of Filial Piety, which describes and glorifies various filial behaviors displayed by
24 famous sons or daughters in Chinese history. Similarly, in Hong Kong, young children
often learn Western values through stories of exemplary persons in Western cultures. For
example, they learn the value of honesty through the story of the U.S. President George
Washington and the value of creativity through the stories of Marie Curie, James Watt,
Thomas Edison, and the Wright brothers. Western moral values such as equality, human
rights, and freedom are learned through the stories of Abraham Lincoln.
   Second, according to the social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979), group members
collectively construct prototypes of the group to represent the group’s defining, positively
distinctive attributes. A prototype is often an exemplary person in the group. According to
Hogg (2004), “prototypes rarely describe average or typical ingroup members—rather
they are polarized away from outgroup features and describe ideal, often hypothetical,
ingroup members” (p. 229). Thus, according to the social identity theory, exemplary per-
sons in a local culture and their attendant values represent the culture’s defining qualities
from which its members derive a sense of positive distinctiveness and collective esteem
vis-à-vis comparison with the global culture. As such, exemplary persons and their atten-
dant values provide rich materials for understanding globalization and value negotiation.
   In this article, we reported three studies conducted in Hong Kong that used the method
described earlier to examine the exemplification of status and solidarity values by individ-
uals who are considered to be exemplary persons from local (Chinese) culture and global



            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
640   JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY


(Western) culture. In each study, a sample of Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates listed the
exemplary persons who came to their mind readily and rated the extent to which these
exemplary persons represented competence values and social, moral values in Chinese and
Western cultures. We predicted that the spontaneously listed Chinese exemplary persons
are likely to embody Chinese social, moral values or values that serve solidarity functions.
Contrarily, spontaneously listed Western exemplary persons are likely to embody compe-
tence values (versus Western moral values) or values that reflect socioeconomic status.


                                                            STUDY 1

METHOD

    Participants. Twenty Hong Kong undergraduate students (4 men and 16 women) from
an introductory psychology class participated in this study in return for course requirement
credit. Their mean age was 19.40 (SD = 0.59).
    Measures and procedures. On arrival and signing the consent form, the participants
completed a questionnaire that was written in Chinese. They listed six exemplary persons
in Chinese culture and six exemplary persons in Western cultures. The order of listing
Chinese and Western exemplary persons was counterbalanced. Specifically, they were
instructed to “list six persons in Chinese (Western) culture who deserve much respect and
are worthy of imitating.” Next, the participants indicated on a 6-point Likert-type scale
(from 1 = not at all to 6 = very much) the extent to which each of these exemplary persons
was an embodiment of six solidarity values from Chinese culture (filial piety, patriotism,
loyalty, righteousness, sacrifice, and bravery) and six status values (intelligence, persis-
tence, wisdom, power, artistic talent, and creativity). The 12 values were presented to the
participants in a random order, with the same order for all participants.
    These 12 values were selected based on the results of a pilot study. In the pilot study,
we first identified 32 (23 men, 9 women) exemplary persons from Chinese culture and 22
(18 male, 4 female) from Western cultures in Hong Kong’s grade school curriculum. Next,
we had a separate sample of 121 Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates (22% male; mean
age = 19.26, SD = 1.38) listed up to five values associated with these models. Half of the
participants in the pilot study listed values associated with the Chinese exemplary persons,
and the remaining half listed values associated with the Western ones. The six most fre-
quently mentioned values associated with the Chinese exemplary persons were patriotism,
bravery, righteousness, loyalty, self-sacrifice, and filial piety. These are all core Confucian
moral values. We used these six values to represent solidarity values in the main study.
    The six most frequently mentioned values associated with the Western exemplary per-
sons were creativity, power, artistic talent, intelligence, persistence, and wisdom. These
values are related to human competencies and are typical status values. Moral values in the
Western cultures (freedom, human rights) were seldom mentioned. These results are con-
sistent with our hypothesis: In Hong Kong, Chinese exemplary persons tend to be associ-
ated with solidarity values and Western exemplary persons with status values.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

  We compiled a list of the exemplary persons listed by the participants in the main study.
Of the 115 Chinese exemplary persons listed, 98.2% were men.2 The six most frequently




            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES                       641


mentioned exemplary persons in Chinese culture were Confucius (551 BC to 479 BC,
philosopher; the percentage of participants who listed this exemplary person was 80.0%),
Sun Yat-Sen (1866 to 1925, founder of the Republic of China; listed by 55.0%), Mencius
(370 BC to 286 BC, philosopher; 55.0%), Li Bai (701 to 762, poet; 45.0%), Zhuangzi (369
BC to 286 BC, philosopher; 40.0%), and Du Fu (712 to 770, poet; 40.0%).
    Of the 105 Western exemplary persons listed, 85.7% were men. The six most frequently
mentioned exemplary persons in Western cultures were James Watt (75.0%), Mother
Teresa (30.0%), Sir Isaac Newton (25.0%), Napoleon Bonaparte (25.0%), Jesus Christ
(25.0%), and Ludwig van Beethoven (20.0%).
    For each participant and for each value, we took the mean of the ratings assigned to the
listed Chinese exemplary persons and the mean of the ratings assigned to the listed Western
exemplary persons. Next, we took the mean of the six values in each of the two value cate-
gories (Chinese solidarity values or status values). A 2 (Culture of Exemplary Person) X 2
(Value Category) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a significant
main effect of value category, F(1, 18) = 9.55, p < .01, η2p = .35, and a significant main effect
of culture, F(1, 18) = 23.21, p < .001, η2p = .56. We interpreted these main effects in the con-
text of the significant Culture X Value Category interaction, F(1, 18) = 8.97, p < .01, η2p =
.33.3 As shown in Figure 1, Chinese exemplary persons were perceived to exemplify Chinese
solidarity values (M = 4.47, SD = 0.79) and status values (M = 4.77, SD = 0.41) to a similar
extent, F(1, 19) = 3.49, ns. Western exemplary persons exemplified status values (M = 4.77,
SD = 0.42) more than they did Chinese solidarity values (M = 3.96, SD = 0.60), F(1, 18) =
49.26, p < .001, η2p = .73. In addition, Chinese and Western exemplary persons did not differ
in how much they exemplified status values, t(19) = 0.00, ns. However, compared to Western
exemplary persons, Chinese exemplary persons were perceived to exemplify solidarity val-
ues more, t(19) = 2.55, p < .05, d = .73.
    We also used exemplary persons as our unit of analysis, treating each exemplary person
listed by the participant as a case in the analysis. First, we took the mean ratings of the six val-
ues in each value category for each exemplary person. Next, we performed a 2 (Culture of
Exemplary Person) X 2 (Value Category) ANOVA on the mean ratings. Gender of the exem-
plary persons was not included in the analysis because the small number of female exemplary
persons (7.66%) yielded highly unstable estimates of the dependent measures for female
exemplary persons. As in the analysis that used participants as the unit of analysis, there was
a significant main effect of value category, F(1, 220) = 67.59, p < .001, η2p = .24, a significant
main effect of culture, F(1, 220) = 8.57, p < .005, η2p = .04, and a significant Culture X Value
Category interaction, F(1, 220) = 14.61, p < .001, η2p = .06. Chinese and Western exemplary
persons did not differ in how much they exemplify status values, t(221) = –0.07, ns. However,
compared to Western exemplary persons, Chinese exemplary persons were perceived to exem-
plify Chinese solidarity values more, t(221) = 2.11, p < .05, d = 0.50.
    In short, the pilot study and the main study yielded consistent results. In the pilot study,
when presented with exemplary persons in Hong Kong’s grade school curriculum, Hong
Kong Chinese college students spontaneously listed solidarity values when the exemplary
persons were from Chinese culture and status values when the exemplary persons were
from Western cultures. In the main study, compared to Western exemplary persons,
Chinese exemplary persons exemplified Chinese moral (solidarity) values more. These
results are consistent with the social identity theory prediction that Hong Kong Chinese
continue to value Chinese moral values; the exemplary persons from Chinese culture that
the participants aspired to imitate were perceived to exemplify these values more than
those from Western culture.



            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
642   JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY




         5




       4.5




         4




       3.5




         3
                     Chinese Exemplary Persons                                   Western Exemplary Persons


                                                Solidarity Values                   Status Values


Figure 1. Mean Ratings of How Much Chinese and Western Exemplary Persons Exemplified Different
          Categories of Values in Study 1
NOTE: Error bars denote standard errors.


   It is interesting that in the main study, Chinese and Western exemplary persons exem-
plified status values to a similar extent. We speculate that because of Hong Kong’s success
in the global economy, many Hong Kong Chinese no longer perceive Western culture as
having superior status than Chinese culture in terms of economic prosperity. Furthermore,
they have started to use Chinese exemplary persons who embody both the perceived moral
superiority of Chinese culture and the competence traits required for success in the global
economy as materials for constructing a positively distinctive identity.


                                                             STUDY 2

    Although Study 1 provided some initial support for our hypothesis, it could be argued
that the findings are due to a priming effect—simply asking participants to think of any
Chinese or Western person might be able to produce the pattern of results obtained in
Study 1. In other words, the effects may have nothing to do with the stimulus person being
an exemplary person.
    In the current study, to address this interpretive ambiguity, we repeated Study 1 with a
slightly different procedure. In Study 1, the participants listed exemplary persons. In the
present study, the participants listed historical figures from Chinese and Western cultures
and rated these figures in terms of how much they exemplified Chinese moral (solidarity)
values and competence (status) values. Next, we had a group of independent judges rate
how positive or negative each historical figure was. According to the social identity theory,
prototypes of a group are idealized and positively evaluated in-group members (Hogg,




             Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES                       643


2004). Thus, the pattern of results in Study 1 should emerge only when the historical
figures are positive figures (when they are like exemplary persons). However, according to
the priming account, the pattern of results in Study 1 would emerge regardless of whether
the historical figures are positive or negative figures. For instance, as long as the historical
figures are Chinese persons, they should exemplify Chinese traditional moral values.

METHOD

   Participants. Fifty Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates (15 men, 34 women, and 1 did
not report gender), with a mean age of 20.25 (SD = 2.26), participated in the current study
in return for course requirement credit.

    Procedures. The procedures were the same as those in Study 1, with the exception that
the participants were instructed to list up to six historical figures in Chinese history and six
historical figures in Western history. Four Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates (2 men, 2
women; mean age = 19.50, SD = 0.58) assigned a valence score to each historical figure
according to the following scheme: 1 = negative figures, 2 = neutral figures, and 3 = pos-
itive figures (a few figures were perceived as ambivalent or vague, and they were coded as
missing data). The correlations of the valence ratings between the four coders ranged from
.57 to .88 (median = .81). An average valence score was obtained for each historical figure
by taking the mean of the four coders’ ratings. The mean of the valence scores was 2.48
(SD = 0.59).

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

    A list of historical figures generated by the participants was compiled. Of the 296
Chinese historical figures generated, 91.9% were men. The six most frequently mentioned
figures in Chinese history were Qin Shi Huang (259 BC to 210 BC, Emperor of the Qin
Dynasty; percentage of participants who mentioned this figure was 62.0%), Yue Fei (1103
to 1141, a general in the Sung Dynasty; 48.0%), Sun Yat-Sen (44.0%), Li Bai (32.0%), Liu
Bei (161 to 223, ruler of the State of Shu; 30.0%), and Zhuge Liang (181-234, a chief mil-
itary advisor; 26.0%).
    Of the 282 Western historical figures listed, 96.1% were men. The six most frequently
mentioned figures in Western history were Adolf Hitler (56.0%), Napoleon Bonaparte
(48.0%), George Washington (32.0%), Alexander the Great (30.0%), Abraham Lincoln
(28.0%), and Marco Polo (28.0%).
    We treated each historical figure as a case in our analysis. For each historical figure, we
computed the mean exemplification rating on each of the two value categories. We mean
centered the valence score to avoid potential problems caused by multicollinearity. Next,
we fitted a 2 (Culture of Historical Figure) X 2 (Value Category) X Valence generalized
linear model to the data, with valence as a mean-centered continuous variable. Again,
gender of the historical figures was not included in the analysis because very few female
historical figures were listed (6.03%). The following effects were significant: the main
effects of culture, F(1, 558) = 9.84, p < .001, η2p = .02, valence, F(1, 558) = 78.74, p <
.001, η2p = .12, value category, F(1, 558) = 91.42, p < .001, η2p = .14, the Culture X Valence
interaction, F(1, 558) = 13.55, p < .001, η2p = .02, the Culture X Value Category interac-
tion, F(1, 558) = 41.61, p < .001, η2p = .07, the Value Category X Valence interaction,




            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
644   JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY


F(1, 558) = 21.99, p < .001, η2p = .04, and Culture X Value Category X Valence interaction,
F(1, 558) = 42.73, p < .001, η2p = .07.
    Follow-up simple slope analyses showed that for positive historical figures (valence cen-
tered at 1 SD above the mean), there was a significant main effects of culture, F(1, 558) =
23.42, p < .001, η2p = .04, value category, F(1, 558) = 12.09, p < .001, η2p = .02, and Culture
X Value Category interaction, F(1, 558) = 84.98, p < .001, η2p = .13. As shown in Figure 2,
positive Chinese historical figures (valence > mean) exemplified Chinese solidarity values
(M = 4.56, SD = 1.11) slightly more than status values (M = 4.37, SD = 0.82), F(1, 196) =
5.91, p < .05, η2p = .03, whereas positive Western historical figures (valence > mean) exem-
plified status values (M = 4.46, SD = 0.75) more than Chinese solidarity values (M = 3.71,
SD = 1.10), F(1, 198) = 83.33, p < .001, η2p = .30. Furthermore, compared to positive Western
historical figures, positive Chinese historical figures exemplified Chinese solidarity values
more, t(396) = 3.29, p < .01, d = .77. As in Study 1, Chinese and Western historical figures
did not differ in how much they exemplified status values, t(396) = –0.51, ns.
    For negative historical figures (valence centered at 1 SD below the mean), the only signif-
icant effect was the main effect of value category, F(1, 558) = 100.89, p < .001, η2p = .15.
Status values (M = 4.11, SD = 0.88) were exemplified more than solidarity values (M = 3.36,
SD = 1.16), t(182) = 6.95, p < .01, d = .73, by these negative historical figures. The Culture X
Value Category interaction was not significant, F(1, 558) = 0.00, ns.
    In short, the results from Study 1 were replicated when the valence of the historical
figures was positive. However, when the valence of the historical figures was negative,
Chinese and Western historical figures did not differ in the types of values they exempli-
fied. These findings eliminated the alternative hypothesis that any Chinese or Western
stimulus person would produce the value association pattern in Study 1.


                                                            STUDY 3

   Results from Studies 1 and 2 showed that Chinese exemplary persons or positively eval-
uated Chinese historical figures are associated with both solidarity (Chinese moral) and
status values and Western exemplary persons or positively evaluated historical figures are
associated with status values only. The current study was designed to replicate these find-
ings and address several concerns. First, it may be argued that the participants recalled the
widely recognized Chinese and Western exemplary persons in Hong Kong but did not
admire or aspire personally to possess the values these persons exemplify. To address this
issue, in Study 3, participants were asked to list the exemplary persons they themselves
admired and aspired to become. In addition, they were not constrained to list exemplary
persons from any particular culture. Instead, we sorted the exemplary persons the partici-
pants listed according to the culture to which the exemplary persons belonged.
   Second, although Hong Kong undergraduates do not associate Western exemplary per-
sons with Chinese moral values, they may associate them with Western moral values. It is
possible that through their exposure to Western cultures, Hong Kong Chinese also aspire
to become some Western exemplary persons who embody Western moral values. If that is
the case, Western moral values may find a way to enter the value system in Hong Kong.
To explore this possibility, in the current study, we included moral values from Western
culture, such as human rights and individuality, and asked the participants to rate the
exemplary persons on these values.




            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES                       645



            5




          4.5




            4




          3.5




            3
                        Positive                  Positive                      Negative                   Negative
                        Chinese                   Western                       Chinese                    Western

                                                           Historical Figures


                                               Solidarity Values                    Status Values


Figure 2. Mean Ratings of How Much Positive and Negative Chinese and Western Historical Figures
          Exemplified Different Categories of Values in Study 2
NOTE: Error bars denote standard errors.


   Finally, according to the social identity theory, a group may construct different proto-
types when the specific out-group that forms the basis of social comparison changes
(Hogg, 2004). Similarly, a new set of exemplary persons carrying different values may
emerge and become widely recognized in a society as the comparison group changes. In
the context of Hong Kong, the return of Hong Kong to China in 1997 has given rise to new
intercultural dynamics: A rapidly growing concern in the city during and immediately after
the political transition was the positive distinctiveness of Hong Kong people vis-à-vis
Chinese mainlanders. There is evidence that during this period, Hong Kong people polar-
ized themselves away from Chinese mainlanders by emphasizing Hong Kong people’s
adherence to Western moral values such as human rights and democracy (Lam et al., 1999;
Tong et al., 1999). Indeed, soon after Hong Kong was returned to China in 1997, the city
was known to the world as the City of Protest. There were large-scale rallies, with tens of
thousands of protesters taking to the streets in Hong Kong to protest for freedom of speech
and other political rights. Thus, it is possible that in response to this shift in the out-group
as the basis for social comparison, Hong Kong Chinese have constructed a new set of
exemplary persons who embody such Western moral values as democracy and human
rights. Aggregating scores across exemplary persons from different epochs in the previous
two studies might have overlooked this subtle development. In the current study, we
explored this issue by examining the values exemplified by historical and contemporary
exemplary persons. We assumed that the values represented by historical exemplary




            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
646   JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY


persons are likely to be longstanding values in the culture and values represented by con-
temporary exemplary persons are likely to be the values that were incorporated into the
culture’s value system relatively recently. Thus, the values historical and contemporary
exemplary persons exemplify may reflect value change in a culture.

METHOD

   Participants. Thirty-one Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates (6 men, 25 women; mean
age = 20, SD = 1.79) received US$7.50 for their participation in the present study.

    Procedures. On arrival and signing the consent form, participants were instructed to
“list 12 exemplary persons you respect and aspire to become” and to rate on a 6-point
Likert-type scale (from not at all to very much) how much each of these exemplary per-
sons exemplified 18 values. Of these values, 12 were those used in Studies 1 and 2. The
remaining 6 were Western rights–based or individualist values: equality, human rights,
democracy, individuality, freedom, and uniqueness. Next, half of the participants rated
how well each exemplary person represented Chinese culture before they rated how well
the exemplary person represented Western culture. The remaining participants performed
the same ratings in the reverse order.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

   Two hundred and sixty-eight exemplary persons (69.3% males) were listed.4 The first
author categorized the exemplary persons into Chinese or Western exemplary persons.
Exemplary persons who did not fall into these categories (4.5%) were excluded from fur-
ther analyses. The exemplary persons were also categorized into historical or contempo-
rary exemplary persons depending on whether they had passed away before the end of
World War II (1945).

   Cultural representativeness of exemplary persons. Treating each exemplary person as a
case, we performed a 2 (Culture: Chinese or Western) X 2 (Epoch: Historical or
Contemporary) ANOVA on how well the exemplary persons represented Chinese culture.
The results indicated that Chinese exemplary persons (M = 4.28, SD = 1.30) were seen as
better representatives of Chinese culture than were Western ones (M = 2.28, SD = 1.04),
F(1, 243) = 222.50, p < .001, η2p = .48, for the main effect of culture. Historical exemplary
persons (M = 3.80, SD = 1.80) were seen as better representatives of Chinese culture than
were contemporary ones (M = 3.33, SD = 1.36), F(1, 243) = 18.43, p < .001, η2p = .07, for
the main effect of epoch. The Culture X Epoch interaction was also significant, F(1, 243) =
18.66, p < .001, η2p = .07. Historical Chinese exemplary persons (M = 5.17, SD = 1.08) were
seen as better representatives of Chinese culture than were contemporary Chinese exemplary
persons (M = 3.87, SD = 1.19), F(1, 149) = 41.18, p < .001, η2p = .22, whereas historical
and contemporary Western exemplary persons did not differ in how well they represented
Chinese culture (M = 2.28, SD = 1.05 versus M = 2.28, SD = 1.04), F(1, 94) < 0.01, ns. As
the means indicated, both historical and contemporary Western exemplary persons were
not seen to be representative of Chinese culture.
   A similar analysis performed on how well the exemplary persons represented Western
culture revealed that Western exemplary persons (M = 4.63, SD = 1.00) were seen as better




            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES                              647


                                                                TABLE 1
             Correlation Between How Well Exemplary Persons Represented
                  Chinese and Western Cultures and How Much They
                  Exemplified Different Categories of Values (Study 3)

                                                                                              Value Category

                                                                   Solidarity                              Status                   Western
                                                             (Chinese Moral) Value                         Value                   Moral Value

Representativeness of Chinese culture                                     .15*                             –.08                      –.08
Representativeness of Western culture                                    –.12*                              .17*                      .23**

*p < .05. **p < .01.


representatives of Western culture than were Chinese exemplary persons (M = 2.88, SD =
1.34), F(1, 242) = 159.65, p < .001, η2p = .40, for the main effect of culture. In addition, con-
temporary exemplary persons (M = 3.71, SD = 1.29) were seen as better representatives of
Western culture than were historical ones (M = 3.31, SD = 1.74; d = .26), F(1, 242) = 12.52,
p < .001, η2p = .05, for the main effect of epoch. The significant Culture of Exemplary Person
X Epoch interaction, F(1, 242) = 14.78, p < .001, η2p = .06, indicated that historical and con-
temporary Western exemplary persons did not differ in how well they represented Western
culture (M = 4.64, SD = 1.02 versus M = 4.60, SD = 0.99), F(1, 94) = 0.05, ns. It is interest-
ing that contemporary Chinese exemplary persons (M = 3.25, SD = 1.18) were viewed as
better representatives of Western culture than were historical Chinese exemplary persons
(M = 2.10, SD = 1.32), F(1, 148) = 28.15, p < .001, η2p = .16.

   Attendant values of the exemplary persons. Table 1 shows the correlation between how
well the exemplary persons represented Chinese and Western cultures and how much they
exemplified the three categories of values. The better the exemplary persons represented
Chinese culture, the more they were perceived to exemplify Chinese moral values (r = .15,
p < .05). The better the exemplary persons represented Western culture, the more they were
perceived to exemplify Western moral values (r = .23, p < .01) and status values (r = .17,
p < .05). Finally, the better the exemplary persons represented Western culture, the less
they were perceived to exemplify Chinese moral values (r = –.12, p < .05).
   To examine how exemplary persons from different cultures and epochs differed in the
values they exemplified, we performed a 2 (Culture of Exemplary Person) X 2 (Epoch of
Exemplary Person) X 3 (Value Category) ANOVA on the exemplification ratings. The fol-
lowing effects were significant: the main effect of value category, F(2, 486) = 26.02, p <
.001, η2p = .10, the Culture X Epoch interaction, F(2, 243) = 5.92, p < .05, η2p = .02, the
Culture X Value Category interaction, F(2, 486) = 9.12, p < .001, η2p = .04, the Epoch X
Value Category interaction, F(2, 486) = 4.20, p <.05, η2p = .02, and the Culture X Epoch
X Value Category interaction, F(2, 486) = 7.74, p < .001, η2p = .03. All the effects were
interpreted in the context of the highest order three-way Culture X Epoch X Value
Category interaction.
   Figure 3 shows that historical Chinese exemplary persons exemplified Chinese moral
values (M = 4.38, SD = 1.14) and status values (M = 4.34, SD = 0.82) more than they did
Western moral values (M = 3.92, SD = 1.11), F(2, 94) = 4.42, p < .05, η2p = .09. In con-
trast, historical Western exemplary persons exemplified status values (M = 4.54, SD =
0.67) more than they did Western moral values (M = 3.86, SD = 1.22) and Chinese moral
values (M = 3.60, SD = 1.35), F(2, 84) = 12.63, p < .001, η2p = .23.


              Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
648   JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY




      4.5




        4




      3.5




        3
                  Historical                     Historical                  Contemporary                   Contemporary
                  Chinese                        Western                       Chinese                        Western

                                                         Exemplary Persons


                         Solidarity Values                   Status Values                    Western Moral Values


Figure 3. Mean Ratings of How Much Historical and Contemporary Exemplary Persons From Chinese
          and Western Cultures Exemplified Different Categories of Values in Study 3
NOTE: Error bars denote standard errors.


   Furthermore, compared to historical Western exemplary persons, historical Chinese
exemplary persons exemplified Chinese moral values more, t(182) = 2.41, p < .05, d = .62.
As in Studies 1 and 2, Chinese and Western exemplary persons did not differ in how much
they exemplified status values, t(182) = 1.17, ns. In short, the findings on Chinese moral
and status values exemplified by historical exemplary persons were similar to those in
Studies 1 and 2. Results also showed that the two groups of exemplary persons did not dif-
fer in how much they exemplified Western moral values, t(182) = 0.2, ns.
   We next examined the values associated with contemporary exemplary persons. The
contemporary Chinese exemplary persons were similar to the historical Western exem-
plary persons in terms of pattern of values they exemplified. Contemporary Chinese
exemplary persons exemplified status values (M = 4.18, SD = 0.85) more than they did
Western moral values (M = 3.65, SD = 1.08) and Chinese moral values (M = 3.58, SD =
0.91), F(2, 204) = 22.81, p < .001, η2p = .18. Finally, contemporary Western exemplary
persons exemplified Western moral values (M = 4.29, SD = 0.86) and status values (M =
4.23; SD = 0.74) more than they did Chinese moral values (M = 3.75, SD = 1.02), F(2, 104) =
9.81, p < .001, η2p = .16.
   Similar to the historical exemplary persons, we explored whether contemporary
Chinese and Western exemplary persons showed differences in exemplifying each type of
values. We found that contemporary Chinese and Western exemplary persons did not dif-
fer in how much they exemplify Chinese moral values, t(312) = –0.72, ns, and status val-
ues t(312) = –0.28, ns. However, compared to contemporary Chinese exemplary persons,


            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES                       649


contemporary Western exemplary persons exemplified Western moral values to a larger
extent, t(312) = 3.10, p < .01, d = .66.
    Apparently, to the Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates in the current study, among the
historical exemplary persons they respected and aspired to become, Chinese exemplary
persons exemplified Chinese moral values and status values, whereas Western exemplary
persons exemplified status values only. This is the pattern we found repeatedly in Studies
1 and 2. It is interesting that Western moral values appear to have found a way to enter the
participants’ value system, through the contemporary Western exemplary persons. At the
same time, contemporary Chinese exemplary persons ceased to be seen as good represen-
tatives of moral values in Chinese culture. Indeed, they were seen to exemplify status val-
ues more than they do moral values from both Chinese and Western cultures.


                                            GENERAL DISCUSSION

    Across three studies, we found that Hong Kong undergraduates associate traditional
Chinese exemplary persons with both Chinese solidarity and status values and traditional
Western exemplary persons primarily with status values. These findings are consistent with
the social identity theory: People in the local culture tend to maintain favorable perceptions
of their culture by affirming their culture’s social and moral values. These results suggest that
Chinese moral values are more resistant to change; Chinese young people continue to look
up to the Chinese exemplary persons (particularly the historical ones) who carry these val-
ues. Consistent with this idea, Stewart, Bond, Deeds, and Chung (1999) reported that among
Hong Kong Chinese, although the younger generation values autonomy more than the older
generation does, Hong Kong Chinese teenagers give lower priority to early autonomy than
do their Western counterparts. Similarly, Yue and Ng (1999) found in another intergenera-
tional study that traditional moral values such as filial piety are still the top moral concerns
among young people in Beijing, China, another cosmopolitan Chinese society (see also Yeh,
2002). In short, it seems that in Hong Kong and other modern Chinese societies, traditional
Chinese moral values are still heavily emphasized.
    In all three studies, the participants linked Western exemplary persons to status values—
they aspired to become the Western figures that embody Western competence traits that are
associated with success and achievement. These results suggest that during globalization,
Western values that support development of arts, science, and technology (such as cre-
ativity, intelligence, and artistic talents) are particularly likely to be incorporated in the
local culture’s value system.

SPECULATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS

   We also found that the Chinese exemplary persons whom the participants aspired to
become also embody status values. We speculate that Hong Kong’s success in the global
economy might have motivated the participants to construct a distinctive identity that
embodies both Chinese moral values and Western competence traits. This identity distin-
guishes Hong Kong Chinese from the global economic powers and at the same time
acknowledges the economic accomplishment of the Hong Kong Chinese.
   The findings in Study 3 revealed that some Western moral values have begun to take
root in Hong Kong society; contemporary Western exemplary persons who represent
Western moral values are also recognized by Hong Kong undergraduates. At the same



            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
650   JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY


time, contemporary Chinese exemplary persons seem to have taken the role of traditional
Western exemplary persons. They represent what competent persons in the culture are like.
However, unlike their traditional counterparts, their influence in the moral domain seems to
have subsided. We suspect that these changes are related to a shift in the comparison group
in intergroup comparison from Western or global culture to mainland Chinese culture. For
example, there is considerable evidence (Ho, Chau, Chiu, & Peng, 2003; Lam et al., 1999)
that during and after the handover of Hong Kong’s sovereignty to China in 1997, Hong Kong
people seek to distinguish themselves from mainland Chinese by emphasizing Hong Kong’s
democracy (as exemplified by contemporary Western exemplary persons) and economic
accomplishments (as exemplified by traditional Western and contemporary Chinese exem-
plary persons). However, a direct test of this speculation is needed in future research.
    Furthermore, it is still unclear whether, in the process of culture change, the influence
of historical exemplary persons may gradually be diluted when new exemplary persons
from the contemporary era become more important and broadly represented in different
public carriers of culture. If this happens, status and Western moral values will be more
widely represented in Hong Kong people’s value system.
    On one hand, the gradual incorporation of Western exemplary persons and their atten-
dant cultural values into Hong Kong culture attests to culture’s malleability. On the other
hand, the fact that Confucian moral values are constantly being reproduced in Hong Kong
Chinese culture indicates that some aspects of culture are relatively stable (Triandis, 2004).
This analysis raises several interesting questions for future research. First, what are the
determinants of the likelihood that a particular aspect of traditional culture would be repro-
duced and maintained in the culture? Could it be how important this aspect is for the sur-
vival of the human species, as evolutionary psychologists have proposed (Kenrick, Li, &
Butner, 2003)? Could it be how widely this aspect is shared and implicated in interpersonal
communication in the population (Latané, 1996; Lau, Chiu, & Lee, 2001; Lau, Lee, &
Chiu, 2004; Lyons & Kashima, 2001)? Could it be how extensively this aspect has been
instituted in the various public carriers of cultural meanings (Kitayama, 2002)?
    Exemplary persons are public carriers of culture as well as vehicles of intergenerational
transmission of cultural values. However, it is unclear how exemplary persons contribute
to internalization of cultural values. One possible mechanism is through culture priming
(Hong, Chiu, & Kung, 1997; Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martinez, 2000). Many exem-
plary persons (e.g., Abraham Lincoln) are embodiments of core values in a culture (e.g.,
human rights in North America) as well as cultural icons. Every time an individual looks
at these icons, the associated cultural values are activated. Children are often exposed to
these cultural icons in their socialization and educational experiences. Frequent exposure
to these cultural icons, which are often displayed extensively in the environment, ensures
frequent activation and chronic accessibility of the cultural values.
    Through their experiences with the public culture, the human mind is socialized.
Meanwhile, changes in the form and structure of social practices may alter the distribution and
significance of certain exemplary persons in the culture (Menon & Morris, 2001). A future
research challenge is to describe the reciprocal influence between public and private cultures.

LIMITATIONS

   The present studies have several limitations. First, there was greater representation of
female (versus male) participants in the studies. A future study with relatively balanced
gender distribution is needed to assure the generality of the results.



            Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES                       651


   Second, the current studies focus on Hong Kong Chinese’s responses to globalization.
Although the results of Inglehart and Baker’s (2000) cross-national survey reviewed in the
introduction suggest that there are similar value negotiation processes in other countries,
replications of our findings in other societies would help to establish the generality of the
value negotiation processes.
   Last, the measures in our studies were written in Chinese. Previous studies have shown
that language may activate its associated linguistic culture and affect responses to culture-
relevant measures (Ross, Xun, & Wilson, 2002). The participants in our studies are
Chinese–English bilinguals. It is important and interesting to examine whether their
responses would differ when the studies are conducted in an English-language context.


                                                        CONCLUSION

   In conclusion, the rapid rate of globalization has made reproduction of culture and cul-
tural change a timely topic in social psychological investigation. It also offers a window of
opportunity for researchers to uncover some dynamic cultural processes. The goal of the
present article is to explore how local culture responds to globalization from the perspec-
tive of the social identity theory and propose a new method that examines value exempli-
fication of exemplary persons in Chinese and Western cultures. Our results offer some
clues that may lead to the unveiling of some facets of the interactions between multiple
cultures and the multicultural mind.


                                                               NOTES

    1. Although the word exemplary can mean serving as an example or serving as a warning, the research par-
ticipants were instructed to list positive exemplary persons (see method sections in Studies 1 and 3). In this arti-
cle, the referents of exemplary persons are restricted to positive exemplary persons only.
    2. The ratio of exemplary persons generated only by men to those generated only by women was 1:5.64,
which is similar to the ratio of men to women in the sample (1:4).
    3. When participant gender was included as a factor in the analyses, no gender effects were significant. The
pattern of value exemplification was similar for men and women:
    Chinese solidarity: Mmen = 4.73, SD = 0.60; Mwomen = 4.41, SD = 0.84; t(18) = 0.85, ns
    Chinese status: Mmen = 4.74, SD = 0.45; Mwomen = 4.78, SD = 0.42; t(18) = –0.16, ns
    Western solidarity: Mmen = 3.58, SD = 0.79; Mwomen = 4.06, SD = 0.53; t(18) = –1.15, ns
    Western status: Mmen = 4.85, SD = 0.31; Mwomen = 4.74, SD = 0.45; t(18) = 0.57, ns

   4. The ratio of exemplary persons generated only by men to those generated only by women was 1:4.43,
which is similar to the ratio of men to women in the sample (1:4.2).


                                                        REFERENCES

Arnett, J. J. (2002). The psychology of globalization. American Psychologist, 57, 774-783.
Bond, M. H. (1985). Language as a carrier of ethnic stereotypes in Hong Kong. Journal of Social Psychology,
   125, 53-62.
Bond, M. H., & King, A. Y. C. (1985). Coping with the threat of Westernization in Hong Kong. International
   Journal of Intercultural Relations, 9, 351-364.
Callan, V. J., Gallois, C., & Forbes, P. A. (1983). Evaluative reactions to accented English: Ethnicity, sex role,
   and context. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 407-426.
Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (2006). The social psychology of culture. New York: Psychology Press.




              Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
652    JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY


Daniels, C. (2003, April 14). Mr. Coffee. Fortune, pp. 139-141.
Falk, R. (1999). Predatory globalization. Cambridge, UK: Polity.
Fischer, M. M. J. (1999). Emergent forms of life: Anthropologies of late or postmodernities. Annual Review of
    Anthropology, 28, 455-478.
Giles, H., Henwood, K., Coupland, N., Harriman, J., & Coupland, J. (1992). Language attitudes and cognitive
    mediation. Human Communication Research, 18, 500-527.
Ho, D. Y. F., Chau, A. W. L., Chiu, C.-Y., & Peng, S. Q. (2003). Ideological orientation and political transition in
    Hong Kong: Confidence in the future. Political Psychology, 24, 403-413.
Hogg, M. A. (2004). Social identity, self-categorization, and communication in small groups. In S.-H. Ng,
    C. Candlin, & C.-Y. Chiu (Eds.), Language matters: Communication, culture, and identity (pp. 221-243).
    Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong Press.
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social identifications: A social psychology of intergroup relations and group
    processes. London: Routledge.
Hogg, M. A., Joyce, N., & Abrams, D. (1984). Diglossia in Switzerland? A social identity analysis of speaker
    evaluations. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 3, 185-196.
Hong, Y.-Y., Chiu, C. Y., & Kung, T. M. (1997). Bringing culture out in front: Effects of cultural meaning system
    activation on social cognition. In K. Leung, Y. Kashima, U. Kim, & S. Yamaguchi (Eds.), Progress in Asian
    social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 135-146). Singapore: Wiley.
Hong, Y-. Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic con-
    structivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709-720.
Inglehart, R., & Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values.
    American Sociological Review, 65, 19-51.
Kenrick, D. T., Li, M., & Butner, J. (2003). Dynamical evolutionary psychology: Individual decision rules and
    emergent social norms. Psychological Review, 110, 3-28.
Kitayama, S. (2002). Culture and basic psychological processes—Toward a system view of culture: Comment on
    Oyserman et al. (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 89-96.
Krauss, R. M., & Chiu, C.- Y. (1998). Language and social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske-Emory, &
    G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 41-88). New York: Guilford.
Lal, D. (2000). Does modernization require westernization? The Independent Review, 5, 5-24.
Lam, S., Lau, I., Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Peng, S. (1999). Differential emphases on modernity and traditional val-
    ues in social categorization. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23, 237-256.
Latané, B. (1996). Dynamic social impact: The creation of culture by communication. Journal of Communication,
    46, 13-25.
Lau, I. Y.-M., Chiu, C., & Lee, S.- L. (2001). Communication and shared reality: Implications for the psycho-
    logical foundations of culture. Social Cognition, 19, 350-371.
Lau, I. Y.-M., Lee, S.-L., & Chiu, C. (2004). Language, cognition and reality: Constructing shared meanings
    through communication. In M. Schaller & C. Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture
    (pp. 77-100). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Lockwood, P., Marshall, T. C., & Sadler, P. (2005). Promoting success or preventing failure: Cultural differences
    in motivation by positive and negative role models. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 379-392.
Lyczak, R., Fu, G. S., & Ho, A. (1976). Attitudes of Hong Kong bilinguals toward English and Chinese speak-
    ers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 7, 425-438.
Lyons, A., & Kashima, Y. (2001). The reproduction of culture: Communication processes tend to maintain cul-
    tural stereotypes. Social Cognition, 19, 372-394.
Menon, T., & Morris, M. W. (2001). Social structure in North American and Chinese cultures: Reciprocal influ-
    ence between objective and subjective structures. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 2, 27-50.
Parameswaran, R. (2002). Local culture in global media: Excavating colonial and material discourses in National
    Geographic. Communication Theory, 12, 287-315.
Pilkington, H., & Johnson, R. (2003). Relations of identity and power in global/local context. Cultural Studies,
    6, 259-283.
Robertson, R. (1992). Globalization: Social theory and global culture. London: Sage.
Ross, M., Xun, W. Q. E., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). Language and the bicultural self. Personality and Social
    Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1040-1050.
Ryan, E. B., & Giles, H. (Eds.). (1982). Attitudes toward language: Social and applied contexts. London: Arnold.
Ryan, E. B., Giles, H., & Sebastian, R. J. (1982). An integrative perspective for the study of attitudes toward lan-
    guage variation. In E. B. Ryan & H. Giles (Eds.), Attitudes toward language variation: Social and applied
    context (pp. 1-19). London: Arnold.




              Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES                       653


Santana, K. (2003, August 12). MTV goes to Asia. Yale Global. Retrieved August 12, 2003, from http://yaleglobal
    .yale.edu/display.article?id=2211&page=1
Sebastian, R. J., & Ryan, E. B. (1985). Speech cues and social evaluations: Markers of ethnicity, social class and
    age. In H. Giles & R. N. S. Clair (Eds.), Recent advances in language, communication and social psychol-
    ogy (pp. 112-143). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Stewart, S. M., Bond, M. H., Deeds, O., & Chung, S. F. (1999). Intergenerational patterns of values and autonomy
    expectations in cultures of relatedness and separateness. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 575-593.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel
    (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 94-109). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Tong, Y., Hong, Y., Lee, S., & Chiu, C. (1999). Language as a carrier of social identity. International Journal of
    Intercultural Relations, 23, 281-296.
Triandis, H. C. (2004). Dimensions of culture beyond Hofstede. In H. Vinken, J. Soeters, & P. Ester (Eds.),
    Comparing cultures: Dimensions of culture in a comparative perspective (pp. 28-42). Leiden, Netherlands:
    Brill Publishers.
Tzeng, O. C. S., & Henderson, M. M. (1999). Objective and subjective cultural relationships related to industrial
    modernization and social progress. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23, 411-445.
Yeh, K.-H. (2002). Is living with elderly parents still a filial obligation for Chinese people? Journal of Psychology
    in Chinese Societies, 3, 61-84.
Yoon, K. (2003). Retraditionalizing the mobile: Young people’s sociality and mobile phone. Cultural Studies, 6,
    327-343.
Yue, X., & Ng, S. H. (1999). Filial obligations and expectations in China: Current views from young and old
    people in Beijing. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 215-226.
Zhang, J., & Shavitt, S. (2003). Cultural values in advertisements to the Chinese X-generation. Journal of
    Advertising, 32, 23-33.



   Jeanne Ho-Ying Fu is an assistant professor at Nanyang Business School, Nanyang Technological
   University, Singapore. She is interested in the dynamics of biculturals, social cognitive factors underly-
   ing cross-cultural conflict resolution and leadership, and social perception in teams.

   Chi-Yue Chiu is a professor of psychology at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. He received
   his PhD in social–personality psychology at Columbia University and taught at Hong Kong University
   before moving to Illinois. His current research focuses on cultures as knowledge traditions and the social,
   cognitive, and motivational processes that mediate the construction and evolution of social consensus. He
   is also interested in the dynamic interactions of cultural identification and cultural knowledge traditions
   and their implications for cultural competence and intercultural relations.




               Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Ähnlich wie Local cultural responses to globalization

Towards a social psychology of globalization 2011
Towards a social psychology of globalization 2011Towards a social psychology of globalization 2011
Towards a social psychology of globalization 2011
Jimmy Valderrama
 
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docxLinking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
jesssueann
 
Sujay Cultural Frame of Reference FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL.pdf
Sujay Cultural Frame of Reference FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL.pdfSujay Cultural Frame of Reference FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL.pdf
Sujay Cultural Frame of Reference FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL.pdf
Sujay Rao Mandavilli
 
Individual’s Leadership Style Changes Due to Different Culture in the UK
Individual’s Leadership Style Changes Due to Different Culture in the UKIndividual’s Leadership Style Changes Due to Different Culture in the UK
Individual’s Leadership Style Changes Due to Different Culture in the UK
YogeshIJTSRD
 
su20061218
su20061218su20061218
su20061218
Jen W
 
Research as situated cultural practice
Research as situated cultural practiceResearch as situated cultural practice
Research as situated cultural practice
Alfredo Artiles
 
Topic EI Assessment (think about both your results and the proc.docx
Topic   EI Assessment (think about both your results and the proc.docxTopic   EI Assessment (think about both your results and the proc.docx
Topic EI Assessment (think about both your results and the proc.docx
turveycharlyn
 
Journal of Social Sciences 6 (1) 99-112, 2010 ISSN 1549-365.docx
Journal of Social Sciences 6 (1) 99-112, 2010 ISSN 1549-365.docxJournal of Social Sciences 6 (1) 99-112, 2010 ISSN 1549-365.docx
Journal of Social Sciences 6 (1) 99-112, 2010 ISSN 1549-365.docx
tawnyataylor528
 

Ähnlich wie Local cultural responses to globalization (20)

Towards a social psychology of globalization 2011
Towards a social psychology of globalization 2011Towards a social psychology of globalization 2011
Towards a social psychology of globalization 2011
 
Sujay Cultural Symbiosis Personality and Mind-orientation FINAL FINAL FINAL.o...
Sujay Cultural Symbiosis Personality and Mind-orientation FINAL FINAL FINAL.o...Sujay Cultural Symbiosis Personality and Mind-orientation FINAL FINAL FINAL.o...
Sujay Cultural Symbiosis Personality and Mind-orientation FINAL FINAL FINAL.o...
 
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docxLinking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
 
Akkari berry
Akkari berryAkkari berry
Akkari berry
 
The Culture of being Cultured in business
The Culture of being Cultured in businessThe Culture of being Cultured in business
The Culture of being Cultured in business
 
Disjuncture and difference in Global Cultural Economy - Prepared by Fiza Zia ...
Disjuncture and difference in Global Cultural Economy - Prepared by Fiza Zia ...Disjuncture and difference in Global Cultural Economy - Prepared by Fiza Zia ...
Disjuncture and difference in Global Cultural Economy - Prepared by Fiza Zia ...
 
Generic-identity-theory-for-the-twenty-first-century.......-2.pdf
Generic-identity-theory-for-the-twenty-first-century.......-2.pdfGeneric-identity-theory-for-the-twenty-first-century.......-2.pdf
Generic-identity-theory-for-the-twenty-first-century.......-2.pdf
 
Culture and Popular Culture: a case for sociology
Culture and Popular Culture: a case for sociologyCulture and Popular Culture: a case for sociology
Culture and Popular Culture: a case for sociology
 
Sujay Cultural Frame of Reference FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL.pdf
Sujay Cultural Frame of Reference FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL.pdfSujay Cultural Frame of Reference FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL.pdf
Sujay Cultural Frame of Reference FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL FINAL.pdf
 
Individual’s Leadership Style Changes Due to Different Culture in the UK
Individual’s Leadership Style Changes Due to Different Culture in the UKIndividual’s Leadership Style Changes Due to Different Culture in the UK
Individual’s Leadership Style Changes Due to Different Culture in the UK
 
Cultural Anthropology And Cultural Diversity
Cultural Anthropology And Cultural DiversityCultural Anthropology And Cultural Diversity
Cultural Anthropology And Cultural Diversity
 
su20061218
su20061218su20061218
su20061218
 
Research as situated cultural practice
Research as situated cultural practiceResearch as situated cultural practice
Research as situated cultural practice
 
About anthropology
About anthropologyAbout anthropology
About anthropology
 
IJISRT23SEP512 Cross cultural frame of reference.pdf
IJISRT23SEP512 Cross cultural frame of reference.pdfIJISRT23SEP512 Cross cultural frame of reference.pdf
IJISRT23SEP512 Cross cultural frame of reference.pdf
 
The production and consumption of culture and relation of power and culture
The production and consumption of culture and relation of power and cultureThe production and consumption of culture and relation of power and culture
The production and consumption of culture and relation of power and culture
 
Week 8 Ethics and Culture.pdf
Week 8 Ethics and Culture.pdfWeek 8 Ethics and Culture.pdf
Week 8 Ethics and Culture.pdf
 
Topic EI Assessment (think about both your results and the proc.docx
Topic   EI Assessment (think about both your results and the proc.docxTopic   EI Assessment (think about both your results and the proc.docx
Topic EI Assessment (think about both your results and the proc.docx
 
Journal of Social Sciences 6 (1) 99-112, 2010 ISSN 1549-365.docx
Journal of Social Sciences 6 (1) 99-112, 2010 ISSN 1549-365.docxJournal of Social Sciences 6 (1) 99-112, 2010 ISSN 1549-365.docx
Journal of Social Sciences 6 (1) 99-112, 2010 ISSN 1549-365.docx
 
The Study on “Psycho-Social Analysis of Religious Impact on Muslims Behaviour...
The Study on “Psycho-Social Analysis of Religious Impact on Muslims Behaviour...The Study on “Psycho-Social Analysis of Religious Impact on Muslims Behaviour...
The Study on “Psycho-Social Analysis of Religious Impact on Muslims Behaviour...
 

Local cultural responses to globalization

  • 1. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology http://jcc.sagepub.com/ Local Culture's Responses to Globalization : Exemplary Persons and Their Attendant Values Jeanne Ho-Ying Fu and Chi-Yue Chiu Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 2007 38: 636 DOI: 10.1177/0022022107305244 The online version of this article can be found at: http://jcc.sagepub.com/content/38/5/636 Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com On behalf of: International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology Additional services and information for Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology can be found at: Email Alerts: http://jcc.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Subscriptions: http://jcc.sagepub.com/subscriptions Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Citations: http://jcc.sagepub.com/content/38/5/636.refs.html Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 2. LOCAL CULTURE’S RESPONSES TO GLOBALIZATION Exemplary Persons and Their Attendant Values JEANNE HO-YING FU Nanyang Technological University, Singapore CHI-YUE CHIU University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign Taking a social identity perspective, the authors predict that when responding to the dominating influence of the global culture brought in by the Western economic powers, Hong Kong Chinese will recognize the global culture’s superiority in status attributes (e.g., competence, achievement), while at the same time maintaining positive evaluations of Chinese culture on solidarity attributes (traditional moral values). The authors test this hypothesis by examining the Chinese and Western exemplary persons listed spontaneously by Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates and the kind of values carried by these exemplary persons. In three studies, partici- pants associate traditional Chinese exemplary persons with both solidarity and status values and traditional Western exemplary persons with status values only. Additionally, participants also associate contemporary Western exemplary persons with Western rights-based moral values, suggesting that contemporary Western exemplary persons could be important drivers of changes in the moral values in Hong Kong. Keywords: culture; exemplary persons; values; globalization The possible effects of globalization on local cultures have generated heated debates in the social sciences. One critical issue in the debate is whether globalization ultimately leads to homogenization of cultures via global hegemony. Researchers from many social science disci- plines have approached this question from their disciplinary perspective. However, despite some recent calls for psychological analyses of globalization (Arnett, 2002), psychologists have not been enthusiastic participants in this dialogue. Yet any answer to the prospect of cultural homogenization will be incomplete without systematic analyses of the psychological responses to globalization. In this article, we examine how Hong Kong Chinese respond to Western and global cultures by examining the cultural values carried by exemplary persons in Hong Kong. In the following section, we review different views of local culture’s responses to globalization in the social science literature and discuss the psychological responses of Hong Kong Chinese to globalization from the perspective of the social identity theory. Next, we introduce a method we developed to examine psychological responses to globalization and present the results of three studies that looked at how Hong Kong Chinese maintain their Chinese cultural heritage in response to globalization. GLOBALIZATION AND HOMOGENIZATION OF CULTURES Globalization involves the spread of the global economy to regional economies around the world. It requires integration of local economies into the global market. Culturally, the AUTHORS’ NOTE: We would like to thank Harry Triandis, Michael Morris, Yoshi Kashima, and Chung-Fang Yang for their insightful comments on the earlier drafts of this article. Correspondences concerning this article should be sent to Jeanne Ho-Ying Fu, Nanyang Business School, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore 639798; e-mail: hyfu@pmail.ntu.edu.sg. JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY, Vol. 38 No. 5, September 2007 636-653 DOI: 10.1177/0022022107305244 © 2007 Sage Publications 636 Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 3. Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES 637 rapid growth of global linkages and global consciousness has led to reorganization of social life on a global scale. As Robertson (1992) noted, globalization involves “the compression of the world and the intensification of consciousness of the world as a whole” (p. 8). Some con- straints that once bound different knowledge traditions have disintegrated. For example, in China, it is common to find commercial messages promoting individualism and modern life styles in popular magazines targeting the younger generation (Zhang & Shavitt, 2003). There are two different views on how local culture responds to globalization. The first view predicts that globalization would inevitably lead to the demise of local cultures and homogenization of world cultures. According to this view, Western nations, representatives of the global culture, are not only perceived to be more economically advanced than non- Western nations, but they are often seen as reference nations in the realm of cultural restruc- turing. The global culture, which privileges consumerism, individualism, competition, and efficiency, has been characterized in some non-Western countries as new, modern, scientific, and results oriented (Lam, Lau, Chiu, Hong, & Peng, 1999; Pilkington & Johnson, 2003). In addition, the global market emphasizes the use of scientific knowledge and technol- ogy to achieve controllable and predictable results. To compete in the global market, instead of relying on traditional knowledge and experiences, local economies need scien- tific knowledge and professional training in business administration. It is not surprising that globalization in many developing countries is often accompanied by a vast demand for science education and MBA programs (Chiu & Hong, 2006; Fischer, 1999; Tzeng & Henderson, 1999). Globalization has also transformed the consumption patterns in local economies, as evi- dent in the spread of global “brand-name” goods and restaurant chains (Daniels, 2003) and the proliferation of global advertising that exploits similar basic material desires and cre- ates similar lifestyles (Parameswaran, 2002). Indeed, the global success of global brands such as McDonald’s and Starbucks has led some writers to predict an inevitable coloniza- tion of world cultures by international corporate brands (Falk, 1999). However, other writers hold a different view, believing that some aspects of the local culture would survive the erosive effects of globalization. For example, Lal (2000) believes that although the material beliefs (beliefs pertinent to making a living) in the global cul- ture will gain popularity as a country is integrated into the global market, beliefs pertinent to social relationship and morality in local cultures are relatively resistant to the influence of globalization. Additionally, multinational survey data revealed that although economic development is accompanied by increased adherence to values that emphasize secular- ism, scientific rationality, and individualism, the broad knowledge tradition that defines personhood and sociality in a society (e.g., Protestantism, Confucianism) is relatively resistant to the erosive effects of globalization (Inglehart & Baker, 2000). Likewise, ethno- graphic studies of mobile phone use in South Korea (Yoon, 2003) and MTV in East Asia (Santana, 2003) found that proliferation of global products and services in East Asia, instead of destroying local cultures, has played a crucial role in reinforcing and reinvent- ing traditional moral values in local communities. THE SOCIAL IDENTITY PERSPECTIVE The social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) offers a theoretical perspective to understand how a local culture may accept the values embedded in the global culture with- out giving up the core values in the local culture. In the face of globalization, a central Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 4. 638 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY issue confronting the local culture is how it is possible to acknowledge the competitive advantages of the capitalist logic and its attendant values in the global market and at the same time affirm the positive distinctiveness of the heritage culture. Based on the assump- tion that group memberships constitute an integral part of an individual’s self-definition, social identity theorists have expounded on the different cognitive and behavioral strate- gies a low-status group would adopt to protect its collective self-esteem when confronting the dominating influence of another group (Hogg & Abrams, 1988). One such strategy involves differential evaluations of the in-group and the dominating group on different dimensions. Early empirical demonstrations of this strategy made use of the fact that language vari- ations within and between speech communities are important markers of the language user’s social group memberships. As such, perceivers can infer a speaker’s group mem- berships from his or her voice, and the characteristics attributed to the speaker may reflect the perceivers’ attitudes toward the speaker’s inferred membership. In these studies, the participants listened to recordings of bilingual speakers reading a passage in one of the tar- get languages and to a translation of the same passage in the second target language. Unaware that they were listening to two readings of each of several bilinguals, the partic- ipants rated the speakers’ personalities (see Krauss & Chiu, 1998; Ryan & Giles, 1982, for reviews). Typically, ratings of speakers’ personalities can be organized into two evaluative dimensions: status and solidarity (Ryan, Giles, & Sebastian, 1982). The status dimension, including such competence-related attributes as successful, competent, and intelligent, is positively correlated with sociopolitical power. Subordinate linguistic groups usually give more favorable ratings to the dominant linguistic groups than to their own group on this dimension (e.g., Callan, Gallois, & Forbes, 1983; Giles, Henwood, Coupland, Harriman, & Coupland, 1992; Lyczak, Fu, & Ho, 1976; Sebastian & Ryan, 1985). The solidarity dimension includes social, moral attributes such as trustworthy, friendly, kind, and benevolent. Minority group members typically rate speakers of their own lan- guage more favorably than speakers of the standard dominant group language on the soli- darity dimension (e.g., Bond, 1985; Giles et al., 1992; Hogg, Joyce, & Abrams, 1984; Lyczak et al., 1976; Tong, Hong, Lee, & Chiu, 1999). This pattern of evaluation may reflect an attempt by the less powerful group to maintain positive evaluation of the group while acknowledging the power difference between its group and the dominant group. Extending this finding to local cultures’ responses to the global culture, we may expect individuals in a local culture to acknowledge the dominating global culture’s superiority on the status dimension, while maintaining a positive perception of the local culture on the solidarity dimension. Individuals adopting this strategy would distinguish between modernization and Westernization, with modernization involving acquisition of specific skills and competencies that have fueled the economic development in the West and Westernization involving adoption of the Western social–moral values. With this strat- egy, people in the local community can accept modernization and resist Westernization at the same time. As a result, they may not see modernization as a threat to the fundamental social and moral values in the local culture. Bond and King (1985) have applied the social identity perspective to understand Hong Kong Chinese’s responses to the global culture, which was brought in through coloniza- tion and then institutionalized through rapid industrialization. In a survey, Bond and King found that most Hong Kong Chinese respondents (about 70%) believed that modernization involved technology, behavior, or material progress, whereas Westernization involved val- ues, thinking, or Western cultural traditions. Almost two thirds of respondents (64%) also Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 5. Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES 639 believed that modernization could proceed in Hong Kong without much cultural implica- tions. About half of them claimed that they managed to preserve their Chineseness by holding onto basic Chinese moral values, such as filial piety and respect for teachers. METHODOLOGICAL APPROACH USING EXEMPLARY PERSONS As the preliminary findings from Bond and King (1985) indicated, in Hong Kong, the global culture is generally perceived to have originated in the West. Accordingly, in our study of responses to the global culture in Hong Kong, we focus on the extent to which status and solidarity values are exemplified by the individuals whom Hong Kong Chinese consider to be exemplary persons in China and the West. We chose this analytic strategy for two reasons. First, exemplary persons are important carriers of culture. Exemplary persons are real or fictional figures who possess qualities that are so positively evaluated in the culture that others would do well to copy.1 Exemplary persons may be persons with outstanding achievements or persons who have endured mis- fortunes or met with failures in the pursuit of socially desirable goals. Both successful and unsuccessful exemplary persons can incite imitation among individuals in the culture (Lockwood, Marshall, & Sadler, 2005). Although successful exemplary persons inspire individuals to pursue similar excellence, unsuccessful exemplary persons encourage indi- viduals to learn from the exemplary persons’ mistakes and from their persistence in pur- suing socially desirable goals. Widely recognized exemplary persons are usually idealized figures that embody certain highly valued virtues in the culture (Chiu & Hong, 2006). As such, they are public repre- sentations of cultural ideals and important vehicles for transmitting cultural values. In Chinese societies, young children often learn cultural values by reading or listening to sto- ries about exemplary persons whose behaviors exemplify these values. For example, the value of “filial piety” is introduced to children through the well-known Twenty-Four Parables of Filial Piety, which describes and glorifies various filial behaviors displayed by 24 famous sons or daughters in Chinese history. Similarly, in Hong Kong, young children often learn Western values through stories of exemplary persons in Western cultures. For example, they learn the value of honesty through the story of the U.S. President George Washington and the value of creativity through the stories of Marie Curie, James Watt, Thomas Edison, and the Wright brothers. Western moral values such as equality, human rights, and freedom are learned through the stories of Abraham Lincoln. Second, according to the social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979), group members collectively construct prototypes of the group to represent the group’s defining, positively distinctive attributes. A prototype is often an exemplary person in the group. According to Hogg (2004), “prototypes rarely describe average or typical ingroup members—rather they are polarized away from outgroup features and describe ideal, often hypothetical, ingroup members” (p. 229). Thus, according to the social identity theory, exemplary per- sons in a local culture and their attendant values represent the culture’s defining qualities from which its members derive a sense of positive distinctiveness and collective esteem vis-à-vis comparison with the global culture. As such, exemplary persons and their atten- dant values provide rich materials for understanding globalization and value negotiation. In this article, we reported three studies conducted in Hong Kong that used the method described earlier to examine the exemplification of status and solidarity values by individ- uals who are considered to be exemplary persons from local (Chinese) culture and global Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 6. 640 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY (Western) culture. In each study, a sample of Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates listed the exemplary persons who came to their mind readily and rated the extent to which these exemplary persons represented competence values and social, moral values in Chinese and Western cultures. We predicted that the spontaneously listed Chinese exemplary persons are likely to embody Chinese social, moral values or values that serve solidarity functions. Contrarily, spontaneously listed Western exemplary persons are likely to embody compe- tence values (versus Western moral values) or values that reflect socioeconomic status. STUDY 1 METHOD Participants. Twenty Hong Kong undergraduate students (4 men and 16 women) from an introductory psychology class participated in this study in return for course requirement credit. Their mean age was 19.40 (SD = 0.59). Measures and procedures. On arrival and signing the consent form, the participants completed a questionnaire that was written in Chinese. They listed six exemplary persons in Chinese culture and six exemplary persons in Western cultures. The order of listing Chinese and Western exemplary persons was counterbalanced. Specifically, they were instructed to “list six persons in Chinese (Western) culture who deserve much respect and are worthy of imitating.” Next, the participants indicated on a 6-point Likert-type scale (from 1 = not at all to 6 = very much) the extent to which each of these exemplary persons was an embodiment of six solidarity values from Chinese culture (filial piety, patriotism, loyalty, righteousness, sacrifice, and bravery) and six status values (intelligence, persis- tence, wisdom, power, artistic talent, and creativity). The 12 values were presented to the participants in a random order, with the same order for all participants. These 12 values were selected based on the results of a pilot study. In the pilot study, we first identified 32 (23 men, 9 women) exemplary persons from Chinese culture and 22 (18 male, 4 female) from Western cultures in Hong Kong’s grade school curriculum. Next, we had a separate sample of 121 Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates (22% male; mean age = 19.26, SD = 1.38) listed up to five values associated with these models. Half of the participants in the pilot study listed values associated with the Chinese exemplary persons, and the remaining half listed values associated with the Western ones. The six most fre- quently mentioned values associated with the Chinese exemplary persons were patriotism, bravery, righteousness, loyalty, self-sacrifice, and filial piety. These are all core Confucian moral values. We used these six values to represent solidarity values in the main study. The six most frequently mentioned values associated with the Western exemplary per- sons were creativity, power, artistic talent, intelligence, persistence, and wisdom. These values are related to human competencies and are typical status values. Moral values in the Western cultures (freedom, human rights) were seldom mentioned. These results are con- sistent with our hypothesis: In Hong Kong, Chinese exemplary persons tend to be associ- ated with solidarity values and Western exemplary persons with status values. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION We compiled a list of the exemplary persons listed by the participants in the main study. Of the 115 Chinese exemplary persons listed, 98.2% were men.2 The six most frequently Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 7. Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES 641 mentioned exemplary persons in Chinese culture were Confucius (551 BC to 479 BC, philosopher; the percentage of participants who listed this exemplary person was 80.0%), Sun Yat-Sen (1866 to 1925, founder of the Republic of China; listed by 55.0%), Mencius (370 BC to 286 BC, philosopher; 55.0%), Li Bai (701 to 762, poet; 45.0%), Zhuangzi (369 BC to 286 BC, philosopher; 40.0%), and Du Fu (712 to 770, poet; 40.0%). Of the 105 Western exemplary persons listed, 85.7% were men. The six most frequently mentioned exemplary persons in Western cultures were James Watt (75.0%), Mother Teresa (30.0%), Sir Isaac Newton (25.0%), Napoleon Bonaparte (25.0%), Jesus Christ (25.0%), and Ludwig van Beethoven (20.0%). For each participant and for each value, we took the mean of the ratings assigned to the listed Chinese exemplary persons and the mean of the ratings assigned to the listed Western exemplary persons. Next, we took the mean of the six values in each of the two value cate- gories (Chinese solidarity values or status values). A 2 (Culture of Exemplary Person) X 2 (Value Category) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a significant main effect of value category, F(1, 18) = 9.55, p < .01, η2p = .35, and a significant main effect of culture, F(1, 18) = 23.21, p < .001, η2p = .56. We interpreted these main effects in the con- text of the significant Culture X Value Category interaction, F(1, 18) = 8.97, p < .01, η2p = .33.3 As shown in Figure 1, Chinese exemplary persons were perceived to exemplify Chinese solidarity values (M = 4.47, SD = 0.79) and status values (M = 4.77, SD = 0.41) to a similar extent, F(1, 19) = 3.49, ns. Western exemplary persons exemplified status values (M = 4.77, SD = 0.42) more than they did Chinese solidarity values (M = 3.96, SD = 0.60), F(1, 18) = 49.26, p < .001, η2p = .73. In addition, Chinese and Western exemplary persons did not differ in how much they exemplified status values, t(19) = 0.00, ns. However, compared to Western exemplary persons, Chinese exemplary persons were perceived to exemplify solidarity val- ues more, t(19) = 2.55, p < .05, d = .73. We also used exemplary persons as our unit of analysis, treating each exemplary person listed by the participant as a case in the analysis. First, we took the mean ratings of the six val- ues in each value category for each exemplary person. Next, we performed a 2 (Culture of Exemplary Person) X 2 (Value Category) ANOVA on the mean ratings. Gender of the exem- plary persons was not included in the analysis because the small number of female exemplary persons (7.66%) yielded highly unstable estimates of the dependent measures for female exemplary persons. As in the analysis that used participants as the unit of analysis, there was a significant main effect of value category, F(1, 220) = 67.59, p < .001, η2p = .24, a significant main effect of culture, F(1, 220) = 8.57, p < .005, η2p = .04, and a significant Culture X Value Category interaction, F(1, 220) = 14.61, p < .001, η2p = .06. Chinese and Western exemplary persons did not differ in how much they exemplify status values, t(221) = –0.07, ns. However, compared to Western exemplary persons, Chinese exemplary persons were perceived to exem- plify Chinese solidarity values more, t(221) = 2.11, p < .05, d = 0.50. In short, the pilot study and the main study yielded consistent results. In the pilot study, when presented with exemplary persons in Hong Kong’s grade school curriculum, Hong Kong Chinese college students spontaneously listed solidarity values when the exemplary persons were from Chinese culture and status values when the exemplary persons were from Western cultures. In the main study, compared to Western exemplary persons, Chinese exemplary persons exemplified Chinese moral (solidarity) values more. These results are consistent with the social identity theory prediction that Hong Kong Chinese continue to value Chinese moral values; the exemplary persons from Chinese culture that the participants aspired to imitate were perceived to exemplify these values more than those from Western culture. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 8. 642 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 5 4.5 4 3.5 3 Chinese Exemplary Persons Western Exemplary Persons Solidarity Values Status Values Figure 1. Mean Ratings of How Much Chinese and Western Exemplary Persons Exemplified Different Categories of Values in Study 1 NOTE: Error bars denote standard errors. It is interesting that in the main study, Chinese and Western exemplary persons exem- plified status values to a similar extent. We speculate that because of Hong Kong’s success in the global economy, many Hong Kong Chinese no longer perceive Western culture as having superior status than Chinese culture in terms of economic prosperity. Furthermore, they have started to use Chinese exemplary persons who embody both the perceived moral superiority of Chinese culture and the competence traits required for success in the global economy as materials for constructing a positively distinctive identity. STUDY 2 Although Study 1 provided some initial support for our hypothesis, it could be argued that the findings are due to a priming effect—simply asking participants to think of any Chinese or Western person might be able to produce the pattern of results obtained in Study 1. In other words, the effects may have nothing to do with the stimulus person being an exemplary person. In the current study, to address this interpretive ambiguity, we repeated Study 1 with a slightly different procedure. In Study 1, the participants listed exemplary persons. In the present study, the participants listed historical figures from Chinese and Western cultures and rated these figures in terms of how much they exemplified Chinese moral (solidarity) values and competence (status) values. Next, we had a group of independent judges rate how positive or negative each historical figure was. According to the social identity theory, prototypes of a group are idealized and positively evaluated in-group members (Hogg, Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 9. Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES 643 2004). Thus, the pattern of results in Study 1 should emerge only when the historical figures are positive figures (when they are like exemplary persons). However, according to the priming account, the pattern of results in Study 1 would emerge regardless of whether the historical figures are positive or negative figures. For instance, as long as the historical figures are Chinese persons, they should exemplify Chinese traditional moral values. METHOD Participants. Fifty Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates (15 men, 34 women, and 1 did not report gender), with a mean age of 20.25 (SD = 2.26), participated in the current study in return for course requirement credit. Procedures. The procedures were the same as those in Study 1, with the exception that the participants were instructed to list up to six historical figures in Chinese history and six historical figures in Western history. Four Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates (2 men, 2 women; mean age = 19.50, SD = 0.58) assigned a valence score to each historical figure according to the following scheme: 1 = negative figures, 2 = neutral figures, and 3 = pos- itive figures (a few figures were perceived as ambivalent or vague, and they were coded as missing data). The correlations of the valence ratings between the four coders ranged from .57 to .88 (median = .81). An average valence score was obtained for each historical figure by taking the mean of the four coders’ ratings. The mean of the valence scores was 2.48 (SD = 0.59). RESULTS AND DISCUSSION A list of historical figures generated by the participants was compiled. Of the 296 Chinese historical figures generated, 91.9% were men. The six most frequently mentioned figures in Chinese history were Qin Shi Huang (259 BC to 210 BC, Emperor of the Qin Dynasty; percentage of participants who mentioned this figure was 62.0%), Yue Fei (1103 to 1141, a general in the Sung Dynasty; 48.0%), Sun Yat-Sen (44.0%), Li Bai (32.0%), Liu Bei (161 to 223, ruler of the State of Shu; 30.0%), and Zhuge Liang (181-234, a chief mil- itary advisor; 26.0%). Of the 282 Western historical figures listed, 96.1% were men. The six most frequently mentioned figures in Western history were Adolf Hitler (56.0%), Napoleon Bonaparte (48.0%), George Washington (32.0%), Alexander the Great (30.0%), Abraham Lincoln (28.0%), and Marco Polo (28.0%). We treated each historical figure as a case in our analysis. For each historical figure, we computed the mean exemplification rating on each of the two value categories. We mean centered the valence score to avoid potential problems caused by multicollinearity. Next, we fitted a 2 (Culture of Historical Figure) X 2 (Value Category) X Valence generalized linear model to the data, with valence as a mean-centered continuous variable. Again, gender of the historical figures was not included in the analysis because very few female historical figures were listed (6.03%). The following effects were significant: the main effects of culture, F(1, 558) = 9.84, p < .001, η2p = .02, valence, F(1, 558) = 78.74, p < .001, η2p = .12, value category, F(1, 558) = 91.42, p < .001, η2p = .14, the Culture X Valence interaction, F(1, 558) = 13.55, p < .001, η2p = .02, the Culture X Value Category interac- tion, F(1, 558) = 41.61, p < .001, η2p = .07, the Value Category X Valence interaction, Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 10. 644 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY F(1, 558) = 21.99, p < .001, η2p = .04, and Culture X Value Category X Valence interaction, F(1, 558) = 42.73, p < .001, η2p = .07. Follow-up simple slope analyses showed that for positive historical figures (valence cen- tered at 1 SD above the mean), there was a significant main effects of culture, F(1, 558) = 23.42, p < .001, η2p = .04, value category, F(1, 558) = 12.09, p < .001, η2p = .02, and Culture X Value Category interaction, F(1, 558) = 84.98, p < .001, η2p = .13. As shown in Figure 2, positive Chinese historical figures (valence > mean) exemplified Chinese solidarity values (M = 4.56, SD = 1.11) slightly more than status values (M = 4.37, SD = 0.82), F(1, 196) = 5.91, p < .05, η2p = .03, whereas positive Western historical figures (valence > mean) exem- plified status values (M = 4.46, SD = 0.75) more than Chinese solidarity values (M = 3.71, SD = 1.10), F(1, 198) = 83.33, p < .001, η2p = .30. Furthermore, compared to positive Western historical figures, positive Chinese historical figures exemplified Chinese solidarity values more, t(396) = 3.29, p < .01, d = .77. As in Study 1, Chinese and Western historical figures did not differ in how much they exemplified status values, t(396) = –0.51, ns. For negative historical figures (valence centered at 1 SD below the mean), the only signif- icant effect was the main effect of value category, F(1, 558) = 100.89, p < .001, η2p = .15. Status values (M = 4.11, SD = 0.88) were exemplified more than solidarity values (M = 3.36, SD = 1.16), t(182) = 6.95, p < .01, d = .73, by these negative historical figures. The Culture X Value Category interaction was not significant, F(1, 558) = 0.00, ns. In short, the results from Study 1 were replicated when the valence of the historical figures was positive. However, when the valence of the historical figures was negative, Chinese and Western historical figures did not differ in the types of values they exempli- fied. These findings eliminated the alternative hypothesis that any Chinese or Western stimulus person would produce the value association pattern in Study 1. STUDY 3 Results from Studies 1 and 2 showed that Chinese exemplary persons or positively eval- uated Chinese historical figures are associated with both solidarity (Chinese moral) and status values and Western exemplary persons or positively evaluated historical figures are associated with status values only. The current study was designed to replicate these find- ings and address several concerns. First, it may be argued that the participants recalled the widely recognized Chinese and Western exemplary persons in Hong Kong but did not admire or aspire personally to possess the values these persons exemplify. To address this issue, in Study 3, participants were asked to list the exemplary persons they themselves admired and aspired to become. In addition, they were not constrained to list exemplary persons from any particular culture. Instead, we sorted the exemplary persons the partici- pants listed according to the culture to which the exemplary persons belonged. Second, although Hong Kong undergraduates do not associate Western exemplary per- sons with Chinese moral values, they may associate them with Western moral values. It is possible that through their exposure to Western cultures, Hong Kong Chinese also aspire to become some Western exemplary persons who embody Western moral values. If that is the case, Western moral values may find a way to enter the value system in Hong Kong. To explore this possibility, in the current study, we included moral values from Western culture, such as human rights and individuality, and asked the participants to rate the exemplary persons on these values. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 11. Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES 645 5 4.5 4 3.5 3 Positive Positive Negative Negative Chinese Western Chinese Western Historical Figures Solidarity Values Status Values Figure 2. Mean Ratings of How Much Positive and Negative Chinese and Western Historical Figures Exemplified Different Categories of Values in Study 2 NOTE: Error bars denote standard errors. Finally, according to the social identity theory, a group may construct different proto- types when the specific out-group that forms the basis of social comparison changes (Hogg, 2004). Similarly, a new set of exemplary persons carrying different values may emerge and become widely recognized in a society as the comparison group changes. In the context of Hong Kong, the return of Hong Kong to China in 1997 has given rise to new intercultural dynamics: A rapidly growing concern in the city during and immediately after the political transition was the positive distinctiveness of Hong Kong people vis-à-vis Chinese mainlanders. There is evidence that during this period, Hong Kong people polar- ized themselves away from Chinese mainlanders by emphasizing Hong Kong people’s adherence to Western moral values such as human rights and democracy (Lam et al., 1999; Tong et al., 1999). Indeed, soon after Hong Kong was returned to China in 1997, the city was known to the world as the City of Protest. There were large-scale rallies, with tens of thousands of protesters taking to the streets in Hong Kong to protest for freedom of speech and other political rights. Thus, it is possible that in response to this shift in the out-group as the basis for social comparison, Hong Kong Chinese have constructed a new set of exemplary persons who embody such Western moral values as democracy and human rights. Aggregating scores across exemplary persons from different epochs in the previous two studies might have overlooked this subtle development. In the current study, we explored this issue by examining the values exemplified by historical and contemporary exemplary persons. We assumed that the values represented by historical exemplary Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 12. 646 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY persons are likely to be longstanding values in the culture and values represented by con- temporary exemplary persons are likely to be the values that were incorporated into the culture’s value system relatively recently. Thus, the values historical and contemporary exemplary persons exemplify may reflect value change in a culture. METHOD Participants. Thirty-one Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates (6 men, 25 women; mean age = 20, SD = 1.79) received US$7.50 for their participation in the present study. Procedures. On arrival and signing the consent form, participants were instructed to “list 12 exemplary persons you respect and aspire to become” and to rate on a 6-point Likert-type scale (from not at all to very much) how much each of these exemplary per- sons exemplified 18 values. Of these values, 12 were those used in Studies 1 and 2. The remaining 6 were Western rights–based or individualist values: equality, human rights, democracy, individuality, freedom, and uniqueness. Next, half of the participants rated how well each exemplary person represented Chinese culture before they rated how well the exemplary person represented Western culture. The remaining participants performed the same ratings in the reverse order. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Two hundred and sixty-eight exemplary persons (69.3% males) were listed.4 The first author categorized the exemplary persons into Chinese or Western exemplary persons. Exemplary persons who did not fall into these categories (4.5%) were excluded from fur- ther analyses. The exemplary persons were also categorized into historical or contempo- rary exemplary persons depending on whether they had passed away before the end of World War II (1945). Cultural representativeness of exemplary persons. Treating each exemplary person as a case, we performed a 2 (Culture: Chinese or Western) X 2 (Epoch: Historical or Contemporary) ANOVA on how well the exemplary persons represented Chinese culture. The results indicated that Chinese exemplary persons (M = 4.28, SD = 1.30) were seen as better representatives of Chinese culture than were Western ones (M = 2.28, SD = 1.04), F(1, 243) = 222.50, p < .001, η2p = .48, for the main effect of culture. Historical exemplary persons (M = 3.80, SD = 1.80) were seen as better representatives of Chinese culture than were contemporary ones (M = 3.33, SD = 1.36), F(1, 243) = 18.43, p < .001, η2p = .07, for the main effect of epoch. The Culture X Epoch interaction was also significant, F(1, 243) = 18.66, p < .001, η2p = .07. Historical Chinese exemplary persons (M = 5.17, SD = 1.08) were seen as better representatives of Chinese culture than were contemporary Chinese exemplary persons (M = 3.87, SD = 1.19), F(1, 149) = 41.18, p < .001, η2p = .22, whereas historical and contemporary Western exemplary persons did not differ in how well they represented Chinese culture (M = 2.28, SD = 1.05 versus M = 2.28, SD = 1.04), F(1, 94) < 0.01, ns. As the means indicated, both historical and contemporary Western exemplary persons were not seen to be representative of Chinese culture. A similar analysis performed on how well the exemplary persons represented Western culture revealed that Western exemplary persons (M = 4.63, SD = 1.00) were seen as better Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 13. Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES 647 TABLE 1 Correlation Between How Well Exemplary Persons Represented Chinese and Western Cultures and How Much They Exemplified Different Categories of Values (Study 3) Value Category Solidarity Status Western (Chinese Moral) Value Value Moral Value Representativeness of Chinese culture .15* –.08 –.08 Representativeness of Western culture –.12* .17* .23** *p < .05. **p < .01. representatives of Western culture than were Chinese exemplary persons (M = 2.88, SD = 1.34), F(1, 242) = 159.65, p < .001, η2p = .40, for the main effect of culture. In addition, con- temporary exemplary persons (M = 3.71, SD = 1.29) were seen as better representatives of Western culture than were historical ones (M = 3.31, SD = 1.74; d = .26), F(1, 242) = 12.52, p < .001, η2p = .05, for the main effect of epoch. The significant Culture of Exemplary Person X Epoch interaction, F(1, 242) = 14.78, p < .001, η2p = .06, indicated that historical and con- temporary Western exemplary persons did not differ in how well they represented Western culture (M = 4.64, SD = 1.02 versus M = 4.60, SD = 0.99), F(1, 94) = 0.05, ns. It is interest- ing that contemporary Chinese exemplary persons (M = 3.25, SD = 1.18) were viewed as better representatives of Western culture than were historical Chinese exemplary persons (M = 2.10, SD = 1.32), F(1, 148) = 28.15, p < .001, η2p = .16. Attendant values of the exemplary persons. Table 1 shows the correlation between how well the exemplary persons represented Chinese and Western cultures and how much they exemplified the three categories of values. The better the exemplary persons represented Chinese culture, the more they were perceived to exemplify Chinese moral values (r = .15, p < .05). The better the exemplary persons represented Western culture, the more they were perceived to exemplify Western moral values (r = .23, p < .01) and status values (r = .17, p < .05). Finally, the better the exemplary persons represented Western culture, the less they were perceived to exemplify Chinese moral values (r = –.12, p < .05). To examine how exemplary persons from different cultures and epochs differed in the values they exemplified, we performed a 2 (Culture of Exemplary Person) X 2 (Epoch of Exemplary Person) X 3 (Value Category) ANOVA on the exemplification ratings. The fol- lowing effects were significant: the main effect of value category, F(2, 486) = 26.02, p < .001, η2p = .10, the Culture X Epoch interaction, F(2, 243) = 5.92, p < .05, η2p = .02, the Culture X Value Category interaction, F(2, 486) = 9.12, p < .001, η2p = .04, the Epoch X Value Category interaction, F(2, 486) = 4.20, p <.05, η2p = .02, and the Culture X Epoch X Value Category interaction, F(2, 486) = 7.74, p < .001, η2p = .03. All the effects were interpreted in the context of the highest order three-way Culture X Epoch X Value Category interaction. Figure 3 shows that historical Chinese exemplary persons exemplified Chinese moral values (M = 4.38, SD = 1.14) and status values (M = 4.34, SD = 0.82) more than they did Western moral values (M = 3.92, SD = 1.11), F(2, 94) = 4.42, p < .05, η2p = .09. In con- trast, historical Western exemplary persons exemplified status values (M = 4.54, SD = 0.67) more than they did Western moral values (M = 3.86, SD = 1.22) and Chinese moral values (M = 3.60, SD = 1.35), F(2, 84) = 12.63, p < .001, η2p = .23. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 14. 648 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY 4.5 4 3.5 3 Historical Historical Contemporary Contemporary Chinese Western Chinese Western Exemplary Persons Solidarity Values Status Values Western Moral Values Figure 3. Mean Ratings of How Much Historical and Contemporary Exemplary Persons From Chinese and Western Cultures Exemplified Different Categories of Values in Study 3 NOTE: Error bars denote standard errors. Furthermore, compared to historical Western exemplary persons, historical Chinese exemplary persons exemplified Chinese moral values more, t(182) = 2.41, p < .05, d = .62. As in Studies 1 and 2, Chinese and Western exemplary persons did not differ in how much they exemplified status values, t(182) = 1.17, ns. In short, the findings on Chinese moral and status values exemplified by historical exemplary persons were similar to those in Studies 1 and 2. Results also showed that the two groups of exemplary persons did not dif- fer in how much they exemplified Western moral values, t(182) = 0.2, ns. We next examined the values associated with contemporary exemplary persons. The contemporary Chinese exemplary persons were similar to the historical Western exem- plary persons in terms of pattern of values they exemplified. Contemporary Chinese exemplary persons exemplified status values (M = 4.18, SD = 0.85) more than they did Western moral values (M = 3.65, SD = 1.08) and Chinese moral values (M = 3.58, SD = 0.91), F(2, 204) = 22.81, p < .001, η2p = .18. Finally, contemporary Western exemplary persons exemplified Western moral values (M = 4.29, SD = 0.86) and status values (M = 4.23; SD = 0.74) more than they did Chinese moral values (M = 3.75, SD = 1.02), F(2, 104) = 9.81, p < .001, η2p = .16. Similar to the historical exemplary persons, we explored whether contemporary Chinese and Western exemplary persons showed differences in exemplifying each type of values. We found that contemporary Chinese and Western exemplary persons did not dif- fer in how much they exemplify Chinese moral values, t(312) = –0.72, ns, and status val- ues t(312) = –0.28, ns. However, compared to contemporary Chinese exemplary persons, Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 15. Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES 649 contemporary Western exemplary persons exemplified Western moral values to a larger extent, t(312) = 3.10, p < .01, d = .66. Apparently, to the Hong Kong Chinese undergraduates in the current study, among the historical exemplary persons they respected and aspired to become, Chinese exemplary persons exemplified Chinese moral values and status values, whereas Western exemplary persons exemplified status values only. This is the pattern we found repeatedly in Studies 1 and 2. It is interesting that Western moral values appear to have found a way to enter the participants’ value system, through the contemporary Western exemplary persons. At the same time, contemporary Chinese exemplary persons ceased to be seen as good represen- tatives of moral values in Chinese culture. Indeed, they were seen to exemplify status val- ues more than they do moral values from both Chinese and Western cultures. GENERAL DISCUSSION Across three studies, we found that Hong Kong undergraduates associate traditional Chinese exemplary persons with both Chinese solidarity and status values and traditional Western exemplary persons primarily with status values. These findings are consistent with the social identity theory: People in the local culture tend to maintain favorable perceptions of their culture by affirming their culture’s social and moral values. These results suggest that Chinese moral values are more resistant to change; Chinese young people continue to look up to the Chinese exemplary persons (particularly the historical ones) who carry these val- ues. Consistent with this idea, Stewart, Bond, Deeds, and Chung (1999) reported that among Hong Kong Chinese, although the younger generation values autonomy more than the older generation does, Hong Kong Chinese teenagers give lower priority to early autonomy than do their Western counterparts. Similarly, Yue and Ng (1999) found in another intergenera- tional study that traditional moral values such as filial piety are still the top moral concerns among young people in Beijing, China, another cosmopolitan Chinese society (see also Yeh, 2002). In short, it seems that in Hong Kong and other modern Chinese societies, traditional Chinese moral values are still heavily emphasized. In all three studies, the participants linked Western exemplary persons to status values— they aspired to become the Western figures that embody Western competence traits that are associated with success and achievement. These results suggest that during globalization, Western values that support development of arts, science, and technology (such as cre- ativity, intelligence, and artistic talents) are particularly likely to be incorporated in the local culture’s value system. SPECULATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH DIRECTIONS We also found that the Chinese exemplary persons whom the participants aspired to become also embody status values. We speculate that Hong Kong’s success in the global economy might have motivated the participants to construct a distinctive identity that embodies both Chinese moral values and Western competence traits. This identity distin- guishes Hong Kong Chinese from the global economic powers and at the same time acknowledges the economic accomplishment of the Hong Kong Chinese. The findings in Study 3 revealed that some Western moral values have begun to take root in Hong Kong society; contemporary Western exemplary persons who represent Western moral values are also recognized by Hong Kong undergraduates. At the same Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 16. 650 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY time, contemporary Chinese exemplary persons seem to have taken the role of traditional Western exemplary persons. They represent what competent persons in the culture are like. However, unlike their traditional counterparts, their influence in the moral domain seems to have subsided. We suspect that these changes are related to a shift in the comparison group in intergroup comparison from Western or global culture to mainland Chinese culture. For example, there is considerable evidence (Ho, Chau, Chiu, & Peng, 2003; Lam et al., 1999) that during and after the handover of Hong Kong’s sovereignty to China in 1997, Hong Kong people seek to distinguish themselves from mainland Chinese by emphasizing Hong Kong’s democracy (as exemplified by contemporary Western exemplary persons) and economic accomplishments (as exemplified by traditional Western and contemporary Chinese exem- plary persons). However, a direct test of this speculation is needed in future research. Furthermore, it is still unclear whether, in the process of culture change, the influence of historical exemplary persons may gradually be diluted when new exemplary persons from the contemporary era become more important and broadly represented in different public carriers of culture. If this happens, status and Western moral values will be more widely represented in Hong Kong people’s value system. On one hand, the gradual incorporation of Western exemplary persons and their atten- dant cultural values into Hong Kong culture attests to culture’s malleability. On the other hand, the fact that Confucian moral values are constantly being reproduced in Hong Kong Chinese culture indicates that some aspects of culture are relatively stable (Triandis, 2004). This analysis raises several interesting questions for future research. First, what are the determinants of the likelihood that a particular aspect of traditional culture would be repro- duced and maintained in the culture? Could it be how important this aspect is for the sur- vival of the human species, as evolutionary psychologists have proposed (Kenrick, Li, & Butner, 2003)? Could it be how widely this aspect is shared and implicated in interpersonal communication in the population (Latané, 1996; Lau, Chiu, & Lee, 2001; Lau, Lee, & Chiu, 2004; Lyons & Kashima, 2001)? Could it be how extensively this aspect has been instituted in the various public carriers of cultural meanings (Kitayama, 2002)? Exemplary persons are public carriers of culture as well as vehicles of intergenerational transmission of cultural values. However, it is unclear how exemplary persons contribute to internalization of cultural values. One possible mechanism is through culture priming (Hong, Chiu, & Kung, 1997; Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martinez, 2000). Many exem- plary persons (e.g., Abraham Lincoln) are embodiments of core values in a culture (e.g., human rights in North America) as well as cultural icons. Every time an individual looks at these icons, the associated cultural values are activated. Children are often exposed to these cultural icons in their socialization and educational experiences. Frequent exposure to these cultural icons, which are often displayed extensively in the environment, ensures frequent activation and chronic accessibility of the cultural values. Through their experiences with the public culture, the human mind is socialized. Meanwhile, changes in the form and structure of social practices may alter the distribution and significance of certain exemplary persons in the culture (Menon & Morris, 2001). A future research challenge is to describe the reciprocal influence between public and private cultures. LIMITATIONS The present studies have several limitations. First, there was greater representation of female (versus male) participants in the studies. A future study with relatively balanced gender distribution is needed to assure the generality of the results. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 17. Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES 651 Second, the current studies focus on Hong Kong Chinese’s responses to globalization. Although the results of Inglehart and Baker’s (2000) cross-national survey reviewed in the introduction suggest that there are similar value negotiation processes in other countries, replications of our findings in other societies would help to establish the generality of the value negotiation processes. Last, the measures in our studies were written in Chinese. Previous studies have shown that language may activate its associated linguistic culture and affect responses to culture- relevant measures (Ross, Xun, & Wilson, 2002). The participants in our studies are Chinese–English bilinguals. It is important and interesting to examine whether their responses would differ when the studies are conducted in an English-language context. CONCLUSION In conclusion, the rapid rate of globalization has made reproduction of culture and cul- tural change a timely topic in social psychological investigation. It also offers a window of opportunity for researchers to uncover some dynamic cultural processes. The goal of the present article is to explore how local culture responds to globalization from the perspec- tive of the social identity theory and propose a new method that examines value exempli- fication of exemplary persons in Chinese and Western cultures. Our results offer some clues that may lead to the unveiling of some facets of the interactions between multiple cultures and the multicultural mind. NOTES 1. Although the word exemplary can mean serving as an example or serving as a warning, the research par- ticipants were instructed to list positive exemplary persons (see method sections in Studies 1 and 3). In this arti- cle, the referents of exemplary persons are restricted to positive exemplary persons only. 2. The ratio of exemplary persons generated only by men to those generated only by women was 1:5.64, which is similar to the ratio of men to women in the sample (1:4). 3. When participant gender was included as a factor in the analyses, no gender effects were significant. The pattern of value exemplification was similar for men and women: Chinese solidarity: Mmen = 4.73, SD = 0.60; Mwomen = 4.41, SD = 0.84; t(18) = 0.85, ns Chinese status: Mmen = 4.74, SD = 0.45; Mwomen = 4.78, SD = 0.42; t(18) = –0.16, ns Western solidarity: Mmen = 3.58, SD = 0.79; Mwomen = 4.06, SD = 0.53; t(18) = –1.15, ns Western status: Mmen = 4.85, SD = 0.31; Mwomen = 4.74, SD = 0.45; t(18) = 0.57, ns 4. The ratio of exemplary persons generated only by men to those generated only by women was 1:4.43, which is similar to the ratio of men to women in the sample (1:4.2). REFERENCES Arnett, J. J. (2002). The psychology of globalization. American Psychologist, 57, 774-783. Bond, M. H. (1985). Language as a carrier of ethnic stereotypes in Hong Kong. Journal of Social Psychology, 125, 53-62. Bond, M. H., & King, A. Y. C. (1985). Coping with the threat of Westernization in Hong Kong. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 9, 351-364. Callan, V. J., Gallois, C., & Forbes, P. A. (1983). Evaluative reactions to accented English: Ethnicity, sex role, and context. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 407-426. Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (2006). The social psychology of culture. New York: Psychology Press. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 18. 652 JOURNAL OF CROSS-CULTURAL PSYCHOLOGY Daniels, C. (2003, April 14). Mr. Coffee. Fortune, pp. 139-141. Falk, R. (1999). Predatory globalization. Cambridge, UK: Polity. Fischer, M. M. J. (1999). Emergent forms of life: Anthropologies of late or postmodernities. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 455-478. Giles, H., Henwood, K., Coupland, N., Harriman, J., & Coupland, J. (1992). Language attitudes and cognitive mediation. Human Communication Research, 18, 500-527. Ho, D. Y. F., Chau, A. W. L., Chiu, C.-Y., & Peng, S. Q. (2003). Ideological orientation and political transition in Hong Kong: Confidence in the future. Political Psychology, 24, 403-413. Hogg, M. A. (2004). Social identity, self-categorization, and communication in small groups. In S.-H. Ng, C. Candlin, & C.-Y. Chiu (Eds.), Language matters: Communication, culture, and identity (pp. 221-243). Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong Press. Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social identifications: A social psychology of intergroup relations and group processes. London: Routledge. Hogg, M. A., Joyce, N., & Abrams, D. (1984). Diglossia in Switzerland? A social identity analysis of speaker evaluations. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 3, 185-196. Hong, Y.-Y., Chiu, C. Y., & Kung, T. M. (1997). Bringing culture out in front: Effects of cultural meaning system activation on social cognition. In K. Leung, Y. Kashima, U. Kim, & S. Yamaguchi (Eds.), Progress in Asian social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 135-146). Singapore: Wiley. Hong, Y-. Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic con- structivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709-720. Inglehart, R., & Baker, W. E. (2000). Modernization, cultural change, and the persistence of traditional values. American Sociological Review, 65, 19-51. Kenrick, D. T., Li, M., & Butner, J. (2003). Dynamical evolutionary psychology: Individual decision rules and emergent social norms. Psychological Review, 110, 3-28. Kitayama, S. (2002). Culture and basic psychological processes—Toward a system view of culture: Comment on Oyserman et al. (2002). Psychological Bulletin, 128, 89-96. Krauss, R. M., & Chiu, C.- Y. (1998). Language and social psychology. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske-Emory, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 41-88). New York: Guilford. Lal, D. (2000). Does modernization require westernization? The Independent Review, 5, 5-24. Lam, S., Lau, I., Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Peng, S. (1999). Differential emphases on modernity and traditional val- ues in social categorization. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23, 237-256. Latané, B. (1996). Dynamic social impact: The creation of culture by communication. Journal of Communication, 46, 13-25. Lau, I. Y.-M., Chiu, C., & Lee, S.- L. (2001). Communication and shared reality: Implications for the psycho- logical foundations of culture. Social Cognition, 19, 350-371. Lau, I. Y.-M., Lee, S.-L., & Chiu, C. (2004). Language, cognition and reality: Constructing shared meanings through communication. In M. Schaller & C. Crandall (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture (pp. 77-100). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Lockwood, P., Marshall, T. C., & Sadler, P. (2005). Promoting success or preventing failure: Cultural differences in motivation by positive and negative role models. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 379-392. Lyczak, R., Fu, G. S., & Ho, A. (1976). Attitudes of Hong Kong bilinguals toward English and Chinese speak- ers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 7, 425-438. Lyons, A., & Kashima, Y. (2001). The reproduction of culture: Communication processes tend to maintain cul- tural stereotypes. Social Cognition, 19, 372-394. Menon, T., & Morris, M. W. (2001). Social structure in North American and Chinese cultures: Reciprocal influ- ence between objective and subjective structures. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 2, 27-50. Parameswaran, R. (2002). Local culture in global media: Excavating colonial and material discourses in National Geographic. Communication Theory, 12, 287-315. Pilkington, H., & Johnson, R. (2003). Relations of identity and power in global/local context. Cultural Studies, 6, 259-283. Robertson, R. (1992). Globalization: Social theory and global culture. London: Sage. Ross, M., Xun, W. Q. E., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). Language and the bicultural self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1040-1050. Ryan, E. B., & Giles, H. (Eds.). (1982). Attitudes toward language: Social and applied contexts. London: Arnold. Ryan, E. B., Giles, H., & Sebastian, R. J. (1982). An integrative perspective for the study of attitudes toward lan- guage variation. In E. B. Ryan & H. Giles (Eds.), Attitudes toward language variation: Social and applied context (pp. 1-19). London: Arnold. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011
  • 19. Fu, Chiu / GLOBALIZATION AND VALUES 653 Santana, K. (2003, August 12). MTV goes to Asia. Yale Global. Retrieved August 12, 2003, from http://yaleglobal .yale.edu/display.article?id=2211&page=1 Sebastian, R. J., & Ryan, E. B. (1985). Speech cues and social evaluations: Markers of ethnicity, social class and age. In H. Giles & R. N. S. Clair (Eds.), Recent advances in language, communication and social psychol- ogy (pp. 112-143). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Stewart, S. M., Bond, M. H., Deeds, O., & Chung, S. F. (1999). Intergenerational patterns of values and autonomy expectations in cultures of relatedness and separateness. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 575-593. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 94-109). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Tong, Y., Hong, Y., Lee, S., & Chiu, C. (1999). Language as a carrier of social identity. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23, 281-296. Triandis, H. C. (2004). Dimensions of culture beyond Hofstede. In H. Vinken, J. Soeters, & P. Ester (Eds.), Comparing cultures: Dimensions of culture in a comparative perspective (pp. 28-42). Leiden, Netherlands: Brill Publishers. Tzeng, O. C. S., & Henderson, M. M. (1999). Objective and subjective cultural relationships related to industrial modernization and social progress. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23, 411-445. Yeh, K.-H. (2002). Is living with elderly parents still a filial obligation for Chinese people? Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 3, 61-84. Yoon, K. (2003). Retraditionalizing the mobile: Young people’s sociality and mobile phone. Cultural Studies, 6, 327-343. Yue, X., & Ng, S. H. (1999). Filial obligations and expectations in China: Current views from young and old people in Beijing. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 2, 215-226. Zhang, J., & Shavitt, S. (2003). Cultural values in advertisements to the Chinese X-generation. Journal of Advertising, 32, 23-33. Jeanne Ho-Ying Fu is an assistant professor at Nanyang Business School, Nanyang Technological University, Singapore. She is interested in the dynamics of biculturals, social cognitive factors underly- ing cross-cultural conflict resolution and leadership, and social perception in teams. Chi-Yue Chiu is a professor of psychology at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. He received his PhD in social–personality psychology at Columbia University and taught at Hong Kong University before moving to Illinois. His current research focuses on cultures as knowledge traditions and the social, cognitive, and motivational processes that mediate the construction and evolution of social consensus. He is also interested in the dynamic interactions of cultural identification and cultural knowledge traditions and their implications for cultural competence and intercultural relations. Downloaded from jcc.sagepub.com at IACCP-International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology on March 16, 2011