SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 18
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
The Journal of Educational Research, 105:336–352, 2012
Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 0022-0671 print / 1940-0675 online
DOI:10.1080/00220671.2011.627401
Comparison of Self-Beliefs
for Predicting Student Motivation
and Achievement
MIMI BONG
CATHERINE CHO
HYUN SEON AHN
HYE JIN KIM
Korea University, Republic of Korea
ABSTRACT. The authors examined whether self-concept,
self-efficacy, and self-esteem show differential predictive util-
ity for academic achievement across age groups and domains.
More specifically, the relationships of 3 self-constructs with
achievement were examined in mathematics for elementary
school students and mathematics and language arts for mid-
dle school students in Korea. Task value and test anxiety
were hypothesized to mediate these relationships. Consistent
with previous reports, domain-specific self-constructs such
as self-efficacy and self-concept were better predictors of task
value and achievement than was general self-esteem. Task
value and test anxiety significantly mediated only the rela-
tionships of self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale
to achievement. These domain-specific relationships tended
to be stronger for middle school than elementary school stu-
dents and in mathematics than language arts.
Keywords: domain specificity, self-concept, self-efficacy, self-
esteem, task value, test anxiety
Educational theorists and researchers have long ex-
plored how perceived self-beliefs predict individ-
uals’ motivation and behavior by examining their
effects on goal setting, task choice, strategy use, and achieve-
ment in academic settings (e.g., Bachman & O’Malley, 1977;
Bandura & Schunk, 1981; Hansford & Hattie, 1982; Ludwig
& Maehr, 1967; Marsh, Byrne, & Shavelson, 1988). Ban-
dura’s social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1977, 1986; Pajares,
1996), for instance, characterizes the relationship between
perceived self-beliefs and personal behavior as reciprocal de-
terminism because individuals’ beliefs about themselves al-
low them to change their thoughts, emotions, and behaviors
and these in turn change their beliefs.
Among different types of self-constructs, self-concept,
self-efficacy, and self-esteem have perhaps been most
actively studied in the field of education and motivation
(Bandura, 1977; Bong & Skaalvik, 2003; Marsh, Walker,
& Debus, 1991). Despite quite a few attempts to arrive
at conceptual distinctions between these self-constructs
(e.g., Bandura, 1997; Bong & Skaalvik, 2003; Pajares,
1996), many researchers still struggle when trying to
separate them empirically (Shavelson & Bolus, 1982). This
difficulty results in mixed reports in the literature regarding
discriminability between these self-constructs and their
roles in academic settings. No consensus has been reached
regarding the relative predictive utility of self-concept
and self-efficacy on academic achievement. The relative
importance of different self-beliefs in affecting task value
and anxiety in achievement situations is also unknown.
We tried to address these questions directly in the present
investigation by comparing the utility of self-esteem, self-
concept, and self-efficacy beliefs for predicting student
achievement. On the basis of existing literature, we also
tested the hypothesis that task value and anxiety would me-
diate the relationship between self-constructs and achieve-
ment. By including samples from two different school levels
and assessing self-beliefs across two different subject matter
areas, we aimed to help clarify the issue of relative predictive
power of the three self-constructs for students’ achievement
and motivation in school.
Conceptual Definitions of Self-Constructs
Self-concept refers to an individual’s perceptions of the self
that are formed through experiences and evaluative feed-
back received from significant others (Shavelson, Hubner,
& Stanton, 1976). It concerns self-descriptions and self-
evaluations and hence is believed to consist of cognitive
and affective components (Covington, 1984). Stated dif-
ferently, self-concept represents an individual’s knowledge
about him- or herself along with emotional reactions toward
Address correspondence to Mimi Bong, Korea University, Department
of Education, Anam-dong, Seongbuk-gu, Seoul, 136–701 Republic of
Korea. (E-mail: mimibong@korea.ac.kr)
The Journal of Educational Research 337
the cognitively recognized competencies and attributes he
or she possesses (Bong & Clark, 1999). Previous research in-
dicates that affective responses are most likely when making
comparisons of an individual’s competencies and attributes
to certain standards and norms (Ruble, Parsons, & Ross,
1976). Therefore, self-concept formation is heavily influ-
enced by how individuals evaluate themselves in comparison
with others and how they emotionally react to the results of
these evaluations.
Self-efficacy refers to an individual’s subjective convic-
tions in his or her capability to organize and execute ac-
tions that are required to achieve a desired outcome in a
given context (Bandura, 1977). Academic self-efficacy more
specifically refers to the belief that an individual can suc-
cessfully accomplish given academic tasks at designated
levels (Schunk, 1991). Beliefs of self-efficacy are formed
through an individual’s own mastery experiences, vicarious
experiences, social persuasion, and physiological reactions
(Bandura, 1977, 1997). Whereas self-concept encompasses
cognitive and affective components, self-efficacy primarily
involves cognitive appraisals of one’s personal capabilities
(Bong & Skaalvik, 2003; Pajares, 1996). Pajares and Schunk
(2002) suggested that the two self-beliefs develop through
the processes of asking different questions. Self-efficacy de-
velops by asking questions about capability (e.g., “Can I write
a logical and persuasive piece on the causes of the Vietnam
War?”). In contrast, self-concept is formed by asking ques-
tions about feelings and being (e.g., “How do I feel about my-
self as a writer?”). Answering self-efficacy questions reveals
students’ confidence toward successfully performing a task,
whereas answering self-concept questions discloses their pos-
itive or negative views of themselves and their competencies.
Another marked distinction between self-concept and
self-efficacy is the type of self-appraisal employed. Prior expe-
rience, whether it is an individual’s own enactive experience
or vicarious experience, is essential in establishing percepts
of self-efficacy. Judgments of success or failure are made pri-
marily against mastery criteria but social comparative infor-
mation could be given more weight when the task is novel
or the criteria for mastery are ambiguous. Also, observing
others’ performance provides important information for esti-
mating an individual’s probability of success on similar tasks.
In such vicarious experiences, students develop efficacy be-
liefs by assessing the similarity of the model to themselves
and gauging the perceived difficulty of the task (Schunk,
1991). In this sense, self-efficacy can be viewed as reflect-
ing more or less a combination of results from criterion-
and norm-referenced evaluations, with heavier weight as-
signed to the former. In comparison, self-concept is more
concerned with an individual’s relative standing in relation
to others’ performances (Zimmerman, 1995). It is thus more
strongly influenced by social comparative information than
is self-efficacy (Bong & Clark, 1999).
Compared to self-efficacy and self-concept, self-esteem
has been rather loosely defined in the literature. It refers
to how individuals feel about themselves and evaluate
their global self-worth (Smelser, 1989). More precisely,
Rosenberg, Schooler, Schoenbach, and Rosenberg (1995)
defined self-esteem as “an individual’s positive or negative
attitude toward the self as a totality” (p. 141). On one hand,
self-esteem is generally considered as a far less context-
specific construct among the three. On the other hand,
self-esteem appears more relevant to an individual’s psycho-
logical well-being than self-concept or self-efficacy. A num-
ber of studies have reported that global self-esteem has shown
strong relationships to levels of anxiety (Rosenberg, 1962)
and psychological symptoms such as depression and juvenile
delinquency (Rosenberg, Schooler, & Schoenbach, 1989).
Relationships with Academic Achievement
Self-concept, self-efficacy, and self-esteem all have dis-
played discernible and independent predictive power for stu-
dents’ scholastic attainments in past research, although it is
difficult to locate studies that include all three constructs
in the same investigation. Self-concept has been a criti-
cal component in most theoretical accounts of academic
achievement, either as a presumed cause (e.g., Brookover,
Thomas, & Paterson, 1964; Marsh, Byrne, & Yeung, 1999)
or a byproduct of academic accomplishments (e.g., Calsyn
& Kenny, 1977; Skaalvik & Hagtvet, 1990). Likewise, self-
efficacy has repeatedly demonstrated itself as a significant
predictor of students’ academic performances. Its predictive
power is even stronger than that of more objective indexes
such as prior achievement levels (Zimmerman, Bandura, &
Martinez-Pons, 1992). Self-concept and self-efficacy have
also displayed significant relations with various indexes of
academic persistence (Hansford & Hattie, 1982; Multon,
Brown, & Lent, 1991).
Nonetheless, questions still remain as to which of the two
self-constructs better represents the very belief that energizes
students in learning and performance situations and hence
should be the target of enhancement efforts. Whereas stud-
ies by Pajares and Miller (1994) and Pietsch, Walker, and
Chapman (2003) demonstrated the stronger predictive util-
ity of self-efficacy compared with that of self-concept within
the same domain, Choi (2005) reported that academic self-
concept was a better predictor of academic performance than
self-efficacy among college students.
Regarding this issue, Bong and Clark (1999) and Bong
and Skaalvik (2003) speculated that self-efficacy would bet-
ter predict academic achievement than would self-concept.
They reasoned that self-efficacy more truthfully mirrors an
individual’s achievement history because past mastery ex-
periences are the most influential source of self-efficacy in-
formation. Moreover, competence judgments in self-efficacy
rely largely on the prospect of successful task mastery as an
absolute frame of reference. Self-efficacy also incorporates
specific features of the performance context, which might af-
fect the quality of performance (Bandura, 1997). Therefore,
self-efficacy should be able to afford an accurate prediction
of subsequent performance.
338 The Journal of Educational Research
In contrast, self-concept appears to be more heavily
swayed by various frames of reference and often does not
exactly correspond to an individual’s achievement history.
For example, students’ verbal and mathematics self-concepts
demonstrate a nonsignificant or near-zero correlation with
each other, even when their achievements in the two
domains show a strong positive correlation (Marsh et al.,
1988). Marsh (1986, 1990) explained this pattern to be a
result of simultaneous operation of internal and external
comparisons in his internal–external model of academic
self-concept. When judging their academic self-concept in
a given area, students compare their competence in that
area to that of their peers. This process is called an external
comparison, which leads to a positive correlation between
the verbal and mathematics self-concepts. At the same
time, students also compare their competence in a given
area to their competence in other areas. Most typically,
students compare their competence in verbal areas to their
competence in mathematics-related areas and vice versa.
This process is called an internal comparison, which yields
a negative correlation between the verbal and mathematics
self-concepts. Such concurrent utilization of the internal
and external frames of reference nullifies the result ob-
tained from each other, thereby making the correlation
between the verbal and mathematics self-concepts to be
nonsignificant and near zero.
Furthermore, academic self-concepts of equally able stu-
dents differ greatly, depending on the ability of their peers
in the reference group. The self-concept of a student attend-
ing a low-average school is higher than the self-concept of
a student attending a high-average school, even when these
two students are performing at the same level. This social
comparison effect is called the big-fish-little-pond-effect by
Marsh (1987, 1991). All these results point to the evalua-
tive nature of self-concept (Bong & Clark, 1999; Bong &
Skaalvik, 2003). Self-concept should thus be able to predict
motivational constructs with strong evaluative and affective
connotations better than self-efficacy.
Findings are even more inconsistent when it comes to
the predictive utility of self-esteem for students’ academic
achievement. Self-esteem is significantly associated with
achievement in some studies (Pullman & Allik, 2008) but
shows negligible correlations with achievement in others
(Byrne & Shavelson, 1986; Hansford & Hattie, 1982).
Rosenberg et al. (1995) concluded that self-esteem appears
to have little or no effect in enhancing academic perfor-
mance among high school students. Still, some have argued
that self-esteem plays a critical role in children’s academic
functioning (Pullman & Allik, 2008). According to these
researchers, an individual’s having positive regard toward
him- or herself is particularly beneficial for young children
because they have not yet developed stable perceptions of
competence across domains.
This claim contradicts the recent trend in self research
that mainly focuses on the role of domain-specific self-
constructs in academic situations. Especially for adolescents,
it is questionable whether nurturing global self-esteem would
yield tangible benefits in academic attainments above and
beyond what could be explained by a positive academic self-
concept or a strong sense of academic self-efficacy. It is in-
teresting to note, compared with the number of studies that
have examined the relative power of self-concept and self-
efficacy for predicting diverse achievement-related outcomes
(e.g., Pajares & Miller, 1994; Pietsch et al., 2003; Skaalvik
& Rankin, 1995), few studies have compared the predictive
utility of self-esteem to that of other self-constructs. Results
of the present research are expected to fill this void in the
literature.
Relationships with Task Value and Anxiety
In addition to specifying predictive relationships between
the three self-beliefs and academic achievement, we also an-
ticipated task value and anxiety to mediate those relation-
ships. The expectancy-value theory asserts that expectancies
for success and task value are important predictors of moti-
vation and achievement. Expectancy for success refers to the
subjective estimation of the probability that an individual’s
performance leads to successful consequences, and task value
refers to an incentive for engagement in particular tasks
or activities (Eccles, Wigfield, Harold, & Blumenfeld, 1993;
Wigfield et al., 1997). Many researchers operationally define
task value as comprising dimensions of interest (i.e., intrinsic
value), perceived importance (i.e., attainment value), and
perceived usefulness (i.e., utility value; Berndt & Miller,
1990; Bong, 2001a, 2001b; Pokay & Blumenfeld, 1990).
Task value is an important predictor of academic choices,
self-reported effort in class, and intention to persist on a
task (Hulleman, Durik, Schweigert, & Harackiewicz, 2008).
Confidence increases students’ interest in and task value for
an activity, which, in turn, enables students to spend more
time on the activity and perform better to meet higher stan-
dards (Eccles et al., 1993; Eccles & Wigfield, 1995). There-
fore, we hypothesized that task value would be an important
mediator linking self-perceived competence to achievement.
Previous research further suggests that emotions and affec-
tive responses are important correlates of various self-beliefs
(Bandura, 1989; Scherer, Schorr, & Johnstone, 2001).
According to Ruthig et al. (2008), distinct patterns of emo-
tional experiences are associated with high personal control.
As heightened senses of self-efficacy and positive self-
concept denote greater perceived control, this suggests that
certain academic emotions such as anxiety are associated
with students’ self-perceptions in learning contexts (Pekrun,
Elliot, & Maier, 2006). Anxiety represents anticipatory
fear accompanied by physiological arousal when ruminating
over possible adversities (Bandura, 1988). It is known to be
heavily influenced by how individuals conceive of specific
situational contexts in which arousal occurs (Hunt, Cole,
& Reis, 1958). Studies examining how self-perceptions
affect anxiety indicate that positive self-concept and strong
The Journal of Educational Research 339
self-efficacy help lower test anxiety (Bandalos, Yates, &
Thorndike-Christ, 1995; Zeidner & Schleyer, 1999).
There is evidence that certain affective factors might be
more strongly associated with self-concept than self-efficacy.
For example, Skaalvik and Rankin (1995) observed that stu-
dents’ anxiety showed stronger correlation with self-concept
than self-efficacy in mathematics and verbal domains. Pa-
jares and Miller (1994) also reported that students’ anxiety
levels were more strongly related to self-concept than self-
efficacy. These results make sense, considering self-concept
includes affective components and self-efficacy does not. On
the basis of these findings, we hypothesized that test anxi-
ety would negatively mediate the relationship between the
three self-constructs and achievement. We also predicted
that self-concept would demonstrate stronger relationships
with test anxiety compared with self-efficacy.
Domain and Age Differences
The self-constructs’ relationships with other variables may
also fluctuate depending on the domain characteristics and
age of students. Academic self-concept is known to be clearly
differentiated across mathematics and verbal areas, despite
the strong positive correlation usually observed between
mathematics and verbal achievements (Marsh et al., 1988).
The frequently nonsignificant and near-zero correlation be-
tween mathematics and verbal self-concepts contrasts with
significant positive correlation between mathematics and
verbal self-efficacy (Bong, 1997, 2001a). This hints at the
possibility that the degree of the self-constructs’ relative pre-
dictive power may differ across academic domains. In fact,
Bong (1998) reported that the relations of achievement in-
dexes to domain-specific self-concept and self-efficacy were
much stronger in mathematics-related subject areas than
verbal subject areas. Because students develop unique under-
standings of not only themselves but also academic domains
(Stodolski, Salk, & Glaessner, 1991), differential relations
may exist among their self-perceptions and their motivation
and achievement across different subject domains.
Age may be another factor that moderates the rela-
tionships of self-constructs with other constructs. It has
been well established in the developmental literature that
younger children evaluate themselves a lot more generously
than do older students, to a degree that is not justified by
their objective performance levels (e.g., Parsons & Ruble,
1977). This overestimation of competence by children is
attributed to their lack of critical thinking and analytic
reasoning ability and also their tendency to equate effort
with ability (Harter, 1999). As children age, they begin to
cognitively and more logically evaluate their achievement-
related experiences and information and discriminate
between their effort and ability. Hence, older students
build more negative but accurate views of their academic
capabilities (Eccles et al., 1993; Kun, Parsons, & Ruble,
1974; Parsons & Ruble, 1977). Reflecting these changes,
students’ ratings of their own academic competence reach
the highest point in the early elementary school years and
steadily decline afterward until early adolescence (Eccles,
Midgley, & Adler, 1984; Marsh, Barnes, Cairns, & Tidman,
1984; Stipek & Tannatt, 1984). Further, this decline is
most dramatic when students transition to middle school
(Simmons et al., 1979; Stipek & Mac Iver, 1989).
By including elementary and middle school students in the
present sample, we examined whether age or school-level
difference moderated the relationships of self-constructs
to other variables. Considering that older students’ self-
perceptions represent more realistic views of their own com-
petencies, we hypothesized that self-constructs in general
would demonstrate stronger predictive relationships with
academic outcomes among the middle school students. We
further hypothesized that this predictive gain would be
more visible with domain-specific self-constructs such as self-
concept and self-efficacy than global self-esteem.
Overview of the Present Hypotheses
In sum, we compared the utility of self-esteem, self-
concept, and self-efficacy for predicting academic achieve-
ments of elementary and middle school students. We also
tested whether task value and anxiety mediated the rela-
tionships between self-perceptions and achievement. In ad-
dition, domain- and age-related differences in predictive re-
lations among the constructs were examined. Specifically,
we generated and tested the following eight hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1 (H1): Domain-specific self-efficacy and domain-
specific self-concept would predict academic achievement
better than general self-esteem.
H2: Between self-concept and self-efficacy, self-efficacy
would predict academic achievement better than self-
concept.
H3: Between self-concept and self-efficacy, self-concept
would predict task value better than self-efficacy.
H4: Between self-concept and self-efficacy, self-concept
would predict anxiety better than self-efficacy.
H5: Task value would mediate the relationships between
self-constructs and academic achievement.
H6: Anxiety would mediate the relationships between self-
constructs and academic achievement.
H7: The relationships between self-constructs and academic
achievement would be stronger among middle school stu-
dents than elementary school students.
H8: The relationships between self-constructs and academic
achievement would be different across mathematics and
language arts.
Figure 1 shows the structure of our theoretical model. Be-
cause the elementary school participants responded only to
the mathematics surveys, Hypothesis 8 could not be tested
among the elementary school students. Therefore, we de-
cided to report our results in two studies to minimize po-
tential confusion due to the difference in the types of data
collected. In Study 1, we presented results regarding the
340 The Journal of Educational Research
FIGURE 1. Structure of the hypothesized theoretical
model.
relative predictive utility of self-constructs (i.e., Hypothe-
ses 1–4), the mediating role of task value and anxiety in
self–achievement relationships (i.e., Hypotheses 5 and 6),
and age-related differences in construct relations (Hypoth-
esis 7). In Study 2, we reported results regarding domain
differences in construct relations across mathematics and
language arts (i.e., Hypothesis 8) with only the middle school
participants.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Participants were 234 elementary and 512 middle school
students. Korean elementary schools offer 6 years of school-
ing before students enter middle schools. Therefore, fifth-
and sixth-grade students are regarded as seniors in elemen-
tary schools. Middle and high schools each offer 3 years of
schooling. All participants were attending public schools
located in Seoul and its vicinity in Korea. The regions in
which the schools were located were considered largely mid-
dle class in terms of socioeconomic status. The students
participated in a larger research project on Korean students’
motivation and learning. There is another report based on
the data from the same research project (Bong, 2009), but
that article deals with different constructs (i.e., achievement
goal orientations) and addresses a completely different set
of research questions. Only the data relevant to the present
research were analyzed in this study.
Surveys were administered during regular classroom hours
in the middle of the semester, a little less than 2 months
into the academic year. Students were told that participation
was voluntary and no one outside the research team would
have access to their individual responses. Elementary school
students responded only to the surveys tapping into their
motivation and learning in mathematics, whereas middle
school students responded to the surveys for mathematics
and language arts. Elementary school teachers’ ratings of
their students’ mathematics competence were obtained with
the survey data. Middle school students’ final examination
scores in mathematics were obtained from the schools at the
end of the semester.
Seven students were excluded from the elementary
school sample before analysis because they either did not
have the achievement data or failed to answer a substantial
portion of the survey. The final elementary school sample
included 110 fifth-grade students and 117 sixth-grade
students (125 boys, 102 girls). Their ages ranged between 10
and 12 years with an average age of 11.3 years (SD = .68).
Five students were excluded from the middle school sample
before analysis for the same reasons. The final breakdown of
the middle school sample was 137 seventh-grade students,
244 eighth-grade students, and 126 ninth-grade students.
The ages ranged between 12 and 15 years with an average
age of 13.8 years (SD = .78).
Measures
Students were asked to rate each item on a 5-point Likert-
type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly
agree). Except for self-esteem, all variables were assessed in
reference to the domains of mathematics and language arts.
Self-esteem. General self-esteem was measured by eight
items from the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire
(ASDQ; e.g., “I have a lot to be proud of ”; Marsh, 1992).
Reliability of the scale was acceptable in the present study
with Cronbach’s alpha values of .84 and .81 for the elemen-
tary and middle school samples, respectively.
Self-concept. Six items from the ASDQ (Marsh, 1992)
were adopted (e.g., “Compared to others my age I am good
at math”). Reliability coefficients (Cronbach’s α) were .88
in mathematics for the elementary school students, and .87
and .75 in mathematics and language arts, respectively, for
the middle school students.
Self-efficacy. We assessed self-efficacy in two different
ways because of their prevalent use in the literature. How
self-efficacy is assessed is a critical issue in its own right
(Pajares, 1996), yet empirical comparability of the scores
obtained by different assessment methods has rarely been
tested (Bong & Hocevar, 2002). One scale included four
items adapted from Bandura’s (1986) Self-Efficacy scale (e.g.,
“I can get more than ‘80’ in the math exam”) and the
other consisted of five items from the Motivated Strategies
for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ; e.g., “I’m confident I
can do an excellent job on the assignments and tests in
this course”; Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1993).
For the Bandura-type Self-Efficacy Scale, Cronbach’s alphas
were .91 in mathematics for the elementary school students,
and .91 and .86 in mathematics and language arts, respec-
tively, for the middle school students. For the MSLQ, Cron-
bach’s alphas were .91 in mathematics for the elementary
school students, and .88 and .86 in mathematics and lan-
guage arts, respectively, for the middle school students.
Task value. Task value consisted of three items, each ask-
ing about students’ perceptions of importance, utility, and
The Journal of Educational Research 341
interest in the subject matter area (Bong, 2001a, 2001b). A
sample item read, “I think math is a useful subject.” Relia-
bility coefficients were a bit low with Cronbach’s alphas of
.52 in mathematics for the elementary school sample, and
.64 and .60 in mathematics and language arts, respectively,
for the middle school sample.
Test anxiety. Three items were adapted from the MSLQ
(e.g. “I worry a great deal about math tests”; Pintrich & De
Groot, 1990). The reliability coefficients (Cronbach’s α)
for test anxiety were .57 in mathematics for the elementary
school sample, and .63 and .61 in mathematics and language
arts, respectively, for the middle school sample.
Achievement indices. For the elementary school partici-
pants, their homeroom teachers rated each student’s level
of mathematics performance on a 3-point Likert-type scale
ranging from 1 (below average) to 3 (above average). We
obtained indices of the elementary school students’ mathe-
matics achievements this way because there was no official
school-wide mathematics examination and the national ed-
ucation policy at the time of the survey dictated that grades
not be assigned for elementary school students. For the mid-
dle school participants, their scores on the first-semester final
examinations in the subjects of mathematics and Korean,
ranging between 0 and 100, were used as achievement in-
dices. To prevent estimation problems associated with such
a large discrepancy in the measurement units between the
middle school achievement scores (i.e., 0–100) and other
variables (i.e., 1–3 or 1–5), we transformed the middle school
students’ achievement data into z scores before analysis.
Results
Study 1: Mediators and Age Differences
Table 1 shows descriptive statistics among the variables
for the elementary and middle school participants. Several
findings are noteworthy. The middle school students’ self-
concept scores on the ASDQ and self-efficacy scores on the
MSLQ (Ms = 3.06 and 3.36, SDs = .90 and .93) were signif-
icantly lower than those of the elementary school students
(Ms = 3.30 and 3.57, SDs = .88 and .88), ts(732) = 3.32
and 2.84, ps < .01. The general self-esteem scores were also
significantly lower among the middle school students (M =
3.19, SD = .81) than the elementary school students (M =
3.39, SD = .84), t(732) = 2.96, p < .01. Test anxiety showed
the opposite trend such that the middle school students ex-
pressed significantly higher levels of test anxiety (M = 3.04,
SD = .98) compared with those of the elementary school
students (M = 2.85, SD = .93), t(732) = −2.40, p < .05.
On the contrary, the middle school students’ self-efficacy
scores on the Bandura-type scale (M = 3.59, SD = 1.10)
did not differ from those of the elementary school coun-
terparts (M = 3.51, SD = 1.09), t(731) = −0.88, p = ns.
Moreover, the elementary and middle school students’ self-
efficacy scores on the Bandura-type scale were associated
with larger standard deviations (SDs = 1.09 and 1.10 for the
elementary and middle school sample, respectively) com-
pared with the standard deviations of the other scales.
Tests of measurement models. Because covariance struc-
ture modeling requires complete data on study variables, all
missing values were replaced with the expected maximiza-
tion (EM) method before analysis (Schreiber, Stage, King,
Nora, & Barlow, 2006). The frequencies of missing values
ranged from 1 (0.2%) to a maximum of 12 cases (2.4%) per
variable, which is judged acceptable.
Before fitting our theoretical model to the empirical data,
we tested the measurement models first by performing confir-
matory factor analyses (CFAs) to check the factor structure.
We used AMOS 7.0 for all our CFA and structural equa-
tion modeling (SEM; Arbuckle, 2006). Because chi-square
statistics are known to be sensitive to sample size (Kline,
2005, p. 146), we applied the root mean square error of ap-
proximation (RMSEA) ≤ .06 (Hu & Bentler, 1999) and the
TABLE 1. Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Coefficients Among Latent Variables for Elementary and Middle School
Students in Mathematics
Elementary Middle
Variable M SD M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. ASDQ 3.30a 0.88 3.06b 0.90 — .91 .84 .39 .74 −.57 .67
2. MSLQ 3.57a 0.88 3.36b 0.93 .96 — .78 .44 .81 −.45 .60
3. BDR 3.51 1.09 3.59 1.10 .78 .76 — .36 .62 −.45 .57
4. GSE 3.39a 0.84 3.19b 0.81 .53 .56 .43 — .22 −.47 .26
5. Task value 3.72 0.80 3.60 0.89 .77 .81 .56 .36 — –.35 .45
6. Test anxiety 2.85a 0.93 3.04b 0.98 −.67 −.63 −.59 −.64 −.49 — −.41
7. Achievement 2.24 0.80 70.42 22.48 .61 .53 .56 .32 .45 −.51 —
Note. Correlation coefficients from elementary school in mathematics are below the diagonal; those from middle school in mathematics are above
the diagonal. All coefficients are significant at p < .01. Different subscripts denote statistically significant difference at p < .05. ASDQ = self-concept
assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire;
BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem.
342 The Journal of Educational Research
Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) and comparative fit index (CFI)
≥ .90 (Bentler, 1990; Tucker & Lewis, 1973) as the criteria
for evaluating model fit.
The CFA models included general self-esteem, math-
ematics self-concept, MSLQ mathematics self-efficacy,
Bandura-type mathematics self-efficacy, mathematics task
value, mathematics test anxiety, and mathematics achieve-
ment. The model exhibited acceptable goodness-of-fit
indices for the elementary, χ2
(355, N = 227) = 678.602,
p < .001, CFI = .916, TLI = .904, RMSEA = .064; and
middle school samples, χ2
(355, N = 507) = 1,027.119, p <
.001, CFI = .916, TLI = .904, RMSEA = .061. All factor
loadings were statistically significant at p < .001.
As can be seen in Table 1, there existed differences in
variable correlation between the two age groups. When we
examined the correlation coefficients among the four self-
constructs, not much difference was observed in how the
domain-specific self-constructs such as self-concept and self-
efficacy correlated with each other across the two groups.
However, general self-esteem, which was assessed globally,
showed substantial drop in its correlation with other self-
constructs in the middle school sample compared with the
elementary school sample. The correlation coefficients of
the general self-esteem factor reduced from phi values of .53
to .39 with self-concept, .56 to .44 with MSLQ self-efficacy,
and .43 to .36 with Bandura-type self-efficacy for the middle
school students, respectively.
The correlation coefficients between general self-esteem
and other variables also decreased considerably (φs = .36 vs.
.22 with task value, −.64 vs. −.47 with test anxiety, and
.32 vs. .26 with achievement in the elementary and mid-
dle school samples, respectively). This was not the case
for the domain-specific self-constructs such as self-concept
and self-efficacy, as they demonstrated correlation coeffi-
cients of similar magnitude across the elementary and mid-
dle school samples with other variables, with the exception
of test anxiety. The correlation coefficients between test
anxiety and self-constructs uniformly dropped from the ele-
mentary to the middle school sample with self-concept (φs
= −.67 vs. −.57), MSLQ self-efficacy (φs = −.63 vs. −.45),
and Bandura-type self-efficacy (φs = −.59 vs. −.45).
Some of the self-constructs were too highly correlated
to ensure their discriminant validity. In particular, the self-
concept and the MSLQ self-efficacy factors correlated too
highly to each other in both samples (φs = .96 and .91 in
the elementary and middle school samples, respectively).
This was surprising because these correlation coefficients
were even stronger than those between the two self-efficacy
factors assessed by different methods, the MSLQ and the
Bandura-type scale (φs = .76 in the elementary and .78 in
the middle school sample). Therefore, to examine the em-
pirical structure of our self-constructs, we conducted CFAs
on two separate models. The first model was composed of
four factors including MSLQ self-efficacy, Bandura-type self-
efficacy, self-concept, and general self-esteem. In the second
model, we merged the two self-efficacy factors together to
have self-efficacy, self-concept, and general self-esteem.
In the elementary school sample, the first model revealed
model fit of TLI = .924, CFI = .933, and RMSEA = .066,
whereas the second model showed lower fit of TLI = .867,
CFI = .882, and RMSEA = .087. Results from the middle
school sample likewise revealed that in mathematics, the first
model displayed a satisfactory model fit of TLI = .922, CFI
= .931, and RMSEA = .064, whereas the second model dis-
played weaker fit of TLI = .874, CFI = .888, and RMSEA =
.081. In language arts, the first model displayed fit of TLI =
.900, CFI = .912, and RMSEA = .064, whereas the second
model showed again decreased fit of TLI = .860, CFI = .876,
and RMSEA = .076. Our CFA results, along with high cor-
relations especially among the MSLQ self-efficacy, Bandura-
type self-efficacy, and self-concept factors, suggest multi-
collinearity among the self-constructs. We thus examined
each self-construct separately in all subsequent analyses to
avoid empirical problems associated with multicollinearity.
SEM. The relative predictive utility of the self-constructs
for achievement (Hypotheses 1 and 2), task value (Hypoth-
esis 3), and anxiety (Hypothesis 4) were compared by SEM.
The role of task value (Hypothesis 5) and anxiety (Hypoth-
esis 6) as mediators of the self–achievement relationship
was also examined. Figure 1 shows the theoretical model we
specified for testing our hypotheses.
All models displayed satisfactory to marginally acceptable
fit to the empirical data analyzed with AMOS 7.0 (Arbuckle,
2006), as reported in Table 2 (i.e., “original” models). Al-
though there was some room for improvement for the general
self-esteem models, we did not add any additional path to
improve the overall fit because doing so might obstruct direct
and more accurate comparison of the results. Figure 2 shows
the results of structural equation modeling with the path
coefficients separately estimated for each self-construct. All
self-constructs except for general self-esteem and MSLQ self-
efficacy for the elementary school sample proved to be signif-
icant positive predictors of students’ achievements in math-
ematics. General self-esteem was not a significant predictor
of middle school students’ mathematics achievements ei-
ther, providing support for Hypothesis 1 that domain-specific
self-efficacy and domain-specific self-concept would predict
academic achievement better than general self-esteem.
For the elementary school sample, self-concept demon-
strated the strongest path to mathematics achievement with
γ = .52, followed by Bandura-type self-efficacy with γ =
.33. MSLQ self-efficacy and general self-esteem were not
able to predict elementary school students’ mathematics
achievements. For the middle school sample, self-concept
again exhibited the strongest path with γ = .69, followed by
MSLQ self-efficacy (γ = .61) and Bandura-type self-efficacy
(γ = .40). Hypothesis 2, self-efficacy would predict aca-
demic achievement better than self-concept, was thus not
supported.
The Journal of Educational Research 343
TABLE 2. Goodness-of-Fit Indices for Elementary and Middle School Models in Study 1
Model CFI TLI RMSEA χ2
(df) χ2
( df)
ASDQ
Elementary
Original .953 .938 .068 101.981(50) 0.026(1)
Alternative .952 .936 .069 101.955(49)
Middle
Original .944 .927 .073 185.988(50) 0.979(1)
Alternative .944 .925 .074 185.009(49)
MSLQ
Elementary
Original .940 .917 .078 95.506(40) 0.014(1)
Alternative .939 .913 .080 95.492(39)
Middle
Original .954 .936 .072 143.927(40) 25.379(0)
Alternative .942 .921 .080 169.306(40)
BDR
Elementary
Original .962 .943 .077 70.149(30) 5.778(0)
Alternative .957 .935 .082 75.927(30)
Middle
Original .965 .948 .072 109.248(30) 38.526(0)
Alternative .948 .922 .088 147.774(30)
GSE
Elementary
Original .907 .883 .073 158.679(72) 10.447(0)
Alternative .919 .897 .068 148.232(72)
Middle
Original .877 .844 .080 306.429(72) 18.426(0)
Alternative .887 .857 .077 288.003(72)
Note. Ns = 227 (elementary students) and 507 (middle school students). CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root
mean square error of approximation; ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed
by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem.
All domain-specific self-constructs as well as general self-
esteem related significantly to task value and test anxiety
in mathematics for the elementary and middle school sam-
ples. As hypothesized, self-constructs displayed positive re-
lationships with task value and negative relationships with
test anxiety. MSLQ self-efficacy demonstrated the strongest
predictive paths to task value in both age groups, followed
by self-concept, Bandura-type self-efficacy, and general self-
esteem. The relationships of self-concept and MSLQ self-
efficacy with task value were so strong as to suspect multi-
collinearity among the constructs. For example, the absence
of significant paths from task value to achievement in these
models likely has been due to the strong correlation between
the self-construct and task value factors. We thus decided to
exclude task value from Study 2.
Whereas the paths from MSLQ self-efficacy, self-concept,
and general self-esteem to anxiety were of similar magnitude
in the elementary school sample with gamma values of −.62,
−.66, and −.66, respectively, the path from self-concept to
anxiety was noticeably stronger in the middle school sam-
ple with a gamma value of −.57. Therefore, Hypothesis 3,
self-concept would predict task value better than self-
efficacy, and Hypothesis 4, self-concept would predict anxi-
ety better than self-efficacy, received only partial support.
The hypothesized mediating roles of task value (Hypoth-
esis 5) and test anxiety (Hypothesis 6) were supported with
respect to general self-esteem and self-efficacy assessed by the
Bandura-type scale for the middle school sample. Whereas
Bandura-type mathematics self-efficacy showed direct pre-
dictive paths and indirect paths via task value and test anx-
iety on middle school students’ mathematics achievement,
general self-esteem only demonstrated indirect predictive
power on achievement through task value and test anxiety
in both age groups. Therefore, the theoretical model with
hypothesized mediators received clear support only with re-
spect to Bandura-type self-efficacy in the middle school sam-
ple. Whereas task value significantly mediated the predic-
tive relation of Bandura-type self-efficacy to mathematics
achievement for the middle school students, it failed to do
so for the elementary school students.
344 The Journal of Educational Research
FIGURE 2. Standardized regression weights for the elementary and middle school models in Study 1. Coefficients before the
slash are from elementary school; those after the slash are from middle school. ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic
Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR
= self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem. None of the path coefficients are significantly
different between the elementary and middle school samples. ∗
p < .05. ∗∗
p < .01. ∗∗∗
p < .001.
Tests of invariance across ages. In general, the relation-
ships of self-constructs to task value and anxiety were either
similar or slightly reduced in magnitude from the elemen-
tary to the middle school sample (see Figure 2). In particular,
all self-constructs demonstrated much stronger relationships
with anxiety for the elementary school students than the
middle school students regardless of the type and the degree
of domain specificity. The difference was relatively greater
with regard to MSLQ self-efficacy (γ s = −.62 vs. −.45) and
general self-esteem (γ s = −.66 vs. −.49 for the elementary
and middle school samples, respectively). The predictive
paths to achievements, in contrast, became stronger in the
middle school sample with respect to all domain-specific
self-constructs. All domain-specific self-constructs displayed
stronger relationships with middle school students’ achieve-
ments than elementary school students’ achievements. The
difference was more pronounced with MSLQ self-efficacy
(γ s = .27 [p = ns] vs. .61) compared with self-concept (γ s =
.52 vs. .69) or Bandura-type self-efficacy (γ s = .33 vs. .40, for
the elementary and the middle school samples, respectively).
To more systematically address age differences in con-
struct relations, we performed multigroup analyses by im-
posing cross-group equality (i.e., invariance) constraints on
the parameters between the two samples. Testing the struc-
tural invariance across groups should be performed in a
two-step approach. First, when testing the metric invari-
ance (i.e., equality of the measurement aspect of the model
across groups), it should be tested that all paths in the model
are invariant except for the structural path (MacCallum,
Roznowski, & Reith, 1994). If the model fit in this stage is
acceptable, the model satisfies cross-validation. The second
stage, testing structural invariance across groups, is ensur-
ing measurement and structural paths to be invariant across
groups by testing the chi-square difference (Byrne, 2001).
Whether there exist significant chi-square changes between
the unconstrained (i.e., baseline) model and constrained
model determines whether there is a group difference in the
structural paths. That is, if there is no significant chi-square
difference, the constrained model should be preferred be-
cause it is more parsimonious.
Results showed that the model fit for the constrained
model showed only minimal changes from the model fit
of the unconstrained model, revealing no significant differ-
ences in chi-square statistics (see Table 3) and indicating
that the fit of the constrained model is superior to that of
the unconstrained model. This suggests that the parame-
ters of both samples within the population are equal (Kline,
2005, p. 290). In other words, the predictive power of latent
The Journal of Educational Research 345
TABLE 3. Goodness-of-Fit Indices from Multigroup Analyses in Study 1
Model CFI TLI RMSEA χ2
(df) χ2
( df)
ASDQ
Unconstrained .945 .933 .050 303.158(108) 3.339(5)
Constrained .946 .937 .048 306.497(113)
MSLQ
Unconstrained .948 .934 .051 249.845(86) 5.924(5)
Constrained .948 .937 .050 255.769(91)
BDR
Unconstrained .960 .945 .053 199.438(66) 2.466(5)
Constrained .961 .950 .050 201.904(71)
GSE
Unconstrained .880 .859 .055 493.423(154) 4.274(5)
Constrained .881 .863 .054 497.697(159)
Note. Ns = 227 (elementary students) and 507 (middle school students). CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root
mean square error of approximation; ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed
by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem.
factors for middle school and elementary school students
did not display significant differences. Therefore, Hypothe-
sis 7, the relationships between self-constructs and academic
achievement would be stronger among the middle school
students than the elementary school students, did not re-
ceive support.
Tests of alternative models. To determine whether the hy-
pothesized model is the most reasonable representation of
the empirical data (Bentler, 1990; Schreiber et al., 2006),
we compared the hypothesized model with an alternative
model by including an additional path linking task value
with test anxiety (see Figure 3). Whereas the original model
presumed task value and anxiety to be mediators of the
self–achievement relationships, the alternative model spec-
ified anxiety to be a mediator in the relationships of both
self-constructs and task value to achievement. This alter-
native model was based on the expectancy-value theory’s
claim that expectancies for success, in the form of positive
self-perceptions in this study, and task value are important
FIGURE 3. Structure of the alternative model.
predictors of motivation and achievement (Eccles et al.,
1993; Eccles & Wigfield, 1995).
Statistical differences in chi squares between models
demonstrate that our original model explains data better
than the alternative models, with general self-esteem models
as possible exceptions (see Table 2). Overall, the majority
of the alternative models revealed that the added path
from task value to test anxiety was statistically insignificant
across grade levels. Because our main interest lied in how
the different self-constructs project different patterns on
achievement mediated by task value and anxiety and the
results attested to empirical superiority of the original model,
we decided to keep self-constructs as antecedents in each
model.
Study 2: Domain Differences
Next, we analyzed the middle school data to examine
if the predictive utility of self-constructs would differ,
depending on the subject matter domain. Table 4 reports
descriptive statistics and the latent variable correlations
for the middle school participants in mathematics and
language arts. Paired-samples t tests revealed statistically
significant differences between the domains in self-concept,
t(505) = −3.44, p < .001; test anxiety, t(505) = 3.48,
p < .001; and achievement, t(506) = −2.82, p < .005.
The middle school participants in this study expressed
significantly lower levels of self-concept (M = 3.06, SD =
.90 vs. M = 3.21, SD = .70) and achievement (M =
70.42, SD = 22.48 vs. M = 72.49, SD = 15.22) and
significantly higher levels of test anxiety in mathematics
(M = 3.04, SD = .98 vs. M = 2.86, SD = .88) compared
with those in language arts. There was no other statistically
significant difference in mean scores across the two domains.
346 The Journal of Educational Research
TABLE 4. Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Coefficients Among Latent Variables for Middle School Students in
Mathematics and Language Arts
Mathematics Language arts
Variable M SD M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. ASDQ 3.06a 0.90 3.21b 0.70 — .91 .84 .39 .74 −.57 .67
2. MSLQ 3.36 0.93 3.44 0.83 .97 — .78 .44 .81 −.45 .60
3. BDR 3.59 1.10 3.61 0.98 .80 .77 — .36 .62 −.45 .57
4. GSE 3.19 0.81 (domain-general) .28 .30 .32 — .22 −.47 .26
5. Task value 3.60 0.89 3.61 0.82 .79 .87 .54 .22 — −.35 .45
6. Test anxiety 3.04a 0.98 2.86b 0.88 −.41 −.24 −.31 −.25 −.20 — −.41
7. Achievement 70.42a 22.48 72.49b 15.22 .37 .28 .50 .23 .11 −.17 —
Note. Correlation coefficients from language arts are below the diagonal; those from mathematics are above the diagonal. Coefficients greater than
.11 and .20 in absolute value are significant at p < .05 and p < .01, respectively. Different subscripts denote a statistically significant difference at p <
.05. ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for
Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem.
Tests of measurement models. As was the case with the
mathematics data in Study 1, the middle school data also
yielded strong correlation coefficients between the self-
concept and the MSLQ self-efficacy factors in the math-
ematics (φ = .91) and language arts domains (φ = .97).
Also consistent with the findings in Study 1, these correla-
tion coefficients were much stronger than those between the
two self-efficacy scores, each assessed by the MSLQ and the
Bandura-type scale (φs = .78 and .77 in mathematics and
language arts, respectively). The strong correlations of self-
concept and MSLQ self-efficacy with task value observed in
mathematics in Study 1 were also obtained in the domain
of language arts (φs = .79 with self-concept and .87 with
MSLQ self-efficacy). Due to these high correlations, we ex-
cluded the task value factor from our model in Study 2 to
avoid multicollinearity problems. The measurement models
with all relevant variables in mathematics, χ2
(355, N = 507)
= 1,027.119, p < .001, CFI = .916, TLI = .904, RMSEA =
.061; and language arts, χ2
(355, N = 507) = 1,108.472, p <
.001, CFI = .883; TLI = .866, RMSEA = .065, both yielded
adequate fit to the data. All factor loadings were statistically
significant at p < .001.
The correlation coefficients of self-constructs with other
variables were, overall, noticeably stronger in the domain of
mathematics than language arts. This trend was particularly
conspicuous in their relationships with achievements. With
the exception of general self-esteem, which showed similarly
weak relationships with achievements in mathematics and
language arts (φs = .26 vs. .23, respectively), self-concept (φs
= .67 vs. .37), MSLQ self-efficacy (φs = .60 vs. .28), and
Bandura-type self-efficacy (φs = .57 vs. .50) all displayed
stronger correlations with achievement in the domain of
mathematics.
Differences in the magnitude of construct correlations
across domains were also evident in the self-constructs’ re-
lationships with test anxiety. The self-concept, MSLQ self-
efficacy, Bandura-type self-efficacy, and general self-esteem
factors demonstrated correlation coefficients that ranged be-
tween −.57 ≤ φ ≤ −.45 with test anxiety in mathematics.
The corresponding correlations obtained from the language
arts domain ranged between −.41 ≤ φ ≤ −.24. General
self-esteem also exhibited much stronger correlation with
self-concept (φs = .39 vs. 28) and MSLQ self-efficacy (φs =
.44 vs. .30) in mathematics than language arts. The correla-
tion between task value and achievement (φ = .45 vs. .11),
test anxiety and task value (φs = −.35 vs. −.20), and test
anxiety and achievement (φs = −.41 vs. −.17 in mathe-
matics and language arts, respectively) were all considerably
stronger in mathematics than language arts.
SEM. Table 5 (i.e., “independent” models) presents the
goodness-of-fit indices of structural models, separately esti-
mated for each self-construct in mathematics and language
arts. All models exhibited acceptable fit indices, whereas
the self-concept model in language arts and the general self-
esteem models in mathematics and language arts demon-
strated only marginally acceptable fit to the empirical data.
As was the case in Study 1, we did not add any path for the
purpose of improving the model fit as to allow more accurate
comparison of the results. Figure 4 presents statistically sig-
nificant paths at p < .05 and the standardized coefficients.
All self-constructs directly predicted achievement with
varying power, with the exception of general self-esteem
in mathematics. Further, all domain-specific self-constructs,
with the exception of Bandura-type self-efficacy, displayed
conspicuously stronger relationships to achievement in the
domain of mathematics than language arts (γ s = .64 vs.
.37 for self-concept and .52 vs. .25 for MSLQ self-efficacy
in mathematics vs. language arts, respectively). The utility
of the self-efficacy beliefs assessed by the Bandura-type scale
for predicting students’ achievements did not differ across
the two domains with γ s = .47 and .49 in mathematics
The Journal of Educational Research 347
TABLE 5. Goodness-of-Fit Indices for Mathematics and Language Arts Models in Study 2
Model CFI TLI RMSEA χ2
(df) χ2
( df)
ASDQ
Independent
mathematics .969 .957 .062 96.199(33)
Language arts .876 .831 .094 179.463(33)
Invariance testing
Unconstrained .914 .894 .061 346.843(73) 33.923(3)∗∗∗
Constrained .905 .887 .063 380.766(76)
MSLQ
Independent
mathematics .952 .931 .082 109.961(25)
Language arts .969 .956 .057 66.115(25)
Invariance testing
Unconstrained .958 .946 .048 186.255(56) 56.100(3)
∗∗∗
Constrained .941 .928 .055 242.355(59)
BDR
Independent
mathematics .984 .973 .061 48.575(17)
Language arts .951 .920 .086 81.272(17)
Invariance testing
Unconstrained .971 .958 .049 132.611(39) 19.378(3)
∗∗∗
Constrained .966 .955 .051 151.989(42)
GSE
Independent
mathematics .889 .857 .084 232.384(51)
Language arts .889 .857 .081 221.177(51)
Invariance testing
Unconstrained .892 .871 .055 454.676(111) 21.984(3)
∗∗∗
Constrained .886 .868 .056 476.660(114)
Note. N = 507. CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; ASDQ = self-concept
assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire;
BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem.
∗p < .05. ∗∗p < .01. ∗∗∗p < .001.
and language arts, respectively. Test anxiety negatively me-
diated the relationships of general self-esteem and MSLQ
self-efficacy to achievements in both domains and of
Bandura-type self-efficacy to achievement in the domain
of mathematics but not language arts.
Tests of invariance across domains. In principle, multi-
group analysis is performed in order to test measurement
or structural invariance across groups composed of indepen-
dent samples. If multigroup analysis is conducted using a
single sample, there is difficulty interpreting the results be-
cause comparing covariance and error covariance becomes
implausible. Nonetheless, multigroup analysis may be per-
formed when there are numerous parameters in the model
and testing all possible interaction effects increases the oc-
currence of errors. For these reasons, we conducted multi-
group analyses with the mathematics and language arts data.
As done in Study 1, we again imposed cross-group equal-
ity constraints to assess whether there were significant differ-
ences in the individual parameters between mathematics and
language arts. According to the chi-square difference statis-
tic, the fit of the constrained model was significantly worse
than that of the unconstrained model (see Table 5), sug-
gesting that the parameters in the population of the two do-
mains were not equal (Kline, 2005, p. 290). Hence, we con-
cluded that there were significant differences in path coeffi-
cients in mathematics and language arts. As Figure 4 shows,
stronger paths were projected from MSLQ self-efficacy and
self-concept to achievement in mathematics compared with
language arts. Similarly, mathematics revealed stronger pre-
dictions from MSLQ self-efficacy and general self-esteem to
test anxiety, and test anxiety to achievement in the Bandura-
type self-efficacy and general self-esteem models.
Discussion
Self-Constructs as Predictors of Student Achievement
The present study confirmed once again that domain
specificity is an important attribute of self-constructs to be
able to function as significant predictors of tangible academic
348 The Journal of Educational Research
FIGURE 4. Standardized regression weights for the mathematics and language arts models in Study 2. Coefficients before the
slash are from mathematics; those after the slash are from language arts. ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic
Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR
= self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem. Bolded numbers represent significant
differences in standardized path coefficients between mathematics and language arts. ∗
p < .05. ∗∗
p < .01. ∗∗∗
p < .001.
outcomes. Academic self-concept and self-efficacy assessed
in reference to particular subject matter areas were able to
predict achievements in their respective domains, regardless
of the assessment scales. The only domain-specific construct
that failed to relate significantly to achievement was the ele-
mentary school students’ self-efficacy assessed by the MSLQ.
However, interpretation of this result as a failure of MSLQ
self-efficacy to predict achievement scores requires caution.
There existed a strong and significant bivariate correlation
between the elementary school students’ MSLQ self-efficacy
and achievement (φ = .53), which was quite commensurate
in magnitude to that between academic self-concept and
achievement (φ = .61) or Bandura-type self-efficacy and
achievement (φ = .56). It thus seems more reasonable to
view this finding as a result of collinearity between MSLQ
self-efficacy and task value included in the same structural
model, as will be discussed again later.
Global self-esteem that did not refer to any particular aca-
demic domain was not able to predict achievements in spe-
cific domains, except for achievement in language arts for the
middle school students. Although general self-esteem corre-
lated positively with achievement indexes for the elementary
and middle school students, these correlations were con-
siderably weaker than those demonstrated by the domain-
specific self-constructs. In fact, they were even weaker than
the correlations that task value and test anxiety, both mea-
sured in reference to specific subject matter areas, exhibited
with achievements. This proves that domain specificity is
an important prerequisite in construct assessment with re-
gard to not only self-constructs but also other motivation
variables, if the goal is to predict students’ achievements.
As our data further suggest, when asked domain-
specifically without any reference to target outcomes,
students do not distinguish between their self-efficacy
and self-concept as clearly as theories would predict.
Students’ academic self-concept assessed by the ASDQ
(Marsh, 1992) and their academic self-efficacy assessed by
the MSLQ (Pintrich et al., 1993) were almost identical,
with correlation coefficients approaching unity. Academic
self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale, which
provided students with concrete referents against which to
make ability judgments (e.g., “I can get more than ‘80’ in
the math exam”), correlated substantially less strongly with
the other two self-beliefs (correlations ranging between .76
and .84). Moreover, academic self-concept displayed the
strongest predictive paths to achievements in mathematics
for the elementary and middle school students in the
present research. This finding was surprising because it is
contrary to previous reports attesting to the superiority
of self-efficacy beliefs in predicting students’ academic
performances (Pajares & Miller, 1995; Pietsch et al., 2003).
One of the possible reasons for these results could be the
normative grading system the participants were exposed to.
Korean middle school students are evaluated against their
peers when receiving grades. The elementary school teach-
ers participating in the present research were also asked to
rate each student in comparison to other students they were
teaching. As Marsh, Trautwein, L¨udtke, and K¨oller (2008)
The Journal of Educational Research 349
suggested, students may engage in habitual comparison of
their performance levels with those of other students when
they answer perceived competence items because that is the
prevalent evaluation criteria they have been accustomed
to. In addition, students become sensitive to and begin
utilizing social comparison information by Grade 4 (Ruble
et al., 1976). Therefore, even without explicit allusion to
comparative information in the assessment scales, students
may nonetheless use normative standards unless concrete
performance targets are presented. This might explain
the strong correlation between the academic self-concept
and MSLQ self-efficacy scores observed in this study. As
normative judgments of competence were explicitly called
for only by the self-concept items (i.e., “Compared to others
my age I am good at mathematics classes”), these judgments
might also better predict performances appraised under a
normative system.
In contrast, Bandura-type self-efficacy demonstrated the
strongest predictive utility in the domain of language arts for
the middle school students among the three domain-specific
self-beliefs. This finding is intriguing because it was also the
only domain-specific self-belief that displayed analogous pre-
dictive power across ages and domains. Whereas academic
self-concept and MSLQ self-efficacy’s predictive paths to
achievements were noticeably stronger for the middle than
elementary school students and in the domain of mathemat-
ics than language arts, Bandura-type self-efficacy’s predictive
relations to performances were comparable in magnitude
across the two age groups and the two subject matter areas.
Considering that it may be easier to judge an individual’s
relative ability compared with others in mathematics than
language arts due to the nature of the subject matter and
the fact that comparative judgments of ability become more
salient as children grow older (Ruble et al., 1976), these re-
sults provide empirical support to the theoretical tenet that
self-concept is more heavily affected by social comparison
than is self-efficacy (Bong & Clark, 1999; Bong & Skaalvik,
2003). More importantly, our findings support the extant
literature that domain-specific constructs, self-efficacy and
self-concept, are better predictors of achievement compared
with global self-esteem (Bandalos et al., 1995; Hansford &
Hattie, 1982).
Task Value and Test Anxiety as Mediators of
Self–Achievement Relations
The theoretical model linking self-constructs to achieve-
ments with task value and test anxiety as mediators received
clear support only with respect to Bandura-type self-efficacy
in the middle school sample. As hypothesized and consis-
tent with previous research (e.g., Pajares & Kranzler, 1995),
task value positively mediated and test anxiety negatively
mediated the link between self-efficacy and achievement.
Students with stronger self-efficacy are likely to experience
lower anxiety, and those who are high in anxiety are likely
to have low achievement (Meier, McCarthy, & Schmeck,
1984). The opposite pattern holds for task value. Students
with stronger self-efficacy also express stronger task value,
and those who perceive stronger value in the subject tend
to attain higher achievement in it (Meier, McCarthy, &
Schmeck, 1984).
The mediation hypothesis did not hold for self-concept.
Students who reported higher levels of self-concept were
likely to demonstrate higher achievement but this rela-
tionship was not necessarily mediated by anxiety. This
is likely due to the strong prediction of achievement by
self-concept. Because self-concept already encompasses
affective responses to perceived competencies (Bong &
Clark, 1999; Covington, 1984), as demonstrated by its
strong correlation with test anxiety in this research as well,
no further variance in student achievement might have
been explained by test anxiety after the variance due to
self-concept was accounted for.
Unfortunately, lack of differentiation among variables was
not a problem confined only to self-constructs. Academic
self-efficacy beliefs assessed by the MSLQ scale correlated
too strongly with task value across ages and domains (with
correlations ranging between .81 and .87). The correlations
of academic self-concept with task value were also very high,
although slightly lower than those of the MSLQ scale (rang-
ing from .74 to .79). We thus warn researchers of the pos-
sibility of multicollinearity hindering attainment of proper
solutions when other domain-specific constructs such as task
value and test anxiety are included in the same model with
self-constructs. We cautiously recommend use of self-efficacy
beliefs assessed by the Bandura-type scale, if the goal is to
examine the predictive utility of perceived self-competence
in a complex nomological network. Use of Bandura-type
self-efficacy ratings will be least affected by collinearity with
other variables and hence enable delineation of all possible
predictive and mediational paths to student achievement,
as documented in this study.
Age and Academic Domain as Moderators of Self–Achievement
Relations
Consistent with our hypothesis, the middle school sample
in our study was associated with stronger predictive pat-
terns between self-constructs and achievements. However,
the differences were not as significant as we had anticipated.
Given our sample of elementary school students consisting
of fifth- and sixth-grade students, it is likely that they were al-
ready realistic and sensitive to normative information when
judging their abilities (Nicholls, 1975, 1978; Ruble et al.,
1976). Therefore, the predictive patterns for the elementary
school students were not drastically different from those for
the middle school students.
Regarding differences between domains, the predictive
paths from domain-specific self-constructs to achievements
were generally stronger in mathematics compared with lan-
guage arts. We interpret this finding to indicate that students
possess better knowledge about their own performance levels
350 The Journal of Educational Research
in mathematics because evaluation standards in mathemat-
ics tend to be more solidly established compared with those
in language arts. Again, the predictive paths emanating from
Bandura-type self-efficacy to achievements were exceptions
to this trend because there was no noticeable difference
in them across domains. It appears that self-efficacy beliefs
assessed by items that present either specific target perfor-
mances or concrete judgment standards afford the most ro-
bust prediction of achievements that is not compromised
by age, domain, or collinearity with other constructs. De-
spite the differences in the magnitude of predictive relations,
the pattern with which each self-construct links to achieve-
ment was consistent in both subjects and no unique pattern
emerged domain-wise.
Interestingly, global self-esteem exhibited much stronger
correlation with academic self-concept and academic self-
efficacy assessed by the MSLQ scale in the domain of math-
ematics than language arts. Moreover, these mathematics-
specific self-perceptions better predicted test anxiety and
achievement in the domain than did domain-specific self-
perceptions in language arts. Taken together, these results
seem to suggest that self-perceived competence in mathe-
matics is more psychologically salient than self-perceived
competence in language arts for the middle school partici-
pants in this study.
Conclusions and Future Directions
We compared the predictive utility of multiple self-
constructs with different degrees of domain specificity. We
found domain-specific self-perceptions such as academic self-
concept and self-efficacy to demonstrate superior predictive
utility compared with global self-esteem. We also tested task
value and test anxiety as potential mediators of the self-
constructs’ effects on achievements. The complete model in-
cluding all hypothesized mediators received clear empirical
support only in the model with Bandura-type self-efficacy.
We examined these hypothesized predictive paths in two age
groups and two subject matter areas in an attempt to locate
important possible moderators and establish generalizability
of our findings. Age and academic domain proved to moder-
ate the predictive relations to a certain degree, with domain
creating a greater gap in the self-constructs’ predictive power
than age.
Several limitations should be noted. First, due to multi-
collinearity problems, we were not able to include all rele-
vant constructs in a single SEM. We suggest researchers ex-
amine ways to distinguish domain-specific constructs more
clearly in future studies. At the same time, the possibil-
ity of cultural differences creating a different pattern of
relationships cannot be completely ruled out. Our results
showed that the model with three separate self-constructs,
self-esteem, self-concept, and self-efficacy displayed satisfac-
tory fit to the empirical data, consistent with the previous
findings with Western samples. However, the correlation co-
efficients between the self-concept and self-efficacy factors
were still too high in this study, which suggests a possibil-
ity of culture-specific finding. Second, although we included
elementary and middle school samples in this study, the
age difference between the two groups did not appear suf-
ficiently large to capture developmental differences. It may
be a worthwhile endeavor for investigators in this area to
examine the point at which diverse self-constructs start to
diverge and demonstrate disparate patterns of association
with achievement-related motivation, emotion, and perfor-
mance. Finally, we attributed the superior predictive utility
of self-concept compared with self-efficacy in the present
study to students’ increased awareness of social comparative
information and the normative evaluation system they were
exposed to. It will be interesting to see whether the present
results replicate among students that are not functioning
under such normative grading structures.
ACKNOWLEDGMENT
This research was supported by the WCU (World Class University) Pro-
gram funded by the Korean Ministry of Education, Science and Technology,
consigned to the Korea Science and Engineering Foundation (Grant no.
R32-2008-000-20023-0).
REFERENCES
Arbuckle, J. L. (2006). AMOS (Version 7.0) [Computer software]. Chicago,
IL: SPSS.
Bachman, J. G., & O’Malley, P. M. (1977). Self-esteem in young men:
A longitudinal analysis of the impact of educational and occupational
attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 365–380.
Bandalos, D. L., Yates, K., & Thorndike-Christ, T. (1995). Effects of math
self-concept, perceived self-efficacy, and attributions for failure and suc-
cess on test anxiety. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 611–623.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral
change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive
theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A. (1988). Self-efficacy conception of anxiety. Anxiety Research,
1, 77–98.
Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American
Psychologist, 44, 1175–1184.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY:
Freeman.
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-
efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 586–598.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 107, 238–246.
Berndt, T. J., & Miller, K. E. (1990). Expectancies, values, and achievement
in junior high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 319–326.
Bong, M. (1997). Generality of academic self-efficacy judgments: Evidence
of hierarchical relations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 696–709.
Bong, M. (1998). Tests of the internal/external frames of reference model
with subject-specific academic self-efficacy and frame-specific academic
self-concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 102–110.
Bong, M. (2001a). Between- and within-domain relations of academic mo-
tivation among middle and high school students: Self-efficacy, task-value,
and achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 23–34.
Bong, M. (2001b). Role of self-efficacy and task-value in predicting college
students’ course performance and future enrollment intentions. Contem-
porary Educational Psychology, 26, 553–570.
Bong, M. (2009). Age-related differences in achievement goal differentia-
tion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 879–896.
Bong, M., & Clark, R. E. (1999). Comparison between self-concept and
self-efficacy in academic motivation research. Educational Psychologist,
34, 139–154.
The Journal of Educational Research 351
Bong, M., & Hocevar, D. (2002). Measuring self-efficacy: Multitrait-
multimethod comparison of scaling procedures. Applied Measurement in
Education, 15, 143–171.
Bong, M., & Skaalvik, E. M. (2003). Academic self-concept and self-
efficacy: How different are they really? Educational Psychology Review,
15, 1–40.
Brookover, W. B., Thomas, S., & Paterson, A. (1964). Self-concept of
ability and school achievement. Sociology of Education, 37, 271–278.
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts,
application, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Byrne, B. M., & Shavelson, R. J. (1986). On the structure of the adolescent
self-concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 474–481.
Calsyn, R. J., & Kenny, D. A. (1977). Self-concept of ability and perceived
evaluation of others: Cause or effect of academic achievement? Journal
of Educational Psychology, 69, 136–145.
Choi, N. (2005). Self-efficacy and self-concept as predictors of college
students’ academic performance. Psychology in the Schools, 42, 197–205.
Covington, M. V. (1984). The self-worth theory of achievement motiva-
tion: Findings and implications. The Elementary School Journal, 85, 4–20.
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., & Adler, T. F. (1984). Grade-related changes
in the school environment: Effects on achievement motivation. In J. G.
Nicholls (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation (pp. 283–331).
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the achiever: The
structure of adolescents’ academic achievement related-beliefs and self-
perceptions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 215–225.
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993). Age
and gender differences in children’s self- and task perceptions during
elementary school. Child Development, 64, 830–847.
Hansford, B. C., & Hattie, J. A. (1982). The relationship between self and
achievement/performance measures. Review of Educational Research, 52,
123–142.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective.
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covari-
ance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives.
Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55.
Hulleman, C. S., Durik, A. M., Schweigert, S. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M.
(2008). Task values, achievement goals, and interest: An integrative
analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 398–416.
Hunt, J. McV., Cole, M. W., & Reis, E. E. S. (1958). Situational cues
distinguishing anger, fear, and sorrow. American Journal of Psychology, 71,
136–151.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd
ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kun, A., Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. (1974). The development of integra-
tion processes using ability and effort information to predict outcome.
Developmental Psychology, 10, 721–732.
Ludwig, D. J., & Maehr, M. L. (1967). Changes in self concept and stated
behavioral preferences. Child Development, 38, 453–467.
MacCallum, R. C., Roznowski, M., Mar, C. M., & Reith, J. V. (1994).
Alternative strategies for cross-validation of covariance structure models.
Multivariate Behavioral Research, 29, 1–32.
Marsh, H. W. (1986). Verbal and math self-concepts: An internal/external
frame of reference model. American Educational Research Journal, 23,
129–149.
Marsh, H. W. (1987). The big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self-
concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 280–295.
Marsh, H. W. (1990). Influences of internal and external frames of reference
on the formation of math and English self-concepts. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 82, 107–116.
Marsh, H. W. (1991). Failure of high-ability high schools to deliver aca-
demic benefits commensurate with their students’ ability levels. American
Educational Research Journal, 28, 445–480.
Marsh, H. W. (1992). Self-Description Questionnaire-II: Manual. New South
Wales, Australia: University of Western Sydney.
Marsh, H. W., Barnes, J., Cairns, L., & Tidman, M. (1984). Self-description
questionnaire: Age and sex effects in the structure and level of self-
concept for preadolescent children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76,
940–956.
Marsh, H. W., Byrne, B. M., & Shavelson, R. J. (1988). A multifaceted aca-
demic self-concept: Its hierarchical structure and its relation to academic
achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 366–380.
Marsh, H. W., Byrne, B. M., & Yeung, A. S. (1999). Causal ordering of
academic self-concept and achievement: Reanalysis of a pioneering study
and revised recommendations. Educational Psychologist, 34, 154–157.
Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., L¨udtke, O., & K¨oller, O. (2008). Social
comparison and big-fish-little-pond effects on self-concept and other
self-belief constructs: Role of generalized and specific others. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 100, 510–524.
Marsh, H. W., Walker, R., & Debus, R. (1991). Subject-specific compo-
nents of academic self-concept and self-efficacy. Contemporary Educa-
tional Psychology, 16, 331–345.
Meier, S., McCarthy, P. R., & Schmeck, R. (1984). Validity of self-efficacy
as a predictor of writing performance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8,
107–120.
Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). Relation of self-efficacy
beliefs to academic outcomes: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 38, 30–38.
Nicholls, J. G. (1975). Causal attributions and other achievement-related
cognitions: effects of task outcome, attainment value, and sex. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 379–389.
Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The development of the concepts of effort and
ability, perception of academic attainment, and the understanding that
difficult tasks require more ability. Child Development, 49, 800–814.
Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Edu-
cational Research, 66, 543–578.
Pajares, F., & Kranzler, J. (1995). Self-efficacy beliefs and general men-
tal ability in mathematical problem-solving. Contemporary Educational
Psychology, 20, 426–443.
Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-concept
beliefs in mathematical problem solving: A path analysis. Journal of Ed-
ucational Psychology, 86, 193–203.
Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1995). Mathematics self-efficacy and math-
ematics performances: The need for specificity of assessment. Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 42, 190–198.
Pajares, F., & Schunk, D. H. (2002). Self and self-belief in psychology and
education: A historical perspective. In J. Aronson & D. Cordova (Eds.),
Improving academic achievement: Impact of psychological factors on education
(pp. 3–21). New York, NY: Academic Press.
Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. N. (1977). The development of achievement-
related expectancies. Child Development, 48, 1075–1079.
Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2006). Achievement goals and
discrete achievement emotions: A theoretical model and prospective
test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 583–597.
Pietsch, J., Walker, R., & Chapman, E. (2003). The relationship among self-
concept, self-efficacy, and performance in mathematics during secondary
school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 589–603.
Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated
learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Ed-
ucational Psychology, 82, 33–40.
Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993).
Reliability and predictive validity of the Motivated Strategies for Learn-
ing Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological Measurement,
53, 801–813.
Pokay, P., & Blumenfeld, P. C. (1990). Predicting achievement early and
late in the semester: The role of motivation and use of learning strategies.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 41–50.
Pullman, H., & Allik, J. (2008). Relations of academic and general self-
esteem to school achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 45,
559–564.
Rosenberg, M. (1962). The association between self-esteem and anxiety.
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 1, 135–152.
Rosenberg, M., Schooler, C., & Schoenbach, C. (1989). Self-esteem and
adolescent problems: Modeling reciprocal effects. American Sociological
Review, 54, 1004–1018.
Rosenberg, M., Schooler, C., Schoenbach, C., & Rosenberg, F. (1995).
Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: Different concepts, different
outcomes. American Sociological Review, 60, 141–156.
Ruble, D. N., Parsons, J. E., & Ross, J. (1976). Self-evaluative responses of
children in an achievement setting. Child Development, 47, 990–997.
Ruthig, J. C., Perry, R. P., Hladkyj, S., Hall, N. C., Pekrun, R., & Chipper-
field, J. G. (2008). A longitudinal analysis of perceived control and emo-
tions in an academic setting. Social Psychology of Education, 11, 161–180.
Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.). (2001). Appraisal pro-
cesses in emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
352 The Journal of Educational Research
Schreiber, J. B., Stage, F. K., King, J., Nora, A., & Barlow, E. A. (2006).
Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis
results: A review. Journal of Educational Research, 99, 323–337.
Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational
Psychologist, 26, 207–231.
Shavelson, R. J., & Bolus, R. (1982). Self-concept: The interplay of theory
and methods. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 3–47.
Shavelson, R. J., Hubner, J. J., & Stanton, G. C. (1976). Self-concept:
Validation of construct interpretations. Review of Educational Research,
46, 407–441.
Simmons, R. G., Blyth, D. A., Van Cleave, E. F., & Bush, D. M.
(1979). Entry into early adolescence: The impact of school structure,
puberty, and early dating on self-esteem. American Sociological Review, 44,
948–967.
Skaalvik, E. M., & Hagtvet, K. A. (1990). Academic achievement and
self-concept: An analysis of causal predominance in a developmental
perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 292–307.
Skaalvik, E. M., & Rankin, R. J. (1995). A test of the Internal/External
Frame of Reference Model at different levels of math and verbal self-
perception. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 161–184.
Smelser, N. (1989). Self-esteem and social problems: An introduction. In A.
M. Mecca, N. J. Smelser, & J. Vasconcellos (Eds.), The social importance
of self-esteem (pp. 294–326). Berkeley: University of California Press.
Stipek, D., & Mac Iver, D. (1989). Developmental change in children’s
assessment of intellectual competence. Child Development, 60, 521–538.
Stipek, D., & Tannatt, L. (1984). Children’s judgments of their own and
their peers’ academic competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76,
75–84.
Stodolski, S. S., Salk, S., & Glaessner, B. (1991). Student views about
learning math and social studies. American Educational Research Journal,
28, 89–116.
Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum
likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38, 1–10.
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., Harold, R. D., Arbreton, A. J.
A., Freedman-Doan, C., & Blumenfeld, P. C. (1997). Change in chil-
dren’s competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elemen-
tary school years: A 3-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89,
451–469.
Zeidner, M., & Schleyer, E. J. (1999). The effects on educational con-
text on individual difference variables, self-perceptions of giftedness, and
school attitudes in gifted adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
28, 687–703.
Zimmerman, B. J. (1995). Self-efficacy and educational development. In
A. Bandura (Ed.), Self-efficacy in changing societies (pp. 202–231). New
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Zimmerman, B. J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Self-
motivation for academic attainment: The role of self-efficacy beliefs
and personal goal setting. American Educational Research Journal, 29,
663–676.
AUTHORS NOTE
Mimi Bong is a Professor of Educational Psychology in
the Department of Education and the Associate Director
of bMRI (Brain and Motivation Research Institute) at Ko-
rea University. Her current research interests focus on the
dynamic interplay between student motivation and learn-
ing contexts, with particular emphases on self-efficacy and
achievement goals.
Catherine Cho was with Korea University at the time of
the study and is now a PhD student in neuroscience in the
Department of Psychology at Rutgers University–Newark.
Her research interest broadly focuses on the interaction
between cognition and emotion that affects decision-
making processes. More specifically, she is interested in
the mechanisms of emotion regulation and how success-
ful regulation of emotions guides optimal decisions and
choices.
Hyun Seon Ahn is a PhD student in educational psy-
chology in the Department of Education and a research
assistant of bMRI at Korea University. Her research in-
terests include self-efficacy beliefs and classroom goal
structures.
Hyejin Kim is a PhD student in educational psychol-
ogy in the Department of Education and a research assis-
tant of bMRI at Korea University. Her current research
interests focus on personality, cognitive engagement, so-
cial goals, social efficacy, and social adjustment among
adolescents.
Copyright of Journal of Educational Research is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not
be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

Styles of learning and thinking matter in instruction and assessment
Styles of learning and thinking matter in instruction and assessmentStyles of learning and thinking matter in instruction and assessment
Styles of learning and thinking matter in instruction and assessmentJamaica Mirador
 
A Review of CDMSES
A Review of CDMSESA Review of CDMSES
A Review of CDMSESJason Cheney
 
The Contributions of Teaching Type and Perceived Task Difficulty on Competency
The Contributions of Teaching Type and Perceived Task Difficulty on CompetencyThe Contributions of Teaching Type and Perceived Task Difficulty on Competency
The Contributions of Teaching Type and Perceived Task Difficulty on CompetencySasha Albrecht
 
Facilitating Group Discussionswebposting
Facilitating Group DiscussionswebpostingFacilitating Group Discussionswebposting
Facilitating Group DiscussionswebpostingVineet
 
Behavior assessment system for children (BASC)
Behavior assessment system for children (BASC)Behavior assessment system for children (BASC)
Behavior assessment system for children (BASC)Anowra Khan
 
300ResearchProject (real one)
300ResearchProject (real one)300ResearchProject (real one)
300ResearchProject (real one)Kory Gill
 
Institutions and the Fromation of Gender Paradigms_Bahctel HDFS 4610docx
Institutions and the Fromation of Gender Paradigms_Bahctel HDFS 4610docxInstitutions and the Fromation of Gender Paradigms_Bahctel HDFS 4610docx
Institutions and the Fromation of Gender Paradigms_Bahctel HDFS 4610docxsara bachtel
 
The Thesis to End all Thesis
The Thesis to End all Thesis The Thesis to End all Thesis
The Thesis to End all Thesis Rebekah DeVore
 
Motivation
Motivation   Motivation
Motivation mizah16
 
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivationCognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivationEl Sameeha
 
Hendrickson praise
Hendrickson praiseHendrickson praise
Hendrickson praiseEmma Grice
 
Test-Anxiety and Self-Esteem as a Predictor of Secondary School Students’ Aca...
Test-Anxiety and Self-Esteem as a Predictor of Secondary School Students’ Aca...Test-Anxiety and Self-Esteem as a Predictor of Secondary School Students’ Aca...
Test-Anxiety and Self-Esteem as a Predictor of Secondary School Students’ Aca...inventionjournals
 

Was ist angesagt? (19)

Styles of learning and thinking matter in instruction and assessment
Styles of learning and thinking matter in instruction and assessmentStyles of learning and thinking matter in instruction and assessment
Styles of learning and thinking matter in instruction and assessment
 
A Review of CDMSES
A Review of CDMSESA Review of CDMSES
A Review of CDMSES
 
Extrinsec & intrinsec
Extrinsec & intrinsecExtrinsec & intrinsec
Extrinsec & intrinsec
 
Vela1
Vela1Vela1
Vela1
 
Basc
BascBasc
Basc
 
The Contributions of Teaching Type and Perceived Task Difficulty on Competency
The Contributions of Teaching Type and Perceived Task Difficulty on CompetencyThe Contributions of Teaching Type and Perceived Task Difficulty on Competency
The Contributions of Teaching Type and Perceived Task Difficulty on Competency
 
Facilitating Group Discussionswebposting
Facilitating Group DiscussionswebpostingFacilitating Group Discussionswebposting
Facilitating Group Discussionswebposting
 
10 chapter3
10 chapter310 chapter3
10 chapter3
 
Motivation Report
Motivation ReportMotivation Report
Motivation Report
 
Behavior assessment system for children (BASC)
Behavior assessment system for children (BASC)Behavior assessment system for children (BASC)
Behavior assessment system for children (BASC)
 
300ResearchProject (real one)
300ResearchProject (real one)300ResearchProject (real one)
300ResearchProject (real one)
 
Institutions and the Fromation of Gender Paradigms_Bahctel HDFS 4610docx
Institutions and the Fromation of Gender Paradigms_Bahctel HDFS 4610docxInstitutions and the Fromation of Gender Paradigms_Bahctel HDFS 4610docx
Institutions and the Fromation of Gender Paradigms_Bahctel HDFS 4610docx
 
The Thesis to End all Thesis
The Thesis to End all Thesis The Thesis to End all Thesis
The Thesis to End all Thesis
 
Motivation
Motivation   Motivation
Motivation
 
Examining Theory
Examining TheoryExamining Theory
Examining Theory
 
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivationCognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
 
Karina
KarinaKarina
Karina
 
Hendrickson praise
Hendrickson praiseHendrickson praise
Hendrickson praise
 
Test-Anxiety and Self-Esteem as a Predictor of Secondary School Students’ Aca...
Test-Anxiety and Self-Esteem as a Predictor of Secondary School Students’ Aca...Test-Anxiety and Self-Esteem as a Predictor of Secondary School Students’ Aca...
Test-Anxiety and Self-Esteem as a Predictor of Secondary School Students’ Aca...
 

Andere mochten auch

Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007-phpapp01
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007-phpapp01Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007-phpapp01
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007-phpapp01Diana Delgado
 
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007Diana Delgado
 
Attitudes self-concept-values-and-ethics
Attitudes self-concept-values-and-ethicsAttitudes self-concept-values-and-ethics
Attitudes self-concept-values-and-ethicsDianopesidas
 
Visual Design with Data
Visual Design with DataVisual Design with Data
Visual Design with DataSeth Familian
 
3 Things Every Sales Team Needs to Be Thinking About in 2017
3 Things Every Sales Team Needs to Be Thinking About in 20173 Things Every Sales Team Needs to Be Thinking About in 2017
3 Things Every Sales Team Needs to Be Thinking About in 2017Drift
 
How to Become a Thought Leader in Your Niche
How to Become a Thought Leader in Your NicheHow to Become a Thought Leader in Your Niche
How to Become a Thought Leader in Your NicheLeslie Samuel
 

Andere mochten auch (6)

Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007-phpapp01
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007-phpapp01Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007-phpapp01
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007-phpapp01
 
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007
Ejerciciosdeconcentracinyatencin 130628171007
 
Attitudes self-concept-values-and-ethics
Attitudes self-concept-values-and-ethicsAttitudes self-concept-values-and-ethics
Attitudes self-concept-values-and-ethics
 
Visual Design with Data
Visual Design with DataVisual Design with Data
Visual Design with Data
 
3 Things Every Sales Team Needs to Be Thinking About in 2017
3 Things Every Sales Team Needs to Be Thinking About in 20173 Things Every Sales Team Needs to Be Thinking About in 2017
3 Things Every Sales Team Needs to Be Thinking About in 2017
 
How to Become a Thought Leader in Your Niche
How to Become a Thought Leader in Your NicheHow to Become a Thought Leader in Your Niche
How to Become a Thought Leader in Your Niche
 

Ähnlich wie Comparison o f self beliefs for predicting student achievement

Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comDr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comWilliam Kritsonis
 
Dr. Paul Watkins & Dr. Janet Moak
Dr. Paul Watkins & Dr. Janet MoakDr. Paul Watkins & Dr. Janet Moak
Dr. Paul Watkins & Dr. Janet MoakWilliam Kritsonis
 
Psycho socio variables as correlates of junior secondary school
Psycho socio variables as correlates of junior secondary schoolPsycho socio variables as correlates of junior secondary school
Psycho socio variables as correlates of junior secondary schoolAlexander Decker
 
A Study on Self Concept in Relation to Academic Achievement Among B.Ed Trainees
A Study on Self Concept in Relation to Academic Achievement Among B.Ed TraineesA Study on Self Concept in Relation to Academic Achievement Among B.Ed Trainees
A Study on Self Concept in Relation to Academic Achievement Among B.Ed Traineesijtsrd
 
Motivation and equity in education
Motivation and equity in educationMotivation and equity in education
Motivation and equity in educationKamden Strunk
 
Rebecca Davis Writing Sample
Rebecca Davis Writing SampleRebecca Davis Writing Sample
Rebecca Davis Writing SampleRebecca Davis
 
A Critical Review Of Research On Student Self-Assessment
A Critical Review Of Research On Student Self-AssessmentA Critical Review Of Research On Student Self-Assessment
A Critical Review Of Research On Student Self-AssessmentGina Rizzo
 
Course ID BTM7300v2 Week 4 Outline of the Comprehensive .docx
Course ID BTM7300v2 Week 4 Outline of the Comprehensive .docxCourse ID BTM7300v2 Week 4 Outline of the Comprehensive .docx
Course ID BTM7300v2 Week 4 Outline of the Comprehensive .docxvoversbyobersby
 
The Private Victory and the Student Readiness Inventory: Three Habits of Stud...
The Private Victory and the Student Readiness Inventory: Three Habits of Stud...The Private Victory and the Student Readiness Inventory: Three Habits of Stud...
The Private Victory and the Student Readiness Inventory: Three Habits of Stud...Sean Weigold Ferguson
 
Article review yukon territory
Article review yukon territoryArticle review yukon territory
Article review yukon territoryAdibah H. Mutalib
 
Relationship between Personality Traits, Academic Achievement and Salary: An ...
Relationship between Personality Traits, Academic Achievement and Salary: An ...Relationship between Personality Traits, Academic Achievement and Salary: An ...
Relationship between Personality Traits, Academic Achievement and Salary: An ...iosrjce
 
In a 1,000-1,250-word paper, consider the outcome and process meas
In a 1,000-1,250-word paper, consider the outcome and process measIn a 1,000-1,250-word paper, consider the outcome and process meas
In a 1,000-1,250-word paper, consider the outcome and process measLizbethQuinonez813
 
About The Differences Of Teachers Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
About The Differences Of Teachers  Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...About The Differences Of Teachers  Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
About The Differences Of Teachers Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...Joshua Gorinson
 

Ähnlich wie Comparison o f self beliefs for predicting student achievement (20)

2014 yesulyurt
2014 yesulyurt2014 yesulyurt
2014 yesulyurt
 
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.comDr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
Dr. W.A. Kritsonis, National FORUM Journals, www.nationalforum.com
 
Dr. Paul Watkins & Dr. Janet Moak
Dr. Paul Watkins & Dr. Janet MoakDr. Paul Watkins & Dr. Janet Moak
Dr. Paul Watkins & Dr. Janet Moak
 
Psycho socio variables as correlates of junior secondary school
Psycho socio variables as correlates of junior secondary schoolPsycho socio variables as correlates of junior secondary school
Psycho socio variables as correlates of junior secondary school
 
A Study on Self Concept in Relation to Academic Achievement Among B.Ed Trainees
A Study on Self Concept in Relation to Academic Achievement Among B.Ed TraineesA Study on Self Concept in Relation to Academic Achievement Among B.Ed Trainees
A Study on Self Concept in Relation to Academic Achievement Among B.Ed Trainees
 
Motivation and equity in education
Motivation and equity in educationMotivation and equity in education
Motivation and equity in education
 
Analysis and Results
Analysis and ResultsAnalysis and Results
Analysis and Results
 
1c assignment
1c assignment1c assignment
1c assignment
 
FinalDraft
FinalDraftFinalDraft
FinalDraft
 
Rebecca Davis Writing Sample
Rebecca Davis Writing SampleRebecca Davis Writing Sample
Rebecca Davis Writing Sample
 
A Critical Review Of Research On Student Self-Assessment
A Critical Review Of Research On Student Self-AssessmentA Critical Review Of Research On Student Self-Assessment
A Critical Review Of Research On Student Self-Assessment
 
Action Research
Action ResearchAction Research
Action Research
 
Course ID BTM7300v2 Week 4 Outline of the Comprehensive .docx
Course ID BTM7300v2 Week 4 Outline of the Comprehensive .docxCourse ID BTM7300v2 Week 4 Outline of the Comprehensive .docx
Course ID BTM7300v2 Week 4 Outline of the Comprehensive .docx
 
The Private Victory and the Student Readiness Inventory: Three Habits of Stud...
The Private Victory and the Student Readiness Inventory: Three Habits of Stud...The Private Victory and the Student Readiness Inventory: Three Habits of Stud...
The Private Victory and the Student Readiness Inventory: Three Habits of Stud...
 
Article review yukon territory
Article review yukon territoryArticle review yukon territory
Article review yukon territory
 
Relationship between Personality Traits, Academic Achievement and Salary: An ...
Relationship between Personality Traits, Academic Achievement and Salary: An ...Relationship between Personality Traits, Academic Achievement and Salary: An ...
Relationship between Personality Traits, Academic Achievement and Salary: An ...
 
Annamarie chapter 2
Annamarie chapter 2Annamarie chapter 2
Annamarie chapter 2
 
Ez35875879
Ez35875879Ez35875879
Ez35875879
 
In a 1,000-1,250-word paper, consider the outcome and process meas
In a 1,000-1,250-word paper, consider the outcome and process measIn a 1,000-1,250-word paper, consider the outcome and process meas
In a 1,000-1,250-word paper, consider the outcome and process meas
 
About The Differences Of Teachers Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
About The Differences Of Teachers  Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...About The Differences Of Teachers  Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
About The Differences Of Teachers Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

FULL NIGHT — 9999894380 Call Girls In Saket | Delhi
FULL NIGHT — 9999894380 Call Girls In Saket | DelhiFULL NIGHT — 9999894380 Call Girls In Saket | Delhi
FULL NIGHT — 9999894380 Call Girls In Saket | DelhiSaketCallGirlsCallUs
 
Patrakarpuram ) Cheap Call Girls In Lucknow (Adult Only) 🧈 8923113531 𓀓 Esco...
Patrakarpuram ) Cheap Call Girls In Lucknow  (Adult Only) 🧈 8923113531 𓀓 Esco...Patrakarpuram ) Cheap Call Girls In Lucknow  (Adult Only) 🧈 8923113531 𓀓 Esco...
Patrakarpuram ) Cheap Call Girls In Lucknow (Adult Only) 🧈 8923113531 𓀓 Esco...akbard9823
 
The First Date by Daniel Johnson (Inspired By True Events)
The First Date by Daniel Johnson (Inspired By True Events)The First Date by Daniel Johnson (Inspired By True Events)
The First Date by Daniel Johnson (Inspired By True Events)thephillipta
 
Storyboard short: Ferrarius Tries to Sing
Storyboard short: Ferrarius Tries to SingStoryboard short: Ferrarius Tries to Sing
Storyboard short: Ferrarius Tries to SingLyneSun
 
Young⚡Call Girls in Uttam Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
Young⚡Call Girls in Uttam Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort ServiceYoung⚡Call Girls in Uttam Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
Young⚡Call Girls in Uttam Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Servicesonnydelhi1992
 
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 892311...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 892311...Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 892311...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 892311...anilsa9823
 
Young⚡Call Girls in Lajpat Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
Young⚡Call Girls in Lajpat Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort ServiceYoung⚡Call Girls in Lajpat Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
Young⚡Call Girls in Lajpat Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Servicesonnydelhi1992
 
Lucknow 💋 Call Girls in Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payment 892311...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girls in Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payment 892311...Lucknow 💋 Call Girls in Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payment 892311...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girls in Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payment 892311...anilsa9823
 
Call girls in Kanpur - 9761072362 with room service
Call girls in Kanpur - 9761072362 with room serviceCall girls in Kanpur - 9761072362 with room service
Call girls in Kanpur - 9761072362 with room servicediscovermytutordmt
 
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow Phone No 8923113531 Elite Escort Service Avail...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow Phone No 8923113531 Elite Escort Service Avail...Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow Phone No 8923113531 Elite Escort Service Avail...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow Phone No 8923113531 Elite Escort Service Avail...anilsa9823
 
Lucknow 💋 Female Escorts Service in Lucknow | Service-oriented sexy call girl...
Lucknow 💋 Female Escorts Service in Lucknow | Service-oriented sexy call girl...Lucknow 💋 Female Escorts Service in Lucknow | Service-oriented sexy call girl...
Lucknow 💋 Female Escorts Service in Lucknow | Service-oriented sexy call girl...anilsa9823
 
Hazratganj / Call Girl in Lucknow - Phone 🫗 8923113531 ☛ Escorts Service at 6...
Hazratganj / Call Girl in Lucknow - Phone 🫗 8923113531 ☛ Escorts Service at 6...Hazratganj / Call Girl in Lucknow - Phone 🫗 8923113531 ☛ Escorts Service at 6...
Hazratganj / Call Girl in Lucknow - Phone 🫗 8923113531 ☛ Escorts Service at 6...akbard9823
 
Lucknow 💋 High Profile Call Girls in Lucknow - Book 8923113531 Call Girls Ava...
Lucknow 💋 High Profile Call Girls in Lucknow - Book 8923113531 Call Girls Ava...Lucknow 💋 High Profile Call Girls in Lucknow - Book 8923113531 Call Girls Ava...
Lucknow 💋 High Profile Call Girls in Lucknow - Book 8923113531 Call Girls Ava...anilsa9823
 
Lucknow 💋 Escort Service in Lucknow (Adult Only) 8923113531 Escort Service 2...
Lucknow 💋 Escort Service in Lucknow  (Adult Only) 8923113531 Escort Service 2...Lucknow 💋 Escort Service in Lucknow  (Adult Only) 8923113531 Escort Service 2...
Lucknow 💋 Escort Service in Lucknow (Adult Only) 8923113531 Escort Service 2...anilsa9823
 
Call Girl Service In Dubai #$# O56521286O #$# Dubai Call Girls
Call Girl Service In Dubai #$# O56521286O #$# Dubai Call GirlsCall Girl Service In Dubai #$# O56521286O #$# Dubai Call Girls
Call Girl Service In Dubai #$# O56521286O #$# Dubai Call Girlsparisharma5056
 
Authentic # 00971556872006 # Hot Call Girls Service in Dubai By International...
Authentic # 00971556872006 # Hot Call Girls Service in Dubai By International...Authentic # 00971556872006 # Hot Call Girls Service in Dubai By International...
Authentic # 00971556872006 # Hot Call Girls Service in Dubai By International...home
 
Bobbie goods coloring book 81 pag_240127_163802.pdf
Bobbie goods coloring book 81 pag_240127_163802.pdfBobbie goods coloring book 81 pag_240127_163802.pdf
Bobbie goods coloring book 81 pag_240127_163802.pdfMARIBEL442158
 
Indira Nagar Lucknow #Call Girls Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payme...
Indira Nagar Lucknow #Call Girls Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payme...Indira Nagar Lucknow #Call Girls Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payme...
Indira Nagar Lucknow #Call Girls Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payme...akbard9823
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

FULL NIGHT — 9999894380 Call Girls In Saket | Delhi
FULL NIGHT — 9999894380 Call Girls In Saket | DelhiFULL NIGHT — 9999894380 Call Girls In Saket | Delhi
FULL NIGHT — 9999894380 Call Girls In Saket | Delhi
 
Patrakarpuram ) Cheap Call Girls In Lucknow (Adult Only) 🧈 8923113531 𓀓 Esco...
Patrakarpuram ) Cheap Call Girls In Lucknow  (Adult Only) 🧈 8923113531 𓀓 Esco...Patrakarpuram ) Cheap Call Girls In Lucknow  (Adult Only) 🧈 8923113531 𓀓 Esco...
Patrakarpuram ) Cheap Call Girls In Lucknow (Adult Only) 🧈 8923113531 𓀓 Esco...
 
The First Date by Daniel Johnson (Inspired By True Events)
The First Date by Daniel Johnson (Inspired By True Events)The First Date by Daniel Johnson (Inspired By True Events)
The First Date by Daniel Johnson (Inspired By True Events)
 
Storyboard short: Ferrarius Tries to Sing
Storyboard short: Ferrarius Tries to SingStoryboard short: Ferrarius Tries to Sing
Storyboard short: Ferrarius Tries to Sing
 
Dubai Call Girl Number # 00971588312479 # Call Girl Number In Dubai # (UAE)
Dubai Call Girl Number # 00971588312479 # Call Girl Number In Dubai # (UAE)Dubai Call Girl Number # 00971588312479 # Call Girl Number In Dubai # (UAE)
Dubai Call Girl Number # 00971588312479 # Call Girl Number In Dubai # (UAE)
 
Young⚡Call Girls in Uttam Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
Young⚡Call Girls in Uttam Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort ServiceYoung⚡Call Girls in Uttam Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
Young⚡Call Girls in Uttam Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
 
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 892311...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 892311...Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 892311...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow 10k @ I'm VIP Independent Escorts Girls 892311...
 
Young⚡Call Girls in Lajpat Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
Young⚡Call Girls in Lajpat Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort ServiceYoung⚡Call Girls in Lajpat Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
Young⚡Call Girls in Lajpat Nagar Delhi >༒9667401043 Escort Service
 
Lucknow 💋 Call Girls in Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payment 892311...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girls in Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payment 892311...Lucknow 💋 Call Girls in Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payment 892311...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girls in Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payment 892311...
 
Call girls in Kanpur - 9761072362 with room service
Call girls in Kanpur - 9761072362 with room serviceCall girls in Kanpur - 9761072362 with room service
Call girls in Kanpur - 9761072362 with room service
 
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow Phone No 8923113531 Elite Escort Service Avail...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow Phone No 8923113531 Elite Escort Service Avail...Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow Phone No 8923113531 Elite Escort Service Avail...
Lucknow 💋 Call Girl in Lucknow Phone No 8923113531 Elite Escort Service Avail...
 
Lucknow 💋 Female Escorts Service in Lucknow | Service-oriented sexy call girl...
Lucknow 💋 Female Escorts Service in Lucknow | Service-oriented sexy call girl...Lucknow 💋 Female Escorts Service in Lucknow | Service-oriented sexy call girl...
Lucknow 💋 Female Escorts Service in Lucknow | Service-oriented sexy call girl...
 
Hazratganj / Call Girl in Lucknow - Phone 🫗 8923113531 ☛ Escorts Service at 6...
Hazratganj / Call Girl in Lucknow - Phone 🫗 8923113531 ☛ Escorts Service at 6...Hazratganj / Call Girl in Lucknow - Phone 🫗 8923113531 ☛ Escorts Service at 6...
Hazratganj / Call Girl in Lucknow - Phone 🫗 8923113531 ☛ Escorts Service at 6...
 
Lucknow 💋 High Profile Call Girls in Lucknow - Book 8923113531 Call Girls Ava...
Lucknow 💋 High Profile Call Girls in Lucknow - Book 8923113531 Call Girls Ava...Lucknow 💋 High Profile Call Girls in Lucknow - Book 8923113531 Call Girls Ava...
Lucknow 💋 High Profile Call Girls in Lucknow - Book 8923113531 Call Girls Ava...
 
Lucknow 💋 Escort Service in Lucknow (Adult Only) 8923113531 Escort Service 2...
Lucknow 💋 Escort Service in Lucknow  (Adult Only) 8923113531 Escort Service 2...Lucknow 💋 Escort Service in Lucknow  (Adult Only) 8923113531 Escort Service 2...
Lucknow 💋 Escort Service in Lucknow (Adult Only) 8923113531 Escort Service 2...
 
RAJKOT CALL GIRL 76313*77252 CALL GIRL IN RAJKOT
RAJKOT CALL GIRL 76313*77252 CALL GIRL IN RAJKOTRAJKOT CALL GIRL 76313*77252 CALL GIRL IN RAJKOT
RAJKOT CALL GIRL 76313*77252 CALL GIRL IN RAJKOT
 
Call Girl Service In Dubai #$# O56521286O #$# Dubai Call Girls
Call Girl Service In Dubai #$# O56521286O #$# Dubai Call GirlsCall Girl Service In Dubai #$# O56521286O #$# Dubai Call Girls
Call Girl Service In Dubai #$# O56521286O #$# Dubai Call Girls
 
Authentic # 00971556872006 # Hot Call Girls Service in Dubai By International...
Authentic # 00971556872006 # Hot Call Girls Service in Dubai By International...Authentic # 00971556872006 # Hot Call Girls Service in Dubai By International...
Authentic # 00971556872006 # Hot Call Girls Service in Dubai By International...
 
Bobbie goods coloring book 81 pag_240127_163802.pdf
Bobbie goods coloring book 81 pag_240127_163802.pdfBobbie goods coloring book 81 pag_240127_163802.pdf
Bobbie goods coloring book 81 pag_240127_163802.pdf
 
Indira Nagar Lucknow #Call Girls Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payme...
Indira Nagar Lucknow #Call Girls Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payme...Indira Nagar Lucknow #Call Girls Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payme...
Indira Nagar Lucknow #Call Girls Lucknow ₹7.5k Pick Up & Drop With Cash Payme...
 

Comparison o f self beliefs for predicting student achievement

  • 1. The Journal of Educational Research, 105:336–352, 2012 Copyright C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC ISSN: 0022-0671 print / 1940-0675 online DOI:10.1080/00220671.2011.627401 Comparison of Self-Beliefs for Predicting Student Motivation and Achievement MIMI BONG CATHERINE CHO HYUN SEON AHN HYE JIN KIM Korea University, Republic of Korea ABSTRACT. The authors examined whether self-concept, self-efficacy, and self-esteem show differential predictive util- ity for academic achievement across age groups and domains. More specifically, the relationships of 3 self-constructs with achievement were examined in mathematics for elementary school students and mathematics and language arts for mid- dle school students in Korea. Task value and test anxiety were hypothesized to mediate these relationships. Consistent with previous reports, domain-specific self-constructs such as self-efficacy and self-concept were better predictors of task value and achievement than was general self-esteem. Task value and test anxiety significantly mediated only the rela- tionships of self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale to achievement. These domain-specific relationships tended to be stronger for middle school than elementary school stu- dents and in mathematics than language arts. Keywords: domain specificity, self-concept, self-efficacy, self- esteem, task value, test anxiety Educational theorists and researchers have long ex- plored how perceived self-beliefs predict individ- uals’ motivation and behavior by examining their effects on goal setting, task choice, strategy use, and achieve- ment in academic settings (e.g., Bachman & O’Malley, 1977; Bandura & Schunk, 1981; Hansford & Hattie, 1982; Ludwig & Maehr, 1967; Marsh, Byrne, & Shavelson, 1988). Ban- dura’s social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1977, 1986; Pajares, 1996), for instance, characterizes the relationship between perceived self-beliefs and personal behavior as reciprocal de- terminism because individuals’ beliefs about themselves al- low them to change their thoughts, emotions, and behaviors and these in turn change their beliefs. Among different types of self-constructs, self-concept, self-efficacy, and self-esteem have perhaps been most actively studied in the field of education and motivation (Bandura, 1977; Bong & Skaalvik, 2003; Marsh, Walker, & Debus, 1991). Despite quite a few attempts to arrive at conceptual distinctions between these self-constructs (e.g., Bandura, 1997; Bong & Skaalvik, 2003; Pajares, 1996), many researchers still struggle when trying to separate them empirically (Shavelson & Bolus, 1982). This difficulty results in mixed reports in the literature regarding discriminability between these self-constructs and their roles in academic settings. No consensus has been reached regarding the relative predictive utility of self-concept and self-efficacy on academic achievement. The relative importance of different self-beliefs in affecting task value and anxiety in achievement situations is also unknown. We tried to address these questions directly in the present investigation by comparing the utility of self-esteem, self- concept, and self-efficacy beliefs for predicting student achievement. On the basis of existing literature, we also tested the hypothesis that task value and anxiety would me- diate the relationship between self-constructs and achieve- ment. By including samples from two different school levels and assessing self-beliefs across two different subject matter areas, we aimed to help clarify the issue of relative predictive power of the three self-constructs for students’ achievement and motivation in school. Conceptual Definitions of Self-Constructs Self-concept refers to an individual’s perceptions of the self that are formed through experiences and evaluative feed- back received from significant others (Shavelson, Hubner, & Stanton, 1976). It concerns self-descriptions and self- evaluations and hence is believed to consist of cognitive and affective components (Covington, 1984). Stated dif- ferently, self-concept represents an individual’s knowledge about him- or herself along with emotional reactions toward Address correspondence to Mimi Bong, Korea University, Department of Education, Anam-dong, Seongbuk-gu, Seoul, 136–701 Republic of Korea. (E-mail: mimibong@korea.ac.kr)
  • 2. The Journal of Educational Research 337 the cognitively recognized competencies and attributes he or she possesses (Bong & Clark, 1999). Previous research in- dicates that affective responses are most likely when making comparisons of an individual’s competencies and attributes to certain standards and norms (Ruble, Parsons, & Ross, 1976). Therefore, self-concept formation is heavily influ- enced by how individuals evaluate themselves in comparison with others and how they emotionally react to the results of these evaluations. Self-efficacy refers to an individual’s subjective convic- tions in his or her capability to organize and execute ac- tions that are required to achieve a desired outcome in a given context (Bandura, 1977). Academic self-efficacy more specifically refers to the belief that an individual can suc- cessfully accomplish given academic tasks at designated levels (Schunk, 1991). Beliefs of self-efficacy are formed through an individual’s own mastery experiences, vicarious experiences, social persuasion, and physiological reactions (Bandura, 1977, 1997). Whereas self-concept encompasses cognitive and affective components, self-efficacy primarily involves cognitive appraisals of one’s personal capabilities (Bong & Skaalvik, 2003; Pajares, 1996). Pajares and Schunk (2002) suggested that the two self-beliefs develop through the processes of asking different questions. Self-efficacy de- velops by asking questions about capability (e.g., “Can I write a logical and persuasive piece on the causes of the Vietnam War?”). In contrast, self-concept is formed by asking ques- tions about feelings and being (e.g., “How do I feel about my- self as a writer?”). Answering self-efficacy questions reveals students’ confidence toward successfully performing a task, whereas answering self-concept questions discloses their pos- itive or negative views of themselves and their competencies. Another marked distinction between self-concept and self-efficacy is the type of self-appraisal employed. Prior expe- rience, whether it is an individual’s own enactive experience or vicarious experience, is essential in establishing percepts of self-efficacy. Judgments of success or failure are made pri- marily against mastery criteria but social comparative infor- mation could be given more weight when the task is novel or the criteria for mastery are ambiguous. Also, observing others’ performance provides important information for esti- mating an individual’s probability of success on similar tasks. In such vicarious experiences, students develop efficacy be- liefs by assessing the similarity of the model to themselves and gauging the perceived difficulty of the task (Schunk, 1991). In this sense, self-efficacy can be viewed as reflect- ing more or less a combination of results from criterion- and norm-referenced evaluations, with heavier weight as- signed to the former. In comparison, self-concept is more concerned with an individual’s relative standing in relation to others’ performances (Zimmerman, 1995). It is thus more strongly influenced by social comparative information than is self-efficacy (Bong & Clark, 1999). Compared to self-efficacy and self-concept, self-esteem has been rather loosely defined in the literature. It refers to how individuals feel about themselves and evaluate their global self-worth (Smelser, 1989). More precisely, Rosenberg, Schooler, Schoenbach, and Rosenberg (1995) defined self-esteem as “an individual’s positive or negative attitude toward the self as a totality” (p. 141). On one hand, self-esteem is generally considered as a far less context- specific construct among the three. On the other hand, self-esteem appears more relevant to an individual’s psycho- logical well-being than self-concept or self-efficacy. A num- ber of studies have reported that global self-esteem has shown strong relationships to levels of anxiety (Rosenberg, 1962) and psychological symptoms such as depression and juvenile delinquency (Rosenberg, Schooler, & Schoenbach, 1989). Relationships with Academic Achievement Self-concept, self-efficacy, and self-esteem all have dis- played discernible and independent predictive power for stu- dents’ scholastic attainments in past research, although it is difficult to locate studies that include all three constructs in the same investigation. Self-concept has been a criti- cal component in most theoretical accounts of academic achievement, either as a presumed cause (e.g., Brookover, Thomas, & Paterson, 1964; Marsh, Byrne, & Yeung, 1999) or a byproduct of academic accomplishments (e.g., Calsyn & Kenny, 1977; Skaalvik & Hagtvet, 1990). Likewise, self- efficacy has repeatedly demonstrated itself as a significant predictor of students’ academic performances. Its predictive power is even stronger than that of more objective indexes such as prior achievement levels (Zimmerman, Bandura, & Martinez-Pons, 1992). Self-concept and self-efficacy have also displayed significant relations with various indexes of academic persistence (Hansford & Hattie, 1982; Multon, Brown, & Lent, 1991). Nonetheless, questions still remain as to which of the two self-constructs better represents the very belief that energizes students in learning and performance situations and hence should be the target of enhancement efforts. Whereas stud- ies by Pajares and Miller (1994) and Pietsch, Walker, and Chapman (2003) demonstrated the stronger predictive util- ity of self-efficacy compared with that of self-concept within the same domain, Choi (2005) reported that academic self- concept was a better predictor of academic performance than self-efficacy among college students. Regarding this issue, Bong and Clark (1999) and Bong and Skaalvik (2003) speculated that self-efficacy would bet- ter predict academic achievement than would self-concept. They reasoned that self-efficacy more truthfully mirrors an individual’s achievement history because past mastery ex- periences are the most influential source of self-efficacy in- formation. Moreover, competence judgments in self-efficacy rely largely on the prospect of successful task mastery as an absolute frame of reference. Self-efficacy also incorporates specific features of the performance context, which might af- fect the quality of performance (Bandura, 1997). Therefore, self-efficacy should be able to afford an accurate prediction of subsequent performance.
  • 3. 338 The Journal of Educational Research In contrast, self-concept appears to be more heavily swayed by various frames of reference and often does not exactly correspond to an individual’s achievement history. For example, students’ verbal and mathematics self-concepts demonstrate a nonsignificant or near-zero correlation with each other, even when their achievements in the two domains show a strong positive correlation (Marsh et al., 1988). Marsh (1986, 1990) explained this pattern to be a result of simultaneous operation of internal and external comparisons in his internal–external model of academic self-concept. When judging their academic self-concept in a given area, students compare their competence in that area to that of their peers. This process is called an external comparison, which leads to a positive correlation between the verbal and mathematics self-concepts. At the same time, students also compare their competence in a given area to their competence in other areas. Most typically, students compare their competence in verbal areas to their competence in mathematics-related areas and vice versa. This process is called an internal comparison, which yields a negative correlation between the verbal and mathematics self-concepts. Such concurrent utilization of the internal and external frames of reference nullifies the result ob- tained from each other, thereby making the correlation between the verbal and mathematics self-concepts to be nonsignificant and near zero. Furthermore, academic self-concepts of equally able stu- dents differ greatly, depending on the ability of their peers in the reference group. The self-concept of a student attend- ing a low-average school is higher than the self-concept of a student attending a high-average school, even when these two students are performing at the same level. This social comparison effect is called the big-fish-little-pond-effect by Marsh (1987, 1991). All these results point to the evalua- tive nature of self-concept (Bong & Clark, 1999; Bong & Skaalvik, 2003). Self-concept should thus be able to predict motivational constructs with strong evaluative and affective connotations better than self-efficacy. Findings are even more inconsistent when it comes to the predictive utility of self-esteem for students’ academic achievement. Self-esteem is significantly associated with achievement in some studies (Pullman & Allik, 2008) but shows negligible correlations with achievement in others (Byrne & Shavelson, 1986; Hansford & Hattie, 1982). Rosenberg et al. (1995) concluded that self-esteem appears to have little or no effect in enhancing academic perfor- mance among high school students. Still, some have argued that self-esteem plays a critical role in children’s academic functioning (Pullman & Allik, 2008). According to these researchers, an individual’s having positive regard toward him- or herself is particularly beneficial for young children because they have not yet developed stable perceptions of competence across domains. This claim contradicts the recent trend in self research that mainly focuses on the role of domain-specific self- constructs in academic situations. Especially for adolescents, it is questionable whether nurturing global self-esteem would yield tangible benefits in academic attainments above and beyond what could be explained by a positive academic self- concept or a strong sense of academic self-efficacy. It is in- teresting to note, compared with the number of studies that have examined the relative power of self-concept and self- efficacy for predicting diverse achievement-related outcomes (e.g., Pajares & Miller, 1994; Pietsch et al., 2003; Skaalvik & Rankin, 1995), few studies have compared the predictive utility of self-esteem to that of other self-constructs. Results of the present research are expected to fill this void in the literature. Relationships with Task Value and Anxiety In addition to specifying predictive relationships between the three self-beliefs and academic achievement, we also an- ticipated task value and anxiety to mediate those relation- ships. The expectancy-value theory asserts that expectancies for success and task value are important predictors of moti- vation and achievement. Expectancy for success refers to the subjective estimation of the probability that an individual’s performance leads to successful consequences, and task value refers to an incentive for engagement in particular tasks or activities (Eccles, Wigfield, Harold, & Blumenfeld, 1993; Wigfield et al., 1997). Many researchers operationally define task value as comprising dimensions of interest (i.e., intrinsic value), perceived importance (i.e., attainment value), and perceived usefulness (i.e., utility value; Berndt & Miller, 1990; Bong, 2001a, 2001b; Pokay & Blumenfeld, 1990). Task value is an important predictor of academic choices, self-reported effort in class, and intention to persist on a task (Hulleman, Durik, Schweigert, & Harackiewicz, 2008). Confidence increases students’ interest in and task value for an activity, which, in turn, enables students to spend more time on the activity and perform better to meet higher stan- dards (Eccles et al., 1993; Eccles & Wigfield, 1995). There- fore, we hypothesized that task value would be an important mediator linking self-perceived competence to achievement. Previous research further suggests that emotions and affec- tive responses are important correlates of various self-beliefs (Bandura, 1989; Scherer, Schorr, & Johnstone, 2001). According to Ruthig et al. (2008), distinct patterns of emo- tional experiences are associated with high personal control. As heightened senses of self-efficacy and positive self- concept denote greater perceived control, this suggests that certain academic emotions such as anxiety are associated with students’ self-perceptions in learning contexts (Pekrun, Elliot, & Maier, 2006). Anxiety represents anticipatory fear accompanied by physiological arousal when ruminating over possible adversities (Bandura, 1988). It is known to be heavily influenced by how individuals conceive of specific situational contexts in which arousal occurs (Hunt, Cole, & Reis, 1958). Studies examining how self-perceptions affect anxiety indicate that positive self-concept and strong
  • 4. The Journal of Educational Research 339 self-efficacy help lower test anxiety (Bandalos, Yates, & Thorndike-Christ, 1995; Zeidner & Schleyer, 1999). There is evidence that certain affective factors might be more strongly associated with self-concept than self-efficacy. For example, Skaalvik and Rankin (1995) observed that stu- dents’ anxiety showed stronger correlation with self-concept than self-efficacy in mathematics and verbal domains. Pa- jares and Miller (1994) also reported that students’ anxiety levels were more strongly related to self-concept than self- efficacy. These results make sense, considering self-concept includes affective components and self-efficacy does not. On the basis of these findings, we hypothesized that test anxi- ety would negatively mediate the relationship between the three self-constructs and achievement. We also predicted that self-concept would demonstrate stronger relationships with test anxiety compared with self-efficacy. Domain and Age Differences The self-constructs’ relationships with other variables may also fluctuate depending on the domain characteristics and age of students. Academic self-concept is known to be clearly differentiated across mathematics and verbal areas, despite the strong positive correlation usually observed between mathematics and verbal achievements (Marsh et al., 1988). The frequently nonsignificant and near-zero correlation be- tween mathematics and verbal self-concepts contrasts with significant positive correlation between mathematics and verbal self-efficacy (Bong, 1997, 2001a). This hints at the possibility that the degree of the self-constructs’ relative pre- dictive power may differ across academic domains. In fact, Bong (1998) reported that the relations of achievement in- dexes to domain-specific self-concept and self-efficacy were much stronger in mathematics-related subject areas than verbal subject areas. Because students develop unique under- standings of not only themselves but also academic domains (Stodolski, Salk, & Glaessner, 1991), differential relations may exist among their self-perceptions and their motivation and achievement across different subject domains. Age may be another factor that moderates the rela- tionships of self-constructs with other constructs. It has been well established in the developmental literature that younger children evaluate themselves a lot more generously than do older students, to a degree that is not justified by their objective performance levels (e.g., Parsons & Ruble, 1977). This overestimation of competence by children is attributed to their lack of critical thinking and analytic reasoning ability and also their tendency to equate effort with ability (Harter, 1999). As children age, they begin to cognitively and more logically evaluate their achievement- related experiences and information and discriminate between their effort and ability. Hence, older students build more negative but accurate views of their academic capabilities (Eccles et al., 1993; Kun, Parsons, & Ruble, 1974; Parsons & Ruble, 1977). Reflecting these changes, students’ ratings of their own academic competence reach the highest point in the early elementary school years and steadily decline afterward until early adolescence (Eccles, Midgley, & Adler, 1984; Marsh, Barnes, Cairns, & Tidman, 1984; Stipek & Tannatt, 1984). Further, this decline is most dramatic when students transition to middle school (Simmons et al., 1979; Stipek & Mac Iver, 1989). By including elementary and middle school students in the present sample, we examined whether age or school-level difference moderated the relationships of self-constructs to other variables. Considering that older students’ self- perceptions represent more realistic views of their own com- petencies, we hypothesized that self-constructs in general would demonstrate stronger predictive relationships with academic outcomes among the middle school students. We further hypothesized that this predictive gain would be more visible with domain-specific self-constructs such as self- concept and self-efficacy than global self-esteem. Overview of the Present Hypotheses In sum, we compared the utility of self-esteem, self- concept, and self-efficacy for predicting academic achieve- ments of elementary and middle school students. We also tested whether task value and anxiety mediated the rela- tionships between self-perceptions and achievement. In ad- dition, domain- and age-related differences in predictive re- lations among the constructs were examined. Specifically, we generated and tested the following eight hypotheses: Hypothesis 1 (H1): Domain-specific self-efficacy and domain- specific self-concept would predict academic achievement better than general self-esteem. H2: Between self-concept and self-efficacy, self-efficacy would predict academic achievement better than self- concept. H3: Between self-concept and self-efficacy, self-concept would predict task value better than self-efficacy. H4: Between self-concept and self-efficacy, self-concept would predict anxiety better than self-efficacy. H5: Task value would mediate the relationships between self-constructs and academic achievement. H6: Anxiety would mediate the relationships between self- constructs and academic achievement. H7: The relationships between self-constructs and academic achievement would be stronger among middle school stu- dents than elementary school students. H8: The relationships between self-constructs and academic achievement would be different across mathematics and language arts. Figure 1 shows the structure of our theoretical model. Be- cause the elementary school participants responded only to the mathematics surveys, Hypothesis 8 could not be tested among the elementary school students. Therefore, we de- cided to report our results in two studies to minimize po- tential confusion due to the difference in the types of data collected. In Study 1, we presented results regarding the
  • 5. 340 The Journal of Educational Research FIGURE 1. Structure of the hypothesized theoretical model. relative predictive utility of self-constructs (i.e., Hypothe- ses 1–4), the mediating role of task value and anxiety in self–achievement relationships (i.e., Hypotheses 5 and 6), and age-related differences in construct relations (Hypoth- esis 7). In Study 2, we reported results regarding domain differences in construct relations across mathematics and language arts (i.e., Hypothesis 8) with only the middle school participants. Method Participants and Procedure Participants were 234 elementary and 512 middle school students. Korean elementary schools offer 6 years of school- ing before students enter middle schools. Therefore, fifth- and sixth-grade students are regarded as seniors in elemen- tary schools. Middle and high schools each offer 3 years of schooling. All participants were attending public schools located in Seoul and its vicinity in Korea. The regions in which the schools were located were considered largely mid- dle class in terms of socioeconomic status. The students participated in a larger research project on Korean students’ motivation and learning. There is another report based on the data from the same research project (Bong, 2009), but that article deals with different constructs (i.e., achievement goal orientations) and addresses a completely different set of research questions. Only the data relevant to the present research were analyzed in this study. Surveys were administered during regular classroom hours in the middle of the semester, a little less than 2 months into the academic year. Students were told that participation was voluntary and no one outside the research team would have access to their individual responses. Elementary school students responded only to the surveys tapping into their motivation and learning in mathematics, whereas middle school students responded to the surveys for mathematics and language arts. Elementary school teachers’ ratings of their students’ mathematics competence were obtained with the survey data. Middle school students’ final examination scores in mathematics were obtained from the schools at the end of the semester. Seven students were excluded from the elementary school sample before analysis because they either did not have the achievement data or failed to answer a substantial portion of the survey. The final elementary school sample included 110 fifth-grade students and 117 sixth-grade students (125 boys, 102 girls). Their ages ranged between 10 and 12 years with an average age of 11.3 years (SD = .68). Five students were excluded from the middle school sample before analysis for the same reasons. The final breakdown of the middle school sample was 137 seventh-grade students, 244 eighth-grade students, and 126 ninth-grade students. The ages ranged between 12 and 15 years with an average age of 13.8 years (SD = .78). Measures Students were asked to rate each item on a 5-point Likert- type scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Except for self-esteem, all variables were assessed in reference to the domains of mathematics and language arts. Self-esteem. General self-esteem was measured by eight items from the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire (ASDQ; e.g., “I have a lot to be proud of ”; Marsh, 1992). Reliability of the scale was acceptable in the present study with Cronbach’s alpha values of .84 and .81 for the elemen- tary and middle school samples, respectively. Self-concept. Six items from the ASDQ (Marsh, 1992) were adopted (e.g., “Compared to others my age I am good at math”). Reliability coefficients (Cronbach’s α) were .88 in mathematics for the elementary school students, and .87 and .75 in mathematics and language arts, respectively, for the middle school students. Self-efficacy. We assessed self-efficacy in two different ways because of their prevalent use in the literature. How self-efficacy is assessed is a critical issue in its own right (Pajares, 1996), yet empirical comparability of the scores obtained by different assessment methods has rarely been tested (Bong & Hocevar, 2002). One scale included four items adapted from Bandura’s (1986) Self-Efficacy scale (e.g., “I can get more than ‘80’ in the math exam”) and the other consisted of five items from the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ; e.g., “I’m confident I can do an excellent job on the assignments and tests in this course”; Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & McKeachie, 1993). For the Bandura-type Self-Efficacy Scale, Cronbach’s alphas were .91 in mathematics for the elementary school students, and .91 and .86 in mathematics and language arts, respec- tively, for the middle school students. For the MSLQ, Cron- bach’s alphas were .91 in mathematics for the elementary school students, and .88 and .86 in mathematics and lan- guage arts, respectively, for the middle school students. Task value. Task value consisted of three items, each ask- ing about students’ perceptions of importance, utility, and
  • 6. The Journal of Educational Research 341 interest in the subject matter area (Bong, 2001a, 2001b). A sample item read, “I think math is a useful subject.” Relia- bility coefficients were a bit low with Cronbach’s alphas of .52 in mathematics for the elementary school sample, and .64 and .60 in mathematics and language arts, respectively, for the middle school sample. Test anxiety. Three items were adapted from the MSLQ (e.g. “I worry a great deal about math tests”; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990). The reliability coefficients (Cronbach’s α) for test anxiety were .57 in mathematics for the elementary school sample, and .63 and .61 in mathematics and language arts, respectively, for the middle school sample. Achievement indices. For the elementary school partici- pants, their homeroom teachers rated each student’s level of mathematics performance on a 3-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (below average) to 3 (above average). We obtained indices of the elementary school students’ mathe- matics achievements this way because there was no official school-wide mathematics examination and the national ed- ucation policy at the time of the survey dictated that grades not be assigned for elementary school students. For the mid- dle school participants, their scores on the first-semester final examinations in the subjects of mathematics and Korean, ranging between 0 and 100, were used as achievement in- dices. To prevent estimation problems associated with such a large discrepancy in the measurement units between the middle school achievement scores (i.e., 0–100) and other variables (i.e., 1–3 or 1–5), we transformed the middle school students’ achievement data into z scores before analysis. Results Study 1: Mediators and Age Differences Table 1 shows descriptive statistics among the variables for the elementary and middle school participants. Several findings are noteworthy. The middle school students’ self- concept scores on the ASDQ and self-efficacy scores on the MSLQ (Ms = 3.06 and 3.36, SDs = .90 and .93) were signif- icantly lower than those of the elementary school students (Ms = 3.30 and 3.57, SDs = .88 and .88), ts(732) = 3.32 and 2.84, ps < .01. The general self-esteem scores were also significantly lower among the middle school students (M = 3.19, SD = .81) than the elementary school students (M = 3.39, SD = .84), t(732) = 2.96, p < .01. Test anxiety showed the opposite trend such that the middle school students ex- pressed significantly higher levels of test anxiety (M = 3.04, SD = .98) compared with those of the elementary school students (M = 2.85, SD = .93), t(732) = −2.40, p < .05. On the contrary, the middle school students’ self-efficacy scores on the Bandura-type scale (M = 3.59, SD = 1.10) did not differ from those of the elementary school coun- terparts (M = 3.51, SD = 1.09), t(731) = −0.88, p = ns. Moreover, the elementary and middle school students’ self- efficacy scores on the Bandura-type scale were associated with larger standard deviations (SDs = 1.09 and 1.10 for the elementary and middle school sample, respectively) com- pared with the standard deviations of the other scales. Tests of measurement models. Because covariance struc- ture modeling requires complete data on study variables, all missing values were replaced with the expected maximiza- tion (EM) method before analysis (Schreiber, Stage, King, Nora, & Barlow, 2006). The frequencies of missing values ranged from 1 (0.2%) to a maximum of 12 cases (2.4%) per variable, which is judged acceptable. Before fitting our theoretical model to the empirical data, we tested the measurement models first by performing confir- matory factor analyses (CFAs) to check the factor structure. We used AMOS 7.0 for all our CFA and structural equa- tion modeling (SEM; Arbuckle, 2006). Because chi-square statistics are known to be sensitive to sample size (Kline, 2005, p. 146), we applied the root mean square error of ap- proximation (RMSEA) ≤ .06 (Hu & Bentler, 1999) and the TABLE 1. Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Coefficients Among Latent Variables for Elementary and Middle School Students in Mathematics Elementary Middle Variable M SD M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1. ASDQ 3.30a 0.88 3.06b 0.90 — .91 .84 .39 .74 −.57 .67 2. MSLQ 3.57a 0.88 3.36b 0.93 .96 — .78 .44 .81 −.45 .60 3. BDR 3.51 1.09 3.59 1.10 .78 .76 — .36 .62 −.45 .57 4. GSE 3.39a 0.84 3.19b 0.81 .53 .56 .43 — .22 −.47 .26 5. Task value 3.72 0.80 3.60 0.89 .77 .81 .56 .36 — –.35 .45 6. Test anxiety 2.85a 0.93 3.04b 0.98 −.67 −.63 −.59 −.64 −.49 — −.41 7. Achievement 2.24 0.80 70.42 22.48 .61 .53 .56 .32 .45 −.51 — Note. Correlation coefficients from elementary school in mathematics are below the diagonal; those from middle school in mathematics are above the diagonal. All coefficients are significant at p < .01. Different subscripts denote statistically significant difference at p < .05. ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem.
  • 7. 342 The Journal of Educational Research Tucker-Lewis index (TLI) and comparative fit index (CFI) ≥ .90 (Bentler, 1990; Tucker & Lewis, 1973) as the criteria for evaluating model fit. The CFA models included general self-esteem, math- ematics self-concept, MSLQ mathematics self-efficacy, Bandura-type mathematics self-efficacy, mathematics task value, mathematics test anxiety, and mathematics achieve- ment. The model exhibited acceptable goodness-of-fit indices for the elementary, χ2 (355, N = 227) = 678.602, p < .001, CFI = .916, TLI = .904, RMSEA = .064; and middle school samples, χ2 (355, N = 507) = 1,027.119, p < .001, CFI = .916, TLI = .904, RMSEA = .061. All factor loadings were statistically significant at p < .001. As can be seen in Table 1, there existed differences in variable correlation between the two age groups. When we examined the correlation coefficients among the four self- constructs, not much difference was observed in how the domain-specific self-constructs such as self-concept and self- efficacy correlated with each other across the two groups. However, general self-esteem, which was assessed globally, showed substantial drop in its correlation with other self- constructs in the middle school sample compared with the elementary school sample. The correlation coefficients of the general self-esteem factor reduced from phi values of .53 to .39 with self-concept, .56 to .44 with MSLQ self-efficacy, and .43 to .36 with Bandura-type self-efficacy for the middle school students, respectively. The correlation coefficients between general self-esteem and other variables also decreased considerably (φs = .36 vs. .22 with task value, −.64 vs. −.47 with test anxiety, and .32 vs. .26 with achievement in the elementary and mid- dle school samples, respectively). This was not the case for the domain-specific self-constructs such as self-concept and self-efficacy, as they demonstrated correlation coeffi- cients of similar magnitude across the elementary and mid- dle school samples with other variables, with the exception of test anxiety. The correlation coefficients between test anxiety and self-constructs uniformly dropped from the ele- mentary to the middle school sample with self-concept (φs = −.67 vs. −.57), MSLQ self-efficacy (φs = −.63 vs. −.45), and Bandura-type self-efficacy (φs = −.59 vs. −.45). Some of the self-constructs were too highly correlated to ensure their discriminant validity. In particular, the self- concept and the MSLQ self-efficacy factors correlated too highly to each other in both samples (φs = .96 and .91 in the elementary and middle school samples, respectively). This was surprising because these correlation coefficients were even stronger than those between the two self-efficacy factors assessed by different methods, the MSLQ and the Bandura-type scale (φs = .76 in the elementary and .78 in the middle school sample). Therefore, to examine the em- pirical structure of our self-constructs, we conducted CFAs on two separate models. The first model was composed of four factors including MSLQ self-efficacy, Bandura-type self- efficacy, self-concept, and general self-esteem. In the second model, we merged the two self-efficacy factors together to have self-efficacy, self-concept, and general self-esteem. In the elementary school sample, the first model revealed model fit of TLI = .924, CFI = .933, and RMSEA = .066, whereas the second model showed lower fit of TLI = .867, CFI = .882, and RMSEA = .087. Results from the middle school sample likewise revealed that in mathematics, the first model displayed a satisfactory model fit of TLI = .922, CFI = .931, and RMSEA = .064, whereas the second model dis- played weaker fit of TLI = .874, CFI = .888, and RMSEA = .081. In language arts, the first model displayed fit of TLI = .900, CFI = .912, and RMSEA = .064, whereas the second model showed again decreased fit of TLI = .860, CFI = .876, and RMSEA = .076. Our CFA results, along with high cor- relations especially among the MSLQ self-efficacy, Bandura- type self-efficacy, and self-concept factors, suggest multi- collinearity among the self-constructs. We thus examined each self-construct separately in all subsequent analyses to avoid empirical problems associated with multicollinearity. SEM. The relative predictive utility of the self-constructs for achievement (Hypotheses 1 and 2), task value (Hypoth- esis 3), and anxiety (Hypothesis 4) were compared by SEM. The role of task value (Hypothesis 5) and anxiety (Hypoth- esis 6) as mediators of the self–achievement relationship was also examined. Figure 1 shows the theoretical model we specified for testing our hypotheses. All models displayed satisfactory to marginally acceptable fit to the empirical data analyzed with AMOS 7.0 (Arbuckle, 2006), as reported in Table 2 (i.e., “original” models). Al- though there was some room for improvement for the general self-esteem models, we did not add any additional path to improve the overall fit because doing so might obstruct direct and more accurate comparison of the results. Figure 2 shows the results of structural equation modeling with the path coefficients separately estimated for each self-construct. All self-constructs except for general self-esteem and MSLQ self- efficacy for the elementary school sample proved to be signif- icant positive predictors of students’ achievements in math- ematics. General self-esteem was not a significant predictor of middle school students’ mathematics achievements ei- ther, providing support for Hypothesis 1 that domain-specific self-efficacy and domain-specific self-concept would predict academic achievement better than general self-esteem. For the elementary school sample, self-concept demon- strated the strongest path to mathematics achievement with γ = .52, followed by Bandura-type self-efficacy with γ = .33. MSLQ self-efficacy and general self-esteem were not able to predict elementary school students’ mathematics achievements. For the middle school sample, self-concept again exhibited the strongest path with γ = .69, followed by MSLQ self-efficacy (γ = .61) and Bandura-type self-efficacy (γ = .40). Hypothesis 2, self-efficacy would predict aca- demic achievement better than self-concept, was thus not supported.
  • 8. The Journal of Educational Research 343 TABLE 2. Goodness-of-Fit Indices for Elementary and Middle School Models in Study 1 Model CFI TLI RMSEA χ2 (df) χ2 ( df) ASDQ Elementary Original .953 .938 .068 101.981(50) 0.026(1) Alternative .952 .936 .069 101.955(49) Middle Original .944 .927 .073 185.988(50) 0.979(1) Alternative .944 .925 .074 185.009(49) MSLQ Elementary Original .940 .917 .078 95.506(40) 0.014(1) Alternative .939 .913 .080 95.492(39) Middle Original .954 .936 .072 143.927(40) 25.379(0) Alternative .942 .921 .080 169.306(40) BDR Elementary Original .962 .943 .077 70.149(30) 5.778(0) Alternative .957 .935 .082 75.927(30) Middle Original .965 .948 .072 109.248(30) 38.526(0) Alternative .948 .922 .088 147.774(30) GSE Elementary Original .907 .883 .073 158.679(72) 10.447(0) Alternative .919 .897 .068 148.232(72) Middle Original .877 .844 .080 306.429(72) 18.426(0) Alternative .887 .857 .077 288.003(72) Note. Ns = 227 (elementary students) and 507 (middle school students). CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem. All domain-specific self-constructs as well as general self- esteem related significantly to task value and test anxiety in mathematics for the elementary and middle school sam- ples. As hypothesized, self-constructs displayed positive re- lationships with task value and negative relationships with test anxiety. MSLQ self-efficacy demonstrated the strongest predictive paths to task value in both age groups, followed by self-concept, Bandura-type self-efficacy, and general self- esteem. The relationships of self-concept and MSLQ self- efficacy with task value were so strong as to suspect multi- collinearity among the constructs. For example, the absence of significant paths from task value to achievement in these models likely has been due to the strong correlation between the self-construct and task value factors. We thus decided to exclude task value from Study 2. Whereas the paths from MSLQ self-efficacy, self-concept, and general self-esteem to anxiety were of similar magnitude in the elementary school sample with gamma values of −.62, −.66, and −.66, respectively, the path from self-concept to anxiety was noticeably stronger in the middle school sam- ple with a gamma value of −.57. Therefore, Hypothesis 3, self-concept would predict task value better than self- efficacy, and Hypothesis 4, self-concept would predict anxi- ety better than self-efficacy, received only partial support. The hypothesized mediating roles of task value (Hypoth- esis 5) and test anxiety (Hypothesis 6) were supported with respect to general self-esteem and self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale for the middle school sample. Whereas Bandura-type mathematics self-efficacy showed direct pre- dictive paths and indirect paths via task value and test anx- iety on middle school students’ mathematics achievement, general self-esteem only demonstrated indirect predictive power on achievement through task value and test anxiety in both age groups. Therefore, the theoretical model with hypothesized mediators received clear support only with re- spect to Bandura-type self-efficacy in the middle school sam- ple. Whereas task value significantly mediated the predic- tive relation of Bandura-type self-efficacy to mathematics achievement for the middle school students, it failed to do so for the elementary school students.
  • 9. 344 The Journal of Educational Research FIGURE 2. Standardized regression weights for the elementary and middle school models in Study 1. Coefficients before the slash are from elementary school; those after the slash are from middle school. ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem. None of the path coefficients are significantly different between the elementary and middle school samples. ∗ p < .05. ∗∗ p < .01. ∗∗∗ p < .001. Tests of invariance across ages. In general, the relation- ships of self-constructs to task value and anxiety were either similar or slightly reduced in magnitude from the elemen- tary to the middle school sample (see Figure 2). In particular, all self-constructs demonstrated much stronger relationships with anxiety for the elementary school students than the middle school students regardless of the type and the degree of domain specificity. The difference was relatively greater with regard to MSLQ self-efficacy (γ s = −.62 vs. −.45) and general self-esteem (γ s = −.66 vs. −.49 for the elementary and middle school samples, respectively). The predictive paths to achievements, in contrast, became stronger in the middle school sample with respect to all domain-specific self-constructs. All domain-specific self-constructs displayed stronger relationships with middle school students’ achieve- ments than elementary school students’ achievements. The difference was more pronounced with MSLQ self-efficacy (γ s = .27 [p = ns] vs. .61) compared with self-concept (γ s = .52 vs. .69) or Bandura-type self-efficacy (γ s = .33 vs. .40, for the elementary and the middle school samples, respectively). To more systematically address age differences in con- struct relations, we performed multigroup analyses by im- posing cross-group equality (i.e., invariance) constraints on the parameters between the two samples. Testing the struc- tural invariance across groups should be performed in a two-step approach. First, when testing the metric invari- ance (i.e., equality of the measurement aspect of the model across groups), it should be tested that all paths in the model are invariant except for the structural path (MacCallum, Roznowski, & Reith, 1994). If the model fit in this stage is acceptable, the model satisfies cross-validation. The second stage, testing structural invariance across groups, is ensur- ing measurement and structural paths to be invariant across groups by testing the chi-square difference (Byrne, 2001). Whether there exist significant chi-square changes between the unconstrained (i.e., baseline) model and constrained model determines whether there is a group difference in the structural paths. That is, if there is no significant chi-square difference, the constrained model should be preferred be- cause it is more parsimonious. Results showed that the model fit for the constrained model showed only minimal changes from the model fit of the unconstrained model, revealing no significant differ- ences in chi-square statistics (see Table 3) and indicating that the fit of the constrained model is superior to that of the unconstrained model. This suggests that the parame- ters of both samples within the population are equal (Kline, 2005, p. 290). In other words, the predictive power of latent
  • 10. The Journal of Educational Research 345 TABLE 3. Goodness-of-Fit Indices from Multigroup Analyses in Study 1 Model CFI TLI RMSEA χ2 (df) χ2 ( df) ASDQ Unconstrained .945 .933 .050 303.158(108) 3.339(5) Constrained .946 .937 .048 306.497(113) MSLQ Unconstrained .948 .934 .051 249.845(86) 5.924(5) Constrained .948 .937 .050 255.769(91) BDR Unconstrained .960 .945 .053 199.438(66) 2.466(5) Constrained .961 .950 .050 201.904(71) GSE Unconstrained .880 .859 .055 493.423(154) 4.274(5) Constrained .881 .863 .054 497.697(159) Note. Ns = 227 (elementary students) and 507 (middle school students). CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem. factors for middle school and elementary school students did not display significant differences. Therefore, Hypothe- sis 7, the relationships between self-constructs and academic achievement would be stronger among the middle school students than the elementary school students, did not re- ceive support. Tests of alternative models. To determine whether the hy- pothesized model is the most reasonable representation of the empirical data (Bentler, 1990; Schreiber et al., 2006), we compared the hypothesized model with an alternative model by including an additional path linking task value with test anxiety (see Figure 3). Whereas the original model presumed task value and anxiety to be mediators of the self–achievement relationships, the alternative model spec- ified anxiety to be a mediator in the relationships of both self-constructs and task value to achievement. This alter- native model was based on the expectancy-value theory’s claim that expectancies for success, in the form of positive self-perceptions in this study, and task value are important FIGURE 3. Structure of the alternative model. predictors of motivation and achievement (Eccles et al., 1993; Eccles & Wigfield, 1995). Statistical differences in chi squares between models demonstrate that our original model explains data better than the alternative models, with general self-esteem models as possible exceptions (see Table 2). Overall, the majority of the alternative models revealed that the added path from task value to test anxiety was statistically insignificant across grade levels. Because our main interest lied in how the different self-constructs project different patterns on achievement mediated by task value and anxiety and the results attested to empirical superiority of the original model, we decided to keep self-constructs as antecedents in each model. Study 2: Domain Differences Next, we analyzed the middle school data to examine if the predictive utility of self-constructs would differ, depending on the subject matter domain. Table 4 reports descriptive statistics and the latent variable correlations for the middle school participants in mathematics and language arts. Paired-samples t tests revealed statistically significant differences between the domains in self-concept, t(505) = −3.44, p < .001; test anxiety, t(505) = 3.48, p < .001; and achievement, t(506) = −2.82, p < .005. The middle school participants in this study expressed significantly lower levels of self-concept (M = 3.06, SD = .90 vs. M = 3.21, SD = .70) and achievement (M = 70.42, SD = 22.48 vs. M = 72.49, SD = 15.22) and significantly higher levels of test anxiety in mathematics (M = 3.04, SD = .98 vs. M = 2.86, SD = .88) compared with those in language arts. There was no other statistically significant difference in mean scores across the two domains.
  • 11. 346 The Journal of Educational Research TABLE 4. Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Coefficients Among Latent Variables for Middle School Students in Mathematics and Language Arts Mathematics Language arts Variable M SD M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1. ASDQ 3.06a 0.90 3.21b 0.70 — .91 .84 .39 .74 −.57 .67 2. MSLQ 3.36 0.93 3.44 0.83 .97 — .78 .44 .81 −.45 .60 3. BDR 3.59 1.10 3.61 0.98 .80 .77 — .36 .62 −.45 .57 4. GSE 3.19 0.81 (domain-general) .28 .30 .32 — .22 −.47 .26 5. Task value 3.60 0.89 3.61 0.82 .79 .87 .54 .22 — −.35 .45 6. Test anxiety 3.04a 0.98 2.86b 0.88 −.41 −.24 −.31 −.25 −.20 — −.41 7. Achievement 70.42a 22.48 72.49b 15.22 .37 .28 .50 .23 .11 −.17 — Note. Correlation coefficients from language arts are below the diagonal; those from mathematics are above the diagonal. Coefficients greater than .11 and .20 in absolute value are significant at p < .05 and p < .01, respectively. Different subscripts denote a statistically significant difference at p < .05. ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem. Tests of measurement models. As was the case with the mathematics data in Study 1, the middle school data also yielded strong correlation coefficients between the self- concept and the MSLQ self-efficacy factors in the math- ematics (φ = .91) and language arts domains (φ = .97). Also consistent with the findings in Study 1, these correla- tion coefficients were much stronger than those between the two self-efficacy scores, each assessed by the MSLQ and the Bandura-type scale (φs = .78 and .77 in mathematics and language arts, respectively). The strong correlations of self- concept and MSLQ self-efficacy with task value observed in mathematics in Study 1 were also obtained in the domain of language arts (φs = .79 with self-concept and .87 with MSLQ self-efficacy). Due to these high correlations, we ex- cluded the task value factor from our model in Study 2 to avoid multicollinearity problems. The measurement models with all relevant variables in mathematics, χ2 (355, N = 507) = 1,027.119, p < .001, CFI = .916, TLI = .904, RMSEA = .061; and language arts, χ2 (355, N = 507) = 1,108.472, p < .001, CFI = .883; TLI = .866, RMSEA = .065, both yielded adequate fit to the data. All factor loadings were statistically significant at p < .001. The correlation coefficients of self-constructs with other variables were, overall, noticeably stronger in the domain of mathematics than language arts. This trend was particularly conspicuous in their relationships with achievements. With the exception of general self-esteem, which showed similarly weak relationships with achievements in mathematics and language arts (φs = .26 vs. .23, respectively), self-concept (φs = .67 vs. .37), MSLQ self-efficacy (φs = .60 vs. .28), and Bandura-type self-efficacy (φs = .57 vs. .50) all displayed stronger correlations with achievement in the domain of mathematics. Differences in the magnitude of construct correlations across domains were also evident in the self-constructs’ re- lationships with test anxiety. The self-concept, MSLQ self- efficacy, Bandura-type self-efficacy, and general self-esteem factors demonstrated correlation coefficients that ranged be- tween −.57 ≤ φ ≤ −.45 with test anxiety in mathematics. The corresponding correlations obtained from the language arts domain ranged between −.41 ≤ φ ≤ −.24. General self-esteem also exhibited much stronger correlation with self-concept (φs = .39 vs. 28) and MSLQ self-efficacy (φs = .44 vs. .30) in mathematics than language arts. The correla- tion between task value and achievement (φ = .45 vs. .11), test anxiety and task value (φs = −.35 vs. −.20), and test anxiety and achievement (φs = −.41 vs. −.17 in mathe- matics and language arts, respectively) were all considerably stronger in mathematics than language arts. SEM. Table 5 (i.e., “independent” models) presents the goodness-of-fit indices of structural models, separately esti- mated for each self-construct in mathematics and language arts. All models exhibited acceptable fit indices, whereas the self-concept model in language arts and the general self- esteem models in mathematics and language arts demon- strated only marginally acceptable fit to the empirical data. As was the case in Study 1, we did not add any path for the purpose of improving the model fit as to allow more accurate comparison of the results. Figure 4 presents statistically sig- nificant paths at p < .05 and the standardized coefficients. All self-constructs directly predicted achievement with varying power, with the exception of general self-esteem in mathematics. Further, all domain-specific self-constructs, with the exception of Bandura-type self-efficacy, displayed conspicuously stronger relationships to achievement in the domain of mathematics than language arts (γ s = .64 vs. .37 for self-concept and .52 vs. .25 for MSLQ self-efficacy in mathematics vs. language arts, respectively). The utility of the self-efficacy beliefs assessed by the Bandura-type scale for predicting students’ achievements did not differ across the two domains with γ s = .47 and .49 in mathematics
  • 12. The Journal of Educational Research 347 TABLE 5. Goodness-of-Fit Indices for Mathematics and Language Arts Models in Study 2 Model CFI TLI RMSEA χ2 (df) χ2 ( df) ASDQ Independent mathematics .969 .957 .062 96.199(33) Language arts .876 .831 .094 179.463(33) Invariance testing Unconstrained .914 .894 .061 346.843(73) 33.923(3)∗∗∗ Constrained .905 .887 .063 380.766(76) MSLQ Independent mathematics .952 .931 .082 109.961(25) Language arts .969 .956 .057 66.115(25) Invariance testing Unconstrained .958 .946 .048 186.255(56) 56.100(3) ∗∗∗ Constrained .941 .928 .055 242.355(59) BDR Independent mathematics .984 .973 .061 48.575(17) Language arts .951 .920 .086 81.272(17) Invariance testing Unconstrained .971 .958 .049 132.611(39) 19.378(3) ∗∗∗ Constrained .966 .955 .051 151.989(42) GSE Independent mathematics .889 .857 .084 232.384(51) Language arts .889 .857 .081 221.177(51) Invariance testing Unconstrained .892 .871 .055 454.676(111) 21.984(3) ∗∗∗ Constrained .886 .868 .056 476.660(114) Note. N = 507. CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem. ∗p < .05. ∗∗p < .01. ∗∗∗p < .001. and language arts, respectively. Test anxiety negatively me- diated the relationships of general self-esteem and MSLQ self-efficacy to achievements in both domains and of Bandura-type self-efficacy to achievement in the domain of mathematics but not language arts. Tests of invariance across domains. In principle, multi- group analysis is performed in order to test measurement or structural invariance across groups composed of indepen- dent samples. If multigroup analysis is conducted using a single sample, there is difficulty interpreting the results be- cause comparing covariance and error covariance becomes implausible. Nonetheless, multigroup analysis may be per- formed when there are numerous parameters in the model and testing all possible interaction effects increases the oc- currence of errors. For these reasons, we conducted multi- group analyses with the mathematics and language arts data. As done in Study 1, we again imposed cross-group equal- ity constraints to assess whether there were significant differ- ences in the individual parameters between mathematics and language arts. According to the chi-square difference statis- tic, the fit of the constrained model was significantly worse than that of the unconstrained model (see Table 5), sug- gesting that the parameters in the population of the two do- mains were not equal (Kline, 2005, p. 290). Hence, we con- cluded that there were significant differences in path coeffi- cients in mathematics and language arts. As Figure 4 shows, stronger paths were projected from MSLQ self-efficacy and self-concept to achievement in mathematics compared with language arts. Similarly, mathematics revealed stronger pre- dictions from MSLQ self-efficacy and general self-esteem to test anxiety, and test anxiety to achievement in the Bandura- type self-efficacy and general self-esteem models. Discussion Self-Constructs as Predictors of Student Achievement The present study confirmed once again that domain specificity is an important attribute of self-constructs to be able to function as significant predictors of tangible academic
  • 13. 348 The Journal of Educational Research FIGURE 4. Standardized regression weights for the mathematics and language arts models in Study 2. Coefficients before the slash are from mathematics; those after the slash are from language arts. ASDQ = self-concept assessed by the Academic Self-Description Questionnaire; MSLQ = self-efficacy assessed by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire; BDR = self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale; GSE = general self-esteem. Bolded numbers represent significant differences in standardized path coefficients between mathematics and language arts. ∗ p < .05. ∗∗ p < .01. ∗∗∗ p < .001. outcomes. Academic self-concept and self-efficacy assessed in reference to particular subject matter areas were able to predict achievements in their respective domains, regardless of the assessment scales. The only domain-specific construct that failed to relate significantly to achievement was the ele- mentary school students’ self-efficacy assessed by the MSLQ. However, interpretation of this result as a failure of MSLQ self-efficacy to predict achievement scores requires caution. There existed a strong and significant bivariate correlation between the elementary school students’ MSLQ self-efficacy and achievement (φ = .53), which was quite commensurate in magnitude to that between academic self-concept and achievement (φ = .61) or Bandura-type self-efficacy and achievement (φ = .56). It thus seems more reasonable to view this finding as a result of collinearity between MSLQ self-efficacy and task value included in the same structural model, as will be discussed again later. Global self-esteem that did not refer to any particular aca- demic domain was not able to predict achievements in spe- cific domains, except for achievement in language arts for the middle school students. Although general self-esteem corre- lated positively with achievement indexes for the elementary and middle school students, these correlations were con- siderably weaker than those demonstrated by the domain- specific self-constructs. In fact, they were even weaker than the correlations that task value and test anxiety, both mea- sured in reference to specific subject matter areas, exhibited with achievements. This proves that domain specificity is an important prerequisite in construct assessment with re- gard to not only self-constructs but also other motivation variables, if the goal is to predict students’ achievements. As our data further suggest, when asked domain- specifically without any reference to target outcomes, students do not distinguish between their self-efficacy and self-concept as clearly as theories would predict. Students’ academic self-concept assessed by the ASDQ (Marsh, 1992) and their academic self-efficacy assessed by the MSLQ (Pintrich et al., 1993) were almost identical, with correlation coefficients approaching unity. Academic self-efficacy assessed by the Bandura-type scale, which provided students with concrete referents against which to make ability judgments (e.g., “I can get more than ‘80’ in the math exam”), correlated substantially less strongly with the other two self-beliefs (correlations ranging between .76 and .84). Moreover, academic self-concept displayed the strongest predictive paths to achievements in mathematics for the elementary and middle school students in the present research. This finding was surprising because it is contrary to previous reports attesting to the superiority of self-efficacy beliefs in predicting students’ academic performances (Pajares & Miller, 1995; Pietsch et al., 2003). One of the possible reasons for these results could be the normative grading system the participants were exposed to. Korean middle school students are evaluated against their peers when receiving grades. The elementary school teach- ers participating in the present research were also asked to rate each student in comparison to other students they were teaching. As Marsh, Trautwein, L¨udtke, and K¨oller (2008)
  • 14. The Journal of Educational Research 349 suggested, students may engage in habitual comparison of their performance levels with those of other students when they answer perceived competence items because that is the prevalent evaluation criteria they have been accustomed to. In addition, students become sensitive to and begin utilizing social comparison information by Grade 4 (Ruble et al., 1976). Therefore, even without explicit allusion to comparative information in the assessment scales, students may nonetheless use normative standards unless concrete performance targets are presented. This might explain the strong correlation between the academic self-concept and MSLQ self-efficacy scores observed in this study. As normative judgments of competence were explicitly called for only by the self-concept items (i.e., “Compared to others my age I am good at mathematics classes”), these judgments might also better predict performances appraised under a normative system. In contrast, Bandura-type self-efficacy demonstrated the strongest predictive utility in the domain of language arts for the middle school students among the three domain-specific self-beliefs. This finding is intriguing because it was also the only domain-specific self-belief that displayed analogous pre- dictive power across ages and domains. Whereas academic self-concept and MSLQ self-efficacy’s predictive paths to achievements were noticeably stronger for the middle than elementary school students and in the domain of mathemat- ics than language arts, Bandura-type self-efficacy’s predictive relations to performances were comparable in magnitude across the two age groups and the two subject matter areas. Considering that it may be easier to judge an individual’s relative ability compared with others in mathematics than language arts due to the nature of the subject matter and the fact that comparative judgments of ability become more salient as children grow older (Ruble et al., 1976), these re- sults provide empirical support to the theoretical tenet that self-concept is more heavily affected by social comparison than is self-efficacy (Bong & Clark, 1999; Bong & Skaalvik, 2003). More importantly, our findings support the extant literature that domain-specific constructs, self-efficacy and self-concept, are better predictors of achievement compared with global self-esteem (Bandalos et al., 1995; Hansford & Hattie, 1982). Task Value and Test Anxiety as Mediators of Self–Achievement Relations The theoretical model linking self-constructs to achieve- ments with task value and test anxiety as mediators received clear support only with respect to Bandura-type self-efficacy in the middle school sample. As hypothesized and consis- tent with previous research (e.g., Pajares & Kranzler, 1995), task value positively mediated and test anxiety negatively mediated the link between self-efficacy and achievement. Students with stronger self-efficacy are likely to experience lower anxiety, and those who are high in anxiety are likely to have low achievement (Meier, McCarthy, & Schmeck, 1984). The opposite pattern holds for task value. Students with stronger self-efficacy also express stronger task value, and those who perceive stronger value in the subject tend to attain higher achievement in it (Meier, McCarthy, & Schmeck, 1984). The mediation hypothesis did not hold for self-concept. Students who reported higher levels of self-concept were likely to demonstrate higher achievement but this rela- tionship was not necessarily mediated by anxiety. This is likely due to the strong prediction of achievement by self-concept. Because self-concept already encompasses affective responses to perceived competencies (Bong & Clark, 1999; Covington, 1984), as demonstrated by its strong correlation with test anxiety in this research as well, no further variance in student achievement might have been explained by test anxiety after the variance due to self-concept was accounted for. Unfortunately, lack of differentiation among variables was not a problem confined only to self-constructs. Academic self-efficacy beliefs assessed by the MSLQ scale correlated too strongly with task value across ages and domains (with correlations ranging between .81 and .87). The correlations of academic self-concept with task value were also very high, although slightly lower than those of the MSLQ scale (rang- ing from .74 to .79). We thus warn researchers of the pos- sibility of multicollinearity hindering attainment of proper solutions when other domain-specific constructs such as task value and test anxiety are included in the same model with self-constructs. We cautiously recommend use of self-efficacy beliefs assessed by the Bandura-type scale, if the goal is to examine the predictive utility of perceived self-competence in a complex nomological network. Use of Bandura-type self-efficacy ratings will be least affected by collinearity with other variables and hence enable delineation of all possible predictive and mediational paths to student achievement, as documented in this study. Age and Academic Domain as Moderators of Self–Achievement Relations Consistent with our hypothesis, the middle school sample in our study was associated with stronger predictive pat- terns between self-constructs and achievements. However, the differences were not as significant as we had anticipated. Given our sample of elementary school students consisting of fifth- and sixth-grade students, it is likely that they were al- ready realistic and sensitive to normative information when judging their abilities (Nicholls, 1975, 1978; Ruble et al., 1976). Therefore, the predictive patterns for the elementary school students were not drastically different from those for the middle school students. Regarding differences between domains, the predictive paths from domain-specific self-constructs to achievements were generally stronger in mathematics compared with lan- guage arts. We interpret this finding to indicate that students possess better knowledge about their own performance levels
  • 15. 350 The Journal of Educational Research in mathematics because evaluation standards in mathemat- ics tend to be more solidly established compared with those in language arts. Again, the predictive paths emanating from Bandura-type self-efficacy to achievements were exceptions to this trend because there was no noticeable difference in them across domains. It appears that self-efficacy beliefs assessed by items that present either specific target perfor- mances or concrete judgment standards afford the most ro- bust prediction of achievements that is not compromised by age, domain, or collinearity with other constructs. De- spite the differences in the magnitude of predictive relations, the pattern with which each self-construct links to achieve- ment was consistent in both subjects and no unique pattern emerged domain-wise. Interestingly, global self-esteem exhibited much stronger correlation with academic self-concept and academic self- efficacy assessed by the MSLQ scale in the domain of math- ematics than language arts. Moreover, these mathematics- specific self-perceptions better predicted test anxiety and achievement in the domain than did domain-specific self- perceptions in language arts. Taken together, these results seem to suggest that self-perceived competence in mathe- matics is more psychologically salient than self-perceived competence in language arts for the middle school partici- pants in this study. Conclusions and Future Directions We compared the predictive utility of multiple self- constructs with different degrees of domain specificity. We found domain-specific self-perceptions such as academic self- concept and self-efficacy to demonstrate superior predictive utility compared with global self-esteem. We also tested task value and test anxiety as potential mediators of the self- constructs’ effects on achievements. The complete model in- cluding all hypothesized mediators received clear empirical support only in the model with Bandura-type self-efficacy. We examined these hypothesized predictive paths in two age groups and two subject matter areas in an attempt to locate important possible moderators and establish generalizability of our findings. Age and academic domain proved to moder- ate the predictive relations to a certain degree, with domain creating a greater gap in the self-constructs’ predictive power than age. Several limitations should be noted. First, due to multi- collinearity problems, we were not able to include all rele- vant constructs in a single SEM. We suggest researchers ex- amine ways to distinguish domain-specific constructs more clearly in future studies. At the same time, the possibil- ity of cultural differences creating a different pattern of relationships cannot be completely ruled out. Our results showed that the model with three separate self-constructs, self-esteem, self-concept, and self-efficacy displayed satisfac- tory fit to the empirical data, consistent with the previous findings with Western samples. However, the correlation co- efficients between the self-concept and self-efficacy factors were still too high in this study, which suggests a possibil- ity of culture-specific finding. Second, although we included elementary and middle school samples in this study, the age difference between the two groups did not appear suf- ficiently large to capture developmental differences. It may be a worthwhile endeavor for investigators in this area to examine the point at which diverse self-constructs start to diverge and demonstrate disparate patterns of association with achievement-related motivation, emotion, and perfor- mance. Finally, we attributed the superior predictive utility of self-concept compared with self-efficacy in the present study to students’ increased awareness of social comparative information and the normative evaluation system they were exposed to. It will be interesting to see whether the present results replicate among students that are not functioning under such normative grading structures. ACKNOWLEDGMENT This research was supported by the WCU (World Class University) Pro- gram funded by the Korean Ministry of Education, Science and Technology, consigned to the Korea Science and Engineering Foundation (Grant no. R32-2008-000-20023-0). REFERENCES Arbuckle, J. L. (2006). AMOS (Version 7.0) [Computer software]. Chicago, IL: SPSS. Bachman, J. G., & O’Malley, P. M. (1977). Self-esteem in young men: A longitudinal analysis of the impact of educational and occupational attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 365–380. Bandalos, D. L., Yates, K., & Thorndike-Christ, T. (1995). Effects of math self-concept, perceived self-efficacy, and attributions for failure and suc- cess on test anxiety. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 611–623. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bandura, A. (1988). Self-efficacy conception of anxiety. Anxiety Research, 1, 77–98. Bandura, A. (1989). Human agency in social cognitive theory. American Psychologist, 44, 1175–1184. Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY: Freeman. Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self- efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 586–598. Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psycho- logical Bulletin, 107, 238–246. Berndt, T. J., & Miller, K. E. (1990). Expectancies, values, and achievement in junior high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 319–326. Bong, M. (1997). Generality of academic self-efficacy judgments: Evidence of hierarchical relations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 696–709. Bong, M. (1998). Tests of the internal/external frames of reference model with subject-specific academic self-efficacy and frame-specific academic self-concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 102–110. Bong, M. (2001a). Between- and within-domain relations of academic mo- tivation among middle and high school students: Self-efficacy, task-value, and achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 23–34. Bong, M. (2001b). Role of self-efficacy and task-value in predicting college students’ course performance and future enrollment intentions. Contem- porary Educational Psychology, 26, 553–570. Bong, M. (2009). Age-related differences in achievement goal differentia- tion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 879–896. Bong, M., & Clark, R. E. (1999). Comparison between self-concept and self-efficacy in academic motivation research. Educational Psychologist, 34, 139–154.
  • 16. The Journal of Educational Research 351 Bong, M., & Hocevar, D. (2002). Measuring self-efficacy: Multitrait- multimethod comparison of scaling procedures. Applied Measurement in Education, 15, 143–171. Bong, M., & Skaalvik, E. M. (2003). Academic self-concept and self- efficacy: How different are they really? Educational Psychology Review, 15, 1–40. Brookover, W. B., Thomas, S., & Paterson, A. (1964). Self-concept of ability and school achievement. Sociology of Education, 37, 271–278. Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, application, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Byrne, B. M., & Shavelson, R. J. (1986). On the structure of the adolescent self-concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 474–481. Calsyn, R. J., & Kenny, D. A. (1977). Self-concept of ability and perceived evaluation of others: Cause or effect of academic achievement? Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 136–145. Choi, N. (2005). Self-efficacy and self-concept as predictors of college students’ academic performance. Psychology in the Schools, 42, 197–205. Covington, M. V. (1984). The self-worth theory of achievement motiva- tion: Findings and implications. The Elementary School Journal, 85, 4–20. Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., & Adler, T. F. (1984). Grade-related changes in the school environment: Effects on achievement motivation. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation (pp. 283–331). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the achiever: The structure of adolescents’ academic achievement related-beliefs and self- perceptions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 215–225. Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993). Age and gender differences in children’s self- and task perceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64, 830–847. Hansford, B. C., & Hattie, J. A. (1982). The relationship between self and achievement/performance measures. Review of Educational Research, 52, 123–142. Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covari- ance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55. Hulleman, C. S., Durik, A. M., Schweigert, S. A., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2008). Task values, achievement goals, and interest: An integrative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 398–416. Hunt, J. McV., Cole, M. W., & Reis, E. E. S. (1958). Situational cues distinguishing anger, fear, and sorrow. American Journal of Psychology, 71, 136–151. Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Kun, A., Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. (1974). The development of integra- tion processes using ability and effort information to predict outcome. Developmental Psychology, 10, 721–732. Ludwig, D. J., & Maehr, M. L. (1967). Changes in self concept and stated behavioral preferences. Child Development, 38, 453–467. MacCallum, R. C., Roznowski, M., Mar, C. M., & Reith, J. V. (1994). Alternative strategies for cross-validation of covariance structure models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 29, 1–32. Marsh, H. W. (1986). Verbal and math self-concepts: An internal/external frame of reference model. American Educational Research Journal, 23, 129–149. Marsh, H. W. (1987). The big-fish-little-pond effect on academic self- concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 280–295. Marsh, H. W. (1990). Influences of internal and external frames of reference on the formation of math and English self-concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 107–116. Marsh, H. W. (1991). Failure of high-ability high schools to deliver aca- demic benefits commensurate with their students’ ability levels. American Educational Research Journal, 28, 445–480. Marsh, H. W. (1992). Self-Description Questionnaire-II: Manual. New South Wales, Australia: University of Western Sydney. Marsh, H. W., Barnes, J., Cairns, L., & Tidman, M. (1984). Self-description questionnaire: Age and sex effects in the structure and level of self- concept for preadolescent children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 940–956. Marsh, H. W., Byrne, B. M., & Shavelson, R. J. (1988). A multifaceted aca- demic self-concept: Its hierarchical structure and its relation to academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 366–380. Marsh, H. W., Byrne, B. M., & Yeung, A. S. (1999). Causal ordering of academic self-concept and achievement: Reanalysis of a pioneering study and revised recommendations. Educational Psychologist, 34, 154–157. Marsh, H. W., Trautwein, U., L¨udtke, O., & K¨oller, O. (2008). Social comparison and big-fish-little-pond effects on self-concept and other self-belief constructs: Role of generalized and specific others. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 510–524. Marsh, H. W., Walker, R., & Debus, R. (1991). Subject-specific compo- nents of academic self-concept and self-efficacy. Contemporary Educa- tional Psychology, 16, 331–345. Meier, S., McCarthy, P. R., & Schmeck, R. (1984). Validity of self-efficacy as a predictor of writing performance. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 107–120. Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). Relation of self-efficacy beliefs to academic outcomes: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 30–38. Nicholls, J. G. (1975). Causal attributions and other achievement-related cognitions: effects of task outcome, attainment value, and sex. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 379–389. Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The development of the concepts of effort and ability, perception of academic attainment, and the understanding that difficult tasks require more ability. Child Development, 49, 800–814. Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of Edu- cational Research, 66, 543–578. Pajares, F., & Kranzler, J. (1995). Self-efficacy beliefs and general men- tal ability in mathematical problem-solving. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 20, 426–443. Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-concept beliefs in mathematical problem solving: A path analysis. Journal of Ed- ucational Psychology, 86, 193–203. Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1995). Mathematics self-efficacy and math- ematics performances: The need for specificity of assessment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 190–198. Pajares, F., & Schunk, D. H. (2002). Self and self-belief in psychology and education: A historical perspective. In J. Aronson & D. Cordova (Eds.), Improving academic achievement: Impact of psychological factors on education (pp. 3–21). New York, NY: Academic Press. Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. N. (1977). The development of achievement- related expectancies. Child Development, 48, 1075–1079. Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2006). Achievement goals and discrete achievement emotions: A theoretical model and prospective test. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 583–597. Pietsch, J., Walker, R., & Chapman, E. (2003). The relationship among self- concept, self-efficacy, and performance in mathematics during secondary school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 589–603. Pintrich, P. R., & De Groot, E. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Ed- ucational Psychology, 82, 33–40. Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the Motivated Strategies for Learn- ing Questionnaire (MSLQ). Educational and Psychological Measurement, 53, 801–813. Pokay, P., & Blumenfeld, P. C. (1990). Predicting achievement early and late in the semester: The role of motivation and use of learning strategies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 41–50. Pullman, H., & Allik, J. (2008). Relations of academic and general self- esteem to school achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 559–564. Rosenberg, M. (1962). The association between self-esteem and anxiety. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 1, 135–152. Rosenberg, M., Schooler, C., & Schoenbach, C. (1989). Self-esteem and adolescent problems: Modeling reciprocal effects. American Sociological Review, 54, 1004–1018. Rosenberg, M., Schooler, C., Schoenbach, C., & Rosenberg, F. (1995). Global self-esteem and specific self-esteem: Different concepts, different outcomes. American Sociological Review, 60, 141–156. Ruble, D. N., Parsons, J. E., & Ross, J. (1976). Self-evaluative responses of children in an achievement setting. Child Development, 47, 990–997. Ruthig, J. C., Perry, R. P., Hladkyj, S., Hall, N. C., Pekrun, R., & Chipper- field, J. G. (2008). A longitudinal analysis of perceived control and emo- tions in an academic setting. Social Psychology of Education, 11, 161–180. Scherer, K. R., Schorr, A., & Johnstone, T. (Eds.). (2001). Appraisal pro- cesses in emotion. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • 17. 352 The Journal of Educational Research Schreiber, J. B., Stage, F. K., King, J., Nora, A., & Barlow, E. A. (2006). Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: A review. Journal of Educational Research, 99, 323–337. Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26, 207–231. Shavelson, R. J., & Bolus, R. (1982). Self-concept: The interplay of theory and methods. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 3–47. Shavelson, R. J., Hubner, J. J., & Stanton, G. C. (1976). Self-concept: Validation of construct interpretations. Review of Educational Research, 46, 407–441. Simmons, R. G., Blyth, D. A., Van Cleave, E. F., & Bush, D. M. (1979). Entry into early adolescence: The impact of school structure, puberty, and early dating on self-esteem. American Sociological Review, 44, 948–967. Skaalvik, E. M., & Hagtvet, K. A. (1990). Academic achievement and self-concept: An analysis of causal predominance in a developmental perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 292–307. Skaalvik, E. M., & Rankin, R. J. (1995). A test of the Internal/External Frame of Reference Model at different levels of math and verbal self- perception. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 161–184. Smelser, N. (1989). Self-esteem and social problems: An introduction. In A. M. Mecca, N. J. Smelser, & J. Vasconcellos (Eds.), The social importance of self-esteem (pp. 294–326). Berkeley: University of California Press. Stipek, D., & Mac Iver, D. (1989). Developmental change in children’s assessment of intellectual competence. Child Development, 60, 521–538. Stipek, D., & Tannatt, L. (1984). Children’s judgments of their own and their peers’ academic competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 75–84. Stodolski, S. S., Salk, S., & Glaessner, B. (1991). Student views about learning math and social studies. American Educational Research Journal, 28, 89–116. Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38, 1–10. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., Harold, R. D., Arbreton, A. J. A., Freedman-Doan, C., & Blumenfeld, P. C. (1997). Change in chil- dren’s competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elemen- tary school years: A 3-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 451–469. Zeidner, M., & Schleyer, E. J. (1999). The effects on educational con- text on individual difference variables, self-perceptions of giftedness, and school attitudes in gifted adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 687–703. Zimmerman, B. J. (1995). Self-efficacy and educational development. In A. Bandura (Ed.), Self-efficacy in changing societies (pp. 202–231). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Zimmerman, B. J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Self- motivation for academic attainment: The role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal setting. American Educational Research Journal, 29, 663–676. AUTHORS NOTE Mimi Bong is a Professor of Educational Psychology in the Department of Education and the Associate Director of bMRI (Brain and Motivation Research Institute) at Ko- rea University. Her current research interests focus on the dynamic interplay between student motivation and learn- ing contexts, with particular emphases on self-efficacy and achievement goals. Catherine Cho was with Korea University at the time of the study and is now a PhD student in neuroscience in the Department of Psychology at Rutgers University–Newark. Her research interest broadly focuses on the interaction between cognition and emotion that affects decision- making processes. More specifically, she is interested in the mechanisms of emotion regulation and how success- ful regulation of emotions guides optimal decisions and choices. Hyun Seon Ahn is a PhD student in educational psy- chology in the Department of Education and a research assistant of bMRI at Korea University. Her research in- terests include self-efficacy beliefs and classroom goal structures. Hyejin Kim is a PhD student in educational psychol- ogy in the Department of Education and a research assis- tant of bMRI at Korea University. Her current research interests focus on personality, cognitive engagement, so- cial goals, social efficacy, and social adjustment among adolescents.
  • 18. Copyright of Journal of Educational Research is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.