SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 59
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



PART ONE: INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS

Every human knows       at least one language, spoken or signed. Linguistics is the
science of language,    including the sounds, words, and grammar rules. Words in
languages are finite,   but sentences are not. It is this creative aspect of human
language that sets       it apart from animal languages, which are essentially
responses to stimuli.

The rules of a language, also called grammar, are learned as one acquires a
language. These rules include phonology, the sound system, morphology, the
structure of words, syntax, the combination of words into sentences,
semantics, the ways in which sounds and meanings are related, and the
lexicon, or mental dictionary of words. When you know a language, you know
words in that language, i.e. sound units that are related to specific meanings.
However, the sounds and meanings of words are arbitrary.

Knowing a language encompasses this entire system, but this knowledge (called
competence) is different from behavior (called performance.) You may know
a language, but you may also choose to not speak it. Although you are not
speaking the language, you still have the knowledge of it. However, if you don't
know a language, you cannot speak it at all.

There are two types of grammars: descriptive and prescriptive. Descriptive
grammars represent the unconscious knowledge of a language. English
speakers, for example, know that "me likes apples" is incorrect and "I like
apples" is correct, although the speaker may not be able to explain why.
Descriptive grammars do not teach the rules of a language, but rather describe
rules that are already known. In contrast, prescriptive grammars dictate what
a speaker's grammar should be and they include teaching grammars, which are
written to help teach a foreign language.
        There are about 5,000 languages in the world right now (give or take a
few thousand), and linguists have discovered that these languages are more
alike than different from each other. There are universal concepts and properties
that are shared by all languages, and these principles are contained in the
Universal Grammar, which forms the basis of all possible human languages.




                                                                                              1
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



PART TWO: MORPHOLOGY AND SYNTAX

Morphemes are the minimal units of words that have a meaning and cannot be
subdivided further. There are two main types: free and bound. Free morphemes
can occur alone and bound morphemes must occur with another morpheme. An
example of a free morpheme is "bad", and an example of a bound morpheme is
"ly." It is bound because although it has meaning, it cannot stand alone. It must
be attached to another morpheme to produce a word.
        Free morpheme:      bad
        Bound morpheme: ly
        Word:               badly

When we talk about words, there are two groups: lexical (or content) and
function (or grammatical) words. Lexical words are called open class words and
include nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs. New words can regularly be added
to this group. Function words, or closed class words, are conjunctions,
prepositions, articles and pronouns; and new words cannot be (or are very
rarely) added to this class.

Affixes are often the bound morpheme. This group includes prefixes,
suffixes, infixes, and circumfixes. Prefixes are added to the beginning of
another morpheme, suffixes are added to the end, infixes are inserted into other
morphemes, and circumfixes are attached to another morpheme at the
beginning and end. Following are examples of each of these:
      Prefix:             re-    added        to doproducesredo
      Suffix:             -or    added        to edit produces editor
      Infix:              -um- added          to fikas (strong) produces
                                              fumikas (to be strong) in Bontoc
      Circumfix:          ge- and -t          to lieb (love) produces geliebt
                                              (loved) in German

There are two categories of affixes: derivational and inflectional. The main
difference between the two is that derivational affixes are added to morphemes
to form new words that may or may not be the same part of speech and
inflectional affixes are added to the end of an existing word for purely
grammatical reasons. In English there are only eight total inflectional affixes:




                                                                                              2
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



      -s           3rd person singular present               she waits
      -ed          past tense                                she waited
      -ing         progressive                               she's eating
      -en          past participle                           she has eaten
      -s           plural                                    three apples
      -'s          possessive                                Lori's son
      -er          comparative                               you are taller
      -est         superlative                               you are the shortest

The other type of bound morphemes are called bound roots. These are
morphemes (and not affixes) that must be attached to another morpheme and
do not have a meaning of their own. Some examples are -ceive in perceive and
mit in submit.

English Morphemes
    A. Free
          1. Open Class
          2. Closed Class
    B. Bound
          1. Affix
                 a. Derivational
                 b. Inflectional
          2. Root
          3.
There are six ways to form new words. Compounds are a combination of
words, acronyms are derived from the initials of words, back-formations are
created from removing what is mistakenly considered to be an affix,
abbreviations or clippings are shortening longer words, eponyms are
created from proper nouns (names), and blending is combining parts of words
into one.
Compound:           doghouse
Acronym:            NBA (National Basketball Association) or
                    scuba(self-contained underwater breathing apparatus)
Back-formation:     edit from editor
Abbreviation:       phone from telephone
Eponym:             sandwich from Earl of Sandwich
Blending:           smog from smoke and fog



                                                                                             3
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



Grammar is learned unconsciously at a young age. Ask any five year old, and he
will tell you that "I eat" and "you eat," but his "dog eats." But a human's
syntactical knowledge goes farther than what is grammatical and what is not. It
also accounts for ambiguity, in which a sentence could have two meanings, and
enables us to determine grammatical relationships such as subject and direct
object. Although we may not consciously be able to define the terms, we
unconsciously know how to use them in sentences.

Syntax, of course, depends on lexical categories (parts of speech.) You probably
learned that there are 8 main parts of speech in grammar school. Linguistics
takes a different approach to these categories and separates words into
morphological and syntactic groups. Linguistics analyzes words according to their
affixes and the words that follow or precede them. Hopefully, the following
definitions of the parts of speech will make more sense and be of more use than
the old definitions of grammar school books.




Open Class Words
             _____ + plural endings          Det. Adj. _____ (this is called a Noun Phrase)
Nouns
             "dogs"                          "the big dog"

             _______ + tense endings Aux. ____ (this is called a Verb Phrase)
Verbs
             "speaks"                "have spoken"
             ______ + er / est               Det. ____ Noun
Adjectives
             "small"                         "the smaller child"

             Adj. + ly                       ____ Adj. or Verb or Adv.
Adverbs
             "quickly"                       "quickly ran"




                                                                                              4
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



Closed Class Words
                 a, an, the, this, that, these, _____________ Adj. Noun
Determiners
                 those, pronouns, quantities "this blue book"

Auxiliary        forms of be, have, may,             NP __________________ VP
Verbs            can, shall                          "the girl is swimming"

                                                     _____ NP (this is called a Prep. Phase)
Prepositions     at, in, on, under, over, of
                                                     "in the room"

                                                     N or V or Adj. ____________ N or V
Conjunctions and, but, or                            or Adj.
                                                     "apples and oranges"


Sub categorization defines the restrictions on which syntactic categories (parts
of speech) can or cannot occur within a lexical item. These additional
specifications of words are included in our mental lexicon. Verbs are the most
common categories that are subcategorized. Verbs can either be transitive or
intransitive. Transitive verbs take a direct object, while intransitive verbs
take an indirect object (usually they need a preposition before the noun).


Transitive verb: to eat     I ate an apple. (direct object)
Intransitive: to sleep      I was sleeping in the bed. (indirect object)


Individual nouns can also be subcategorized. For example, the noun idea can be
followed by a Prepositional Phrase or that and a sentence. But the noun
compassion can only be followed by a Prepositional Phrase and not a sentence.
 (Ungrammatical sentences are marked with asterisks.)


the idea of stricter laws                   his compassion for the animals
the idea that stricter laws
                                            *his compassion that the animals are hurt
are necessary




                                                                                               5
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



Phrase structure rules describe how phrases are formed and in what order.
These rules define the following:


Noun Phrase (NP)                    (Det.) (Adj.) Noun (PP)
Verb Phrase (VP)                    Verb (NP) (PP)
Prepositional Phrase (PP)           Prep. NP
Sentence (S)                        NP VP


The parentheses indicate the categories are optional. Verbs don't always have to
be followed by prepositional phrases and nouns don't always have to be
preceded by adjectives.

Passive Sentences
The difference between the two sentences "Mary hired Bill" and "Bill was hired
by Mary" is that the first is active and the second is passive. In order to change
an active sentence into a passive one, the object of the active must become the
subject of the passive. The verb in the passive sentence becomes a form of "be"
plus the participle form of the main verb. And the subject of the active becomes
the object of the passive preceded by the word "by."


Active                         Passive
Mary hired Bill.               Bill was hired by Mary.
Subject + Verb + Object        Object + "be" + Verb + by + Subject




                                                                                                6
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



PART THREE: PHONETICS AND PHONOLOGY

There are three types of the study of the sounds of language. Acoustic
Phonetics is the study of the physical properties of sounds. Auditory
Phonetics is the study of the way listeners perceive sounds. Articulatory
Phonetics (the type this lesson is concerned with) is the study of how the vocal
tracts produce the sounds.

The orthography (spelling) of words in misleading, especially in English. One
sound can be represented by several different combinations of letters. For
example, all of the following words contain the same vowel sound: he, believe,
Lee, Caesar, key, amoeba, loudly, machine, people, and sea. The following poem
illustrates this fact of English humorously (note the pronunciation of the bold
words):

I take it you already know of tough and bough and cough and dough?
Some may stumble, but not you, on hiccough, thorough, slough, and
through?
So now you are ready, perhaps, to learn of less familiar traps?
Beware of heard, a dreadful word, that looks like beard, but sounds like bird.
And dead, it's said like bed, not bead; for goodness' sake, don't call it deed!
Watch out for meat and great and threat. (They rhyme with suite and
straight and debt.)
A moth is not a moth in mother, nor both in bother, broth in brother.
And here is not a match for there, nor dear and fear, for bear and pear.
And then there's dose and rose and lose - just look them up - and goose and
choose
Andcork and work and card and ward and font and front and word and
sword
And do and go, then thwart and cart, come, come! I've hardly made a start.
A dreadful language? Why man alive! I've learned to talk it when I was five.
And yet to write it, the more I tried, I hadn't learned it at fifty-five.
- Author Unknown

The discrepancy between spelling and sounds led to the formation of the
International Phonetics Alphabet (IPA.) The symbols used in this alphabet
can be used to represent all sounds of all human languages. The following is the



                                                                                              7
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



English Phonetic alphabet. You might want to memorize all of these symbols, as
most foreign language dictionaries use the IPA.


                Phonetic Alphabet for English Pronunciation

                p      pill       d    dill           h   heal     ʌ    but

                b      bill       n    neal           l   leaf     aj   light

                m mill            s    seal           r   reef     ɔ j boy

                f      feel       z    zeal           j   you      ɪ    bit

                v      veal       č    chill          w witch      ɛ    bet

                θ      thigh      ǰ    Jill           i   beet     ʊ    foot

                ð      thy        ʍ    which          e   bait     ɔ    awe

                š      shill      k    kill           u   boot     a    bar

                ž      azure      g    gill           o   boat     ə    sofa

                t      till       ŋ    ring           æ bat        aw cow



Some speakers of English pronounce the words which and witch differently, but
if you pronounce both words identically, just use w for both words. And the
sounds /ʌ / and /ə/ are pronounced the same, but the former is used in stressed
syllables, while the latter is used in unstressed syllables. This list does not even
begin to include all of the phonetic symbols though. One other symbol is the
glottal stop, ʔ which is somewhat rare in English. Some linguists in the United
States traditionally use different symbols than the IPA symbols. These are listed
below.
                                          U.S. IPA
                                          š       ʃ
                                          ž       ʒ
                                          č       tʃ
                                          ǰ       dʒ
                                          U       ʊ



                                                                                                 8
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



The production of any speech sound involves the movement of air. Air is pushed
through the lungs, larynx (vocal folds) and vocal tract (the oral and nasal
cavities.) Sounds produced by using air from the lungs are called pulmonic
sounds. If the air is pushed out, it is called egressive. If the air is sucked in, it is
called ingressive. Sounds produced by ingressive airstreams are ejectives,
implosives, and clicks. These sounds are common among African and American
Indian languages. The majority of languages in the world use pulmonic egressive
airstream mechanisms, and I will present only these types of sounds in this
lesson.

Consonants
Consonants are produced as air from the lungs is pushed through the glottis (the
opening between the vocal cords) and out the mouth. They are classified
according to voicing, aspiration, nasal/oral sounds, places of articulation and
manners of articulation. Voicing is whether the vocal folds vibrate or not. The
sound /s/ is called voiceless because there is no vibration, and the sound /z/ is
called voiced because the vocal folds do vibrate (you can feel on your neck if
there is vibration.) Only three sounds in English have aspiration, the sounds /b/,
/p/ and /t/. An extra puff of air is pushed out when these sounds begin a word
or stressed syllable. Hold a piece of paper close to your mouth when saying the
words pin and spin. You should notice extra air when you say pin. Aspiration is
indicated in writing with a superscript h, as in /pʰ /. Nasal sounds are produced
when the velum (the soft palate located in the back of the roof of the mouth) is
lowered and air is passed through the nose and mouth. Oral sounds are
produced when the velum is raised and air passes only through the mouth.

Places of Articulation
Bilabial:    lips together
Labiodental: lower lip against front teeth
Interdental: tongue between teeth
Alveolar:    tongue near alveolar ridge on roof of mouth (in between teeth
             and hard palate)
Palatal:     tongue on hard palate
Velar:       tongue near velum
Glottal:     space between vocal folds




                                                                                               9
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



   The following sound is not found in the English language, although it is common
   in languages such as French and Arabic:

   Uvular:       raise back of tongue to uvula (the appendage hanging down from
                the velum)

   Manners of Articulation
   Stop:      obstruct airstream completely
   Fricative: partial obstruction with friction
   Affricate: stop airstream, then release
   Liquids:   partial obstruction, no friction
   Glides:    little or no obstruction, must occur with a vowel

   You should practice saying the sounds of the English alphabet to see if you can
   identify the places of articulation in the mouth. The sounds are described by
   voicing, place and then manner of articulation, so the sound /j/ would be called a
   voiced palatal glide and the sound /s/ would be called a voiceless alveolar
   fricative.



                                          Interdent Alveola
              Bilabial Labiodental                          Palatal Velar                     Glottal
                                              al       r
                 p                                             t                      k
Stop (oral)
                 b                                             d                      g
Nasal
                m                                              n                      ŋ
(stop)
                                f               θ              s           š
Fricative                                                                                         h
                                v               ð              z           ž
                                                                           č
Affricate
                                                                          ǰ
                ʍ                                                                    ʍ
Glide                                                                                             h
                w                                                          j         w
Liquid                                                        lr




                                                                                                 10
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



For rows that have two consonants, the top consonant is voiceless and the
bottom consonant is voiced. Nasal stops are all voiced, as are liquids. The sound
/j/ is also voiced. If sounds are in two places on the chart, that means they can
be pronounced either way.

Vowels
Vowels are produced by a continuous airstream and all are voiced. They are
classified according to height of the tongue, part of tongue involved, and position
of the lips. The tongue can be high, mid, or low; and the part of the tongue used
can be front, central or back. Only four vowels are produced with rounded lips
and only four vowels are considered tense instead of lax. The sound /a/ would
be written as a low back lax unrounded vowel. Many languages also have vowels
called diphthongs, a sequence of two sounds, vowel + glide. Examples in English
include oy in boy and ow in cow. In addition, vowels can be nasalized when they
occur before nasal consonants. A diacritic mark [~] is placed over the vowel to
show this. The vowel sounds in bee and bean are considered different because
the sound in bean is nasalized.


                                                    Part of Tongue

                                     Front                 Central                 Back

                                        i                                            u
                 High
                                       ɪ                                             ʊ
Tongue
                                       e                      ə                      o
Height           Mid
                                       ɛ                      ʌ                      ɔ
                 Low                   æ                                              a


The bold vowels are tense, and the italic vowels are rounded. English also
includes the diphthongs: [aj] as in bite, [aw] as in cow, and [oj] as in boy.
For the complete IPA chart with symbols for the sounds of every human
language, please visit the International Phonetic Association's website. And
you're looking for a way to type English IPA symbols online, please visit
ipa.typeit.org




                                                                                              11
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



Major Classes of Sounds (Distinctive Features)
All of the classes of sounds described above can be put into more general classes
that include the patterning of sounds in the world's languages. Continuant
sounds indicate a continuous airflow, while non-continuant sounds indicate
total obstruction of the airstream. Obstruent sounds do not allow air to escape
through the nose, while sonorant sounds have a relatively free airflow through
the mouth or nose. The following table summarizes this information:


                                   Obstruent                      Sonorant
     Continuant                      fricatives             liquids, glides, vowels
                                    oral stops,
     Non-Continuant                                               nasal stops
                                    affricates


Major Class Features                             [+ Lateral] [l]
[+ Consonantal] consonants                      [- Lateral] [r]
[- Consonantal] vowels                          [+ Delayed Release] affricates [č, ǰ ]
[+Sonorant] nasals, liquids, glides,            [- Delayed Release] stops [p, b, t, d, k,
vowels                                          g, ʔ ]
[- Sonorant] stops, fricatives, affricates      [+ Strident] “noisy” fricatives [f, v, s, z,
(obstruents)                                    š, ž]
[+ Approximant] glides [j, w]                   [- Strident] [?, ð, h]
[- Approximant] everything else                 Place Features
Voice Features                                  [Labial] involves lips [f, v, p, b, w]
[+ Voice] voiced                                [Coronal] alveolar ridge to palate [θ, ð,
[- Voice] voiceless                             s, z, t, d, š, ž, n, r, l]
[+ Spread Glottis] aspirated [pʰ , tʰ ,         [+ Anterior] interdentals and true
kʰ ]                                            alveolars
[- Spread Glottis] unaspirated                  [- Anterior] retroflex and palatals [š, ž,
[+ Constricted Glottis] ejectives,              č, ǰ , j]
implosives                                      [Dorsal] from velum back [k, g, ŋ]
[- Constricted Glottis] everything else         [Glottal] in larynx [h, ʔ ]
Manner Features                                 Vowels
[+ Continuant] fricatives [f, v, s, z, š, ž, Height [± high] [± low]
θ, ð]                                        Backness [± back]
[- Continuant] stops [p, b, t, d, k, g,      Lip Rounding [± round]
ʔ ]                                          Tenseness [± tense]



                                                                                              12
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

[+ Nasal] nasal consonants [m, n, ŋ]
[- Nasal] all oral consonants

Whereas phonetics is the study of sounds and is concerned with the production,
audition and perception of speech sounds (called phones), phonology describes
the way sounds function within a given language and operates at the level of
sound systems and abstract sound units. Knowing the sounds of a language is
only a small part of phonology. This importance is shown by the fact that you
can change one word into another by simply changing one sound. Consider the
differences between the words time and dime. The words are identical except for
the first sound. [t] and [d] can therefore distinguish words, and are called
contrasting sounds. They are distinctive sounds in English, and all distinctive
sounds are classified as phonemes.

Minimal Pairs
Minimal pairs are words with different meanings that have the same sounds
except for one. These contrasting sounds can either be consonants or vowels.
The words pin and bin are minimal pairs because they are exactly the same
except for the first sound. The words read and rude are also exactly the same
except for the vowel sound. The examples from above, time and dime, are also
minimal pairs. In effect, words with one contrastive sound are minimal pairs.
Another feature of minimal pairs is overlapping distribution. Sounds that occur in
phonetic environments that are identical are said to be in overlapping
distribution. The sounds of [ɪ n] from pin and bin are in overlapping distribution
because they occur in both words. The same is true for three and through. The
sounds of [θr] is in overlapping distribution because they occur in both words as
well.

Free Variation
Some words in English are pronounced differently by different speakers. This is
most noticeable among American English speakers and British English speakers,
as well as dialectal differences. This is evidenced in the ways neither, for
example, can be pronounced. American English pronunciation is [niðər], while
British English pronunciation is [najðər].

Phones and Allophones
Phonemes are not physical sounds. They are abstract mental representations of
the phonological units of a language. Phones are considered to be any single



                                                                                             13
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

speech sound of which phonemes are made. Phonemes are a family of phones
regarded as a single sound and represented by the same symbol. The different

phones that are the realization of a phoneme are called allophones of that
phoneme. The use of allophones is not random, but rule-governed. No one is
taught these rules as they are learned subconsciously when the native language



                                                                                         ] are
allophones of the phoneme /ɪ /.

Complementary Distribution
If two sounds are allophones of the same phoneme, they are said to be in
complementary distribution. These sounds cannot occur in minimal pairs and
they cannot change the meaning of otherwise identical words. If you interchange
the sounds, you will only change the pronunciation of the words, not the
meaning. Native speakers of the language regard the two allophones as
variations of the same sound. To hear this, start to say the word cool (your lips
should be pursed in anticipation of /u/ sound), but then say kill instead (with
your lips still pursed.) Your pronunciation of kill should sound strange because
cool and kill are pronounced with different allophones of the phoneme /k/.
Nasalized vowels are allophones of the

                           ]. Yet in French, nasalized vowels are not allophones
of the same phonemes. They are separate phonemes. The words beau [bo] and
        ] are not in complementary distribution because they are minimal pairs
and have contrasting sounds. Changing the sounds changes the meaning of the
words. This is just one example of differences between languages.

Phonological Rules
     Assimilation: sounds become more like neighboring sounds, allowing for
                    ease of articulation or pronunciation; such as vowels are
                    nasalized before nasal consonants.
     Harmony:       non-adjacent vowels become more similar by sharing a
                    feature or set of features (common in Finnish)
     Gemination: sound becomes identical to an adjacent sound
     Regressive Assimilation: sound on left is the target, and sound on right
     is the trigger.Dissimilation:        sounds become less like neighboring



                                                                                             14
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

                                               sounds; these rules are quite rare, but

                               one example in English is [fɪ fθ] becoming [fɪ ft] (/f/
                               and /θ/ are both fricatives, but /t/ is a stop)
      Epenthesis:              insertion of a sound, e.g. Latin "homre" became
                               Spanish "hombre"
      Prothesis:               insertion of vowel sound at beginning of word
      Anaptyxis:               vowel sound with predictable quality is inserted word-
      internally
      Paragoge:         insertion of vowel sound at end of word
      Excrescence:      consonant sound inserted between other consonants
                        (also called stop-intrusion)
      Deletion:         deletion of a sound; e.g. French word-final
                        consonants are deleted when the next word begins
                        with a consonant (but are retained when the
                        following word begins with a vowel)
      Aphaeresis:       vowel sound deleted at beginning of word.
      Syncope:          vowel sound is deleted word-internally.
      Apocope:          vowel sound deleted at end of word.
      Metathesis:       reordering of phonemes; in some dialects of English,
                        the word asked is pronounced [æks]; children's
                        speech shows many cases of metathesis such as
                        aminal for animal.
      Lenition:         consonant changes to a weaker manner of
                        articulation; voiced stopbecomes a fricative, fricative
                        becomes a glide, etc.
      Palatalization:   sound becomes palatal when adjacent to a front
                        vowel
      Compensatory Lengthening:        sound becomes long as a result of
                                       sound loss, e.g. Latin "octo" became
                                       Italian "otto"

Assimilation in English

An interesting observation of assimilation rules is evidenced in the formation of
plurals and the past tense in English. When pluralizing nouns, the last letter is
pronounced as either [s], [z], or [əz]. When forming past tenses of verbs, the -
ed ending is pronounced as either [t], [d], [əd].



                                                                                               15
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)




If you were to sort words into three columns, you would be able to tell why
certain words are followed by certain sounds:


         Plural nouns             Hopefully, you can determine which consonants
  /s/        /z/      /əz/        produce which sounds. In the nouns, /s/ is added
  cats      dads churches         after voiceless consonants, and /z/ is added after
                                  voiced consonants. /əz/ is added after sibilants. For
  tips       bibs    kisses
                                  the verbs, /t/ is added after voiceless consonants,
 laughs     dogs    judges        and /d/ is added after voiced consonants. /əd/ is
         Past Tense               added after alveolar stops. The great thing about
                                  this is that no one ever taught you this in school.
   /t/       /d/      /əd/
                                  But thanks to linguistics, you now know why there
 kissed     loved    patted       are different sounds (because of assimilation rules,
washed jogged waded               the consonants become more like their neighboring
coughed teased seeded             consonants.)


Writing Rules

A general phonological rule is A → B / D __ E (said: A becomes B when it occurs
between D and E) Other symbols in rule writing include: C = any obstruent, V =
any vowel, Ø = nothing, # = word boundary, ( ) = optional, and { } = either/or.
A deletion rule is A → Ø / E __ (A is deleted when it occurs after E) and an
insertion rule is Ø → A / E __ (A is inserted when it occurs after E).
Alpha notation is used to collapse similar assimilation rules into one. C → [Α
voice] / __ [Α voice] (An obstruent becomes voiced when it occurs before a
voiced obstruent AND an obstruent becomes voiceless when it occurs before a
voiceless obstruent.) Similarly, it can be used for dissimilation rules too. C → [-Α
voice] / __ [Α voice] (An obstruent becomes voiced when it occurs before a
voiceless obstruent AND an obstruent becomes voiceless when it occurs before a
voiced obstruent.) Gemination rules are written as C1C2 → C2C2 (for example,
pd→dd)

Syllable Structure




                                                                                                16
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

There are three peaks to a syllable: nucleus (vowel), onset (consonant before
nucleus) and coda (consonant after nucleus.) The onset and coda are both

optional, meaning that a syllable could contain a vowel and nothing else. The
nucleus is required in every syllable by definition. The order of the peaks is
always onset - nucleus - coda. All languages permit open syllables (Consonant +
Vowel), but not all languages allow closed syllables (Consonant + Vowel +
Consonant). Languages that only allow open syllables are called CV languages.
In addition to not allowing codas, some CV languages also have constraints on
the number of consonants allowed in the onset.

The sonority profile dictates that sonority must rise to the nucleus and fall to the
coda in every language. The sonority scale (from most to least sonorous) is
vowels - glides - liquids - nasals - obstruents. Sonority must rise in the onset, but
the sounds cannot be adjacent to or share a place of articulation (except [s] in
English) nor can there be more than two consonants in the onset. This explains
why English allows some consonant combinations, but not others. For example,
price [prajs] is a well-formed syllable and word because the sonority rises in the
onset (p, an obstruent, is less sonorous than r, a liquid); however, rpice [rpajs] is
not a syllable in English because the sonority does not rise in the onset.

The Maximality Condition states that onsets are as large as possible up to the
well-formedness rules of a language. Onsets are always preferred over codas
when syllabifying words. There are also constraints that state the maximum
number of consonants between two vowels is four; onsets and codas have two
consonants maximally; and onsets and codas can be bigger only at the edges of
words.

PART FOUR: SEMANTICS AND PRAGMATICS

Semantics
Lexical semantics is concerned with the meanings of words and the meaning of
relationships among words, while phrasal semantics is concerned with the
meaning of syntactic units larger than the word. Pragmatics is the study of how
context affects meaning, such as how sentences are interpreted in certain
situations.




                                                                                              17
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

Semantic properties are the components of meanings of words. For example, the
semantic property "human" can be found in many words such as parent, doctor,



baby, professor, widow, and aunt. Other semantic properties include animate
objects, male, female, countable items and non-countable items.

The –nyms
Homonyms: different words that are pronounced the same, but may or may not
             be spelled the same (to, two, and too)
Polysemous: word that has multiple meanings that are related conceptually or
             historically (bear can mean to tolerate or to carry or to support)
Homograph: different words that are spelled identically and possibly pronounced
             the same; if they are pronounced the same, they are also
             homonyms (pen can mean writing utensil or cage)
Heteronym: homographs that are pronounced differently (dove the bird and
             dove the past tense of dive)
Synonym:     words that mean the same but sound different (couch and sofa)
Antonym:     words that are opposite in meaning.
Complementary pairs:        alive and dead
Gradable pairs:             big and small (no absolute scale)
Hyponym: set of related words (red, white, yellow, blue are all hyponyms of
             "color")
Metonym: word used in place of another to convey the same meaning (jock
             used for athlete, Washington used for American government,
             crown used for monarchy)
Retronym: expressions that are no longer redundant (silent movie used to be
             redundant because a long time ago, all movies were silent, but this
             is no longer true or redundant)

Thematic Roles
Thematic roles are the semantic relationships between the verbs and noun
phrases of sentences. The following chart shows the thematic roles in
relationship to verbs of sentences:


   Thematic
                                 Description                               Example
     Role
  Agent         the one who performs an action                       Maria ran


                                                                                             18
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

                the person or thing that undergoes an
  Theme                                                              Mary called John
                action


  Location      the place where an action takes place                It rains in Spain
                                                                     Put the cat on the
  Goal          the place to which an action is directed
                                                                     porch
                the place from which an action                       He flew from
  Source
                originates                                           Chicago to LA
                the means by which an action is                      He cuts his hair
  Instrument
                performed                                            with scissors
                                                                     She heard Bob
  Experiencer one who perceives something
                                                                     play the piano
                                                                     The wind
  Causative     a natural force that causes a change                 destroyed the
                                                                     house
                                                                     The tail of the cat
  Possessor     one who has something
                                                                     got caught
  Recipient     one who receives something                           I gave it to the girl



Sentential Meaning
The meaning of sentences is built from the meaning of noun phrases and verbs.
Sentences contain truth conditions if the circumstances in the sentence are true.
Paraphrases are two sentences with the same truth conditions, despite subtle
differences in structure and emphasis. The ball was kicked by the boy is a
paraphrase of the sentence the boy kicked the ball, but they have the same truth
conditions - that a boy kicked a ball. Sometimes the truth of one sentence entails
or implies the truth of another sentence. This is called entailment and the
opposite of this is called contradiction, where one sentence implies the falseness
of another. He was assassinated entails that he is dead. He was assassinated
contradicts with the statement he is alive.

Pragmatics
Pragmatics is the interpretation of linguistic meaning in context. Linguistic
context is discourse that precedes a sentence to be interpreted and situational
context is knowledge about the world. In the following sentences, the kids have


                                                                                             19
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

eaten already and surprisingly, they are hungry, the linguistic context helps to
interpret the second sentence depending on what the first sentence says.

Maxims of Conversation
Grice's maxims for conversation are conventionsof speech such as the maxim of
quantity that states a speaker should be as informative as is required and
neither more nor less. The maxim of relevance essentially states a speaker
should stay on the topic, and the maxim of manner states the speaker should
be brief and orderly, and avoid ambiguity. The fourth maxim, the maxim of
quality, states that a speaker should not lie or make any unsupported claims.

Performative Sentences
In these types of sentences, the speaker is the subject who, by uttering the
sentence, is accomplishing some additional action, such as daring, resigning, or
nominating. These sentences are all affirmative, declarative and in the present
tense. An informal test to see whether a sentence is performative or not is to
insert the words I hereby before the verb.I hereby challenge you to a match or I
hereby fine you $500 are both performative, but I hereby know that girl is not.
Other performative verbs are bet, promise, pronounce, bequeath, swear, testify,
and dismiss.

Presuppositions
These are implicit assumptions required to make a sentence meaningful.
Sentences that contain presuppositions are not allowed in court because
accepting the validity of the statement mean accepting the presuppositions as
well. Have you stopped stealing cars?is not admissible in court because no
matter how the defendant answers, the presupposition that he steals cars
already will be acknowledged. Have you stopped smoking?Implies that you
smoke already.

Deixis
Deixis is reference to a person, object, or event which relies on the situational
context. First and second person pronouns such as my, mine, you, your, yours,
we, ours and us are always deictic because their reference is entirely dependent
on context. Demonstrative articles like this, that, these and those and
expressions of time and place are always deictic as well. In order to understand
what specific times or places such expressions refer to, we also need to know
when or where the utterance was said. If someone says "I'm over here!" you



                                                                                             20
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

would need to know who "I" referred to, as well as where "here" is. Deixis marks
one of the boundaries of semantics and pragmatics.

PART FIVE: NEUROLINGUISTICS

The human brain consists of 10 billion nerve cells (neurons) and billions of fibers
that connect them. These neurons or gray matter form the cortex, the surface
of the brain, and the connecting fibers or white matter form the interior of the
brain. The brain is divided into two hemispheres, the left and right cerebral
hemispheres. These hemispheres are connected by the corpus callosum. In
general, the left hemisphere of the brain controls the right side of the body and
vice versa.


The auditory cortex receives and interprets auditory stimuli, while the visual
cortex receives and interprets visual stimuli. The angular gyrus converts the
auditory stimuli to visual stimuli and vice versa. The motor cortex signals the
muscles to move when we want to talk and is directed by Broca's area. The
nerve fiber connecting Wernicke's and Broca's area is called the arcuate
fasciculus.

Lateralization refers to any cognitive functions that are localized to one side of
the brain or the other. Language is said to be lateralized and processed in the
left hemisphere of the brain. Paul Broca first related language to the left side of
the brain when he noted that damage to the front part of the left hemisphere
(now called Broca's area) resulted in a loss of speech, while damage to the
right side did not. He determined this through autopsies of patients who had
acquired language deficits following brain injuries. A language disorder that
follows a brain lesion is called aphasia, and patients with damage to Broca's
area have slow and labored speech, loss of function words, and poor word order,
yet good comprehension.

Carl Wernicke also used studies of autopsies to describe another type of aphasia
that resulted from lesions in the back portion of the left hemisphere (now called
Wernicke's area.) Unlike Broca's patients, Wernicke's spoke fluently and with
good pronunciation, but with many lexical errors and a difficulty in
comprehension. Broca's and Wernicke's area are the two main regions of the
cortex of the brain related to language processing.



                                                                                             21
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

Aphasics can suffer from anomia, jargon aphasia, and acquired dyslexia.
Anomia is commonly referred to as "tip of the tongue" phenomenon and many

aphasics experience word finding difficulty on a regular basis. Jargon aphasia
results in the substitution of one word or sound for another. Some aphasics may
substitute similar words for each other, such as table for chair, or they may
substitute completely unrelated words, such as chair for engine. Others may
pronounce table as sable, substituting an s sound for a t sound. Aphasics who
became dyslexic after brain damage are called acquired dyslexics. When reading
aloud words printed on cards, the patients produced the following substitutions:


Stimuli Response One Response Two
  Act          Play                Play
 South        East                West
                                Medicine
 Heal         Pain

The substitution of phonologically similar words, such as pool and tool, also
provides evidence that a human's mental lexicon is organized by both phonology
and semantics.

Broca's aphasics and some acquired dyslexics are unable to read function words,
and when presented with them on the cards, the patients say no, as shown in
the following example:


Stimuli One Response Stimuli Two Response
   Witch        Witch          Which             no!
    Hour         Time            Our             no!
   Wood         Wood           Would             no!


The patient's errors suggest our mental dictionary is further organized into parts
consisting of major content words (first stimuli) and grammatical words (second
stimuli.)

In addition, split-brain patients (those who have had their corpus callosum
severed) provide evidence for language lateralization. If an object is placed in
the left hand of split-brain patient whose vision is cut off, the person cannot


                                                                                             22
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

name the object, but will know how to use it. The information is sent to the right
side of the brain, but cannot be relayed to the left side for linguistic naming.

However, if the object is placed in the person's right hand, the person can
immediately name it because the information is sent directly to the left
hemisphere.

Dichotic listening is another experimental technique, using auditory signals.
Subjects hear a different sound in each ear, such as boy in the left ear and girl in
the right ear or water rushing in the left ear and a horn honking in the right ear.
When asked to state what they heard in each ear, subjects are more frequently
correct in reporting linguistic stimuli in the right ear (girl) and nonverbal stimuli
in the left ear (water rushing.) This is because the left side of the brain is
specialized for language and a word heard in the right ear will transfer directly to
the left side of the body because of the contralateralization of the brain.
Furthermore, the right side of the brain is specialized for nonverbal stimuli, such
as music and environmental sounds, and a noise heard in the left ear will
transfer directly to the right side of the brain.

PART SIX: CHILD LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND SECOND LANGUAGE
          ACQUISITION

Linguistic competence develops in stages, from babbling to one word to two
word, then telegraphic speech. Babbling is now considered the earliest form of
language acquisition because infants will produce sounds based on what
language input they receive. One word sentences (holophrastic speech) are
generally monosyllabic in consonant-vowel clusters. During two word stage,
there are no syntactic or morphological markers, no inflections for plural or past
tense, and pronouns are rare, but the intonation contour extends over the whole
utterance. Telegraphic speech lacks function words and only carries the open
class content words, so that the sentences sound like a telegram.

Three theories
The three theories of language acquisition: imitation, reinforcement and
analogy, do not explain very well how children acquire language. Imitation does
not work because children produce sentences never heard before, such as "cat
stand up table." Even when they try to imitate adult speech, children cannot
generate the same sentences because of their limited grammar. And children



                                                                                              23
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

who are unable to speak still learn and understand the language, so that when
they overcome their speech impairment they immediately begin speaking the

language. Reinforcement also does not work because it actually seldomly occurs
and when it does, the reinforcement is correcting pronunciation or truthfulness,
and not grammar. A sentence such as "apples are purple" would be corrected
more often because it is not true, as compared to a sentence such as "apples is
red" regardless of the grammar. Analogy also cannot explain language
acquisition. Analogy involves the formation of sentences or phrases by using
other sentences as samples. If a child hears the sentence, "I painted a red barn,"
he can say, by analogy, "I painted a blue barn." Yet if he hears the sentence, "I
painted a barn red," he cannot say "I saw a barn red." The analogy did not work
this time, and this is not a sentence of English.

Acquisitions
Phonology: A child's error in pronunciation is not random, but rule-governed.
Typical phonological rules include: consonant cluster simplification (spoon
becomes poon), devoicing of final consonants (dog becomes dok), voicing of
initial consonants (truck becomes druck), and consonant harmony (doggy
becomes goggy, or big becomes gig.)

Morphology: An overgeneralization of constructed rules is shown when children
treat irregular verbs and nouns as regular. Instead of went as the past tense of
go, children use goed because the regular verbs add an -ed ending to form the
past tense. Similarly, children use gooses as the plural of goose instead of geese,
because regular nouns add an -s in the plural.

The "Innateness Hypothesis" of child language acquisition, proposed by
Noam Chomsky, states that the human species is prewired to acquire language,
and that the kind of language is also determined. Many factors have led to this
hypothesis such as the ease and rapidity of language acquisition despite
impoverished input as well as the uniformity of languages. All children will learn
a language, and children will also learn more than one language if they are
exposed to it. Children follow the same general stages when learning a language,
although the linguistic input is widely varied.

The poverty of the stimulus states that children seem to learn or know the
aspects of grammar for which they receive no information. In addition, children



                                                                                             24
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

do not produce sentences that could not be sentences in some human language.
The principles of Universal Grammar underlie the specific grammars of all

languages and determine the class of languages that can be acquired
unconsciously without instruction. It is the genetically determined faculty of the
left hemisphere, and there is little doubt that the brain is specially equipped for
acquisition of human language.

The "Critical Age Hypothesis" suggests that there is a critical age for
language acquisition without the need for special teaching or learning. During
this critical period, language learning proceeds quickly and easily. After this
period, the acquisition of grammar is difficult, and for some people, never fully
achieved. Cases of children reared in social isolation have been used for testing
the critical age hypothesis. None of the children who had little human contact
were able to speak any language once reintroduced into society. Even the
children who received linguistic input after being reintroduced to society were
unable to fully develop language skills. These cases of isolated children, and of
deaf children, show that humans cannot fully acquire any language to which they
are exposed unless they are within the critical age. Beyond this age, humans are
unable to acquire much of syntax and inflectional morphology.

Second Language Acquisition Teaching Methods
Grammar-translation: The student memorizes words, inflected words, and
                     syntactic rules and uses them to translate from native
                     to target language and vice versa; most commonly
                     used method in schools because it does not require
                     teacher to be fluent; however, least effective method
                     of teaching.
Direct method:       The native language is not used at all in the
                     classroom, and the student must learn the new
                     language without formal instruction; based on
                     theories of first language acquisition.
Audio-lingual:       Heavy use of dialogs and audio, based on the
                     assumption that language learning is acquired mainly
                     through imitation, repetition, and reinforcement;
                     influenced by psychology.




                                                                                             25
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

Natural Approach:            Emphasis on vocabulary and not grammar; focus on
                             meaning, not form; use of authentic materials instead
                             of textbook

Silent Way:              Teachers remain passive observers while students
                         learn, which is a process of personal growth; no
                         grammatical explanation or modeling by the teacher.
Total Physical Response: Students play active role as listener and performer,
                         must respond to imperative drills with physical action.
Suggestopedia:           Students always remain comfortable and relaxed and
                         learn through memorization of meaningful texts,
                         although the goal is understanding
Community Language Learning: Materials are developed as course progresses
                                and teacher understands what students need
                                and want to learn; learning involves the whole
                                person and language is seen as more than just
                                communication.
Community Language Teaching: Incorporates all components of language and
                                helps students with various learning styles; use
                                of communication-based activities with
                                authentic materials, needs of learner are taken
                                into consideration when planning topics and
                                objectives.

Four skill areas
The four skill areas of learning a foreign language need to be addressed
consistently and continually. Good lesson plans incorporate all four: Listening,
Speaking, Reading (and Vocabulary), and Writing (and Grammar).
Native speakers do not learn the skill areas separately, nor do they use them
separately, so they shouldn’t be taught separately. However, it is easy to fall into
the trap of teaching about the language, instead of actually teaching the
language. Most textbooks resort to teaching grammar and vocabulary lists and
nothing more.




                                                                                             26
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)




PART SEVEN: SOCIOLINGUISTICS

A dialect is a variety of language that is systematically different from other
varieties of the same language. The dialects of a single language are mutually
intelligible, but when the speakers can no longer understand each other, the
dialects become languages. Geographical regions are also considered when
dialects become languages. Swedish, Norwegian, and Danish are all considered
separate languages because of regular differences in grammar and the countries
in which they are spoken, yet Swedes, Norwegians, and Danes can all
understand one another. Hindi and Urdu are considered mutually intelligible
languages when spoken, yet the writing systems are different. On the other
hand, Mandarin and Cantonese are mutually unintelligible languages when
spoken, yet the writing systems are the same.

A dialect is considered standard if it is used by the upper class, political leaders,
in literature and is taught in schools as the correct form of the language. Overt
prestige refers to this dominant dialect. A non-standard dialect is associated with
covert prestige and is an ethnic or regional dialect of a language. These non-
standard dialects are just as linguistically sophisticated as the standard dialect,
and judgments to the inferiority of them are based on social or racist judgments.
African-American English contains many regular differences of the standard
dialect. These differences are the same as the differences among many of the
world's dialects. Phonological differences include r and l deletion of words like
poor (pa) and all (awe.) Consonant cluster simplification also occurs (passed
pronounced like pass), as well as a loss of interdental fricatives. Syntactic
differences include the double negative and the loss of and habitual use of the
verb "be." He late means he is late now, but he be late means he is always late.

A lingua franca is a major language used in an area where speakers of more
than one language live that permits communication and commerce among them.
English is called the lingua franca of the whole world, while French used to be
the lingua franca of diplomacy.




                                                                                              27
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

A pidgin is a rudimentary language of few lexical items and less complex
grammatical rules based on another language. No one learns a pidgin as a native
language, but children do learn creoles as a first language. Creoles are defined
as pidgins that are adopted by a community as its native tongue.

Besides dialects, speakers may use different styles or registers (such as
contractions) depending on the context. Slang may also be used in speech, but
is not often used in formal situations or writing. Jargon refers to the unique
vocabulary pertaining to a certain area, such as computers or medicine. Words
or expressions referring to certain acts that are forbidden or frowned upon are
considered taboo. These taboo words produce euphemisms, words or phrases
that replace the expressions that are being avoided.

The use of words may indicate a society's attitude toward sex, bodily functions or
religious beliefs, and they may also reflect racism or sexism in a society.
Language itself is not racist or sexist, but the society may be. Such insulting
words may reinforce biased views, and changes in society may be reflected in
the changes in language.




                                                                                             28
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)




PART EIGHT: HISTORICAL LINGUISTICS

Languages that evolve from a common source are genetically related. These
languages were once dialects of the same language. Earlier forms of Germanic
languages, such as German, English, and Swedish were dialects of Proto-
Germanic, while earlier forms of Romance languages, such as Spanish, French,
and Italian were dialects of Latin. Furthermore, earlier forms of Proto-Germanic
and Latin were once dialects of Indo-European.

Linguistic changes like sound shift are found in the history of all languages, as
evidenced by the regular sound correspondences that exist between different
stages of the same language, different dialects, and different languages. Words,
morphemes, and phonemes may be altered, added or lost. The meaning of
words may broaden, narrow or shift. New words may be introduced into a
language by borrowing, or by coinage, blends and acronyms. The lexicon may
also shrink as older words become obsolete.

Change comes about as a result of the restructuring of grammar by children
learning the language. Grammars seem to become simple and regular, but these
simplifications may be compensated for by more complexities. Sound changes
can occur because of assimilation, a process of ease of articulation. Some
grammatical changes are analogic changes, generalizations that lead to more
regularity, such as sweeped instead of swept.

The study of linguistic change is called historical and comparative linguistics.
Linguists identify regular sound correspondences using the comparative method
among the cognates (words that developed from the same ancestral language)
of related languages. They can restructure an earlier protolanguage and this
allows linguists to determine the history of a language family.

Old English, Middle English, Modern English
Old English     499-1066 CE Beowulf
Middle English 1066-1500 CE Canterbury Tales


                                                                                             29
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

Modern English 1500-present Shakespeare




Phonological change:          Between 1400 and 1600 CE, the Great Vowel Shift
                              took place. The seven long vowels of Middle English
                              underwent changes. The high vowels [i] and [u]
                              became the diphthongs [aj] and [aw]. The long
                              vowels increased tongue height and shifted upward,
                              and [a] was fronted. Many of the spelling
                              inconsistencies of English are because of the Great
                              Vowel Shift. Our spelling system still reflects the way
                              words were pronounced before the shift took place.
Morphological change:         Many Indo-European languages had extensive case
                              endings that governed word order, but these are no
                              longer found in Romance languages or English.
                              Although pronouns still show a trace of the case
                              system (he vs. him), English uses prepositions to
                              show the case. Instead of the dative case (indirect
                              objects), English usually the words to or for. Instead
                              of the genitive case, English uses the word ofor 's
                              after a noun to show possession. Other cases include
                              the nominative (subject pronouns), accusative (direct
                              objects), and vocative.
Syntactic change:             Because of the lack of the case system, word order
                              has become more rigid and strict in Modern English.
                              Now it is strictly Subject - Verb - Object order.
Orthographic change:          Consonant clusters have become simplified, such as
                              hlaf becoming loaf and hnecca becoming neck.
                              However, some of these clusters are still written, but
                              are no longer pronounced, such as gnaw, write, and
                              dumb.
Lexical change:               Old English borrowed place names from Celtic, army,
                              religious and educational words from Latin, and
                              everyday words from Scandinavian. Angle and Saxon
                              (German dialects) form the basis of Old English
                              phonology, morphology, syntax and lexicon.




                                                                                              30
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)




PART NINE: CLASSIFICATION OF LANGUAGES

Indo-European family of languages
     Italic (Latin)
         o Romance
                  Catalan
                  French
                  Italian
                  Occitan (Provençal)
                  Portuguese
                  Rhaeto-Romansch
                  Romanian
                  Spanish
     Germanic
         o North Germanic
                  Danish
                  Faroese
                  Icelandic
                  Norwegian
                  Swedish
         o East Germanic
                  Gothic (extinct)
         o West Germanic
                  Afrikaans
                  Dutch
                  English
                  Flemish
                  Frisian
                  German
                  Yiddish
     Slavic
         o Western
                  Czech
                  Polish




                                                                                            31
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

              Slovak
             Sorbian
   o     Eastern
             Belarusian
             Russian
             Ukrainian
   o     Southern
             Bulgarian
             Croatian
             Macedonian
             Old Church Slavonic
             Serbian
             Slovene
Baltic
   o     Latvian
   o     Lithuanian
   o     Old Prussian (extinct)
Celtic
   o   Brythonic
            Breton
            Cornish (extinct)
            Gaulish (extinct)
            Welsh
   o Goidelic
            Irish
            Manx Gaelic (extinct)
            Scots Gaelic
Hellenic (Greek)
Albanian
Armenian
Anatolian (extinct)
Tocharian (extinct)
Indo-Iranian
   o Indo-Aryan (Indic)
            Assamese
            Bengali
            Bihari
            Gujarati




                                                                                        32
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

                   Hindi-Urdu
                 Marathi
                 Punjabi
                 Romani
                 Sanskrit
                 Sindhi
                 Singhalese
         o   Iranian
                 Avestan
                 Balochi
                 Farsi (Persian)
                 Kurdish
                 Pashtu (Afghan)
                 Sogdian


Uralic (or Finno-Ugric) is the other major family of languages spoken on the
European continent. Finnish, Estonian and Hungarian are examples.

Afro-Asiatic languages are spoken in Northern Africa and the Middle East. They
include Berber, Egyptian, Omotic and Cushitic languages (Somali, Iraqw) as well
as the modern Semitic languages of Hebrew, Arabic and Amharic, in addition to
languages spoken in biblical times, such as Aramaic, Akkadian, Babylonian,
Canaanite, and Phoenician.

The Altaic languages are classified as Japanese and Korean, though some
linguists separate these languages into their own groups.

Sino-Tibetan languages include Mandarin, Hakka, Wu, Burmese, Tibetan, and
all of the Chinese "dialects."

Austro-tai languages include Indonesian, Javanese and Thai; while the Asiatic
group includes Vietnamese.

The Dravidian languages of Tamil and Telugu are spoken in southeastern India
and Sri Lanka.

The Caucasian language family consists of 40 different languages, and is
divided into Cartvelian (south Caucasian), North-West Caucasian and North-East



                                                                                            33
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

Caucasian language groups. Some languages are Georgian, Megrelian, Chechen,
Ingush Avarian, Lezgian and Dargin. These languages are mostly spoken in
Georgia, Turkey, Syria, Iran, Jordan and parts of the Russian federation.

The Niger-Congo family includes most of the African languages. About 1,500
languages belong to this group, including the Bantu languages of Swahili,
Tswana, Xhosa, Zulu, Kikuyu, and Shona. Other languages are Ewe, Mina,
Yoruba, Igbo, Wolof, Kordofanian and Fulfulde.

Other African language groups are Nilo-Saharan, which includes 200 languages
spoken in Central and Eastern Africa; and Khoisan, the click languages of
southern Africa. The Khoisan group only contains about 30 languages, most of
which are spoken in Namibia and Botswana.

The Austronesian family also contains about 900 languages, spoken all over
the globe. Hawaiian, Maori, Tagalog, and Malay are all representatives of this
language family.

Many languages are, or were, spoken in North and South America by the native
peoples before the European conquests. Knowledge of these languages is
limited, and because many of the languages are approaching extinction, linguists
have little hope of achieving a complete understanding of the Amerindian
language families.




                                                                                             34
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)




Linguistics 101: Introduction to Linguistics I - Fall 1996
Anthony Kroch
614 Williams Hall
898-3212
kroch@change.ling.upenn.edu

Linguistics 101 is an introduction to linguistics from the point of view of its logical
and mathematical foundations. Its goal is to teach the basic structural properties
of natural language sentences in their syntactic, semantic and pragmatic aspects,
as well as the elements of automata theory and logic needed to describe natural
language precisely. The course begins with a discussion of the philosophical and
psychological foundations of modern linguistics and moves from there to more
technical matters.

Linguistics 101 has no prerequisites and fulfills the formal reasoning requirement
in SAS. It is also a basic part of the Linguistics major and of the interdisciplinary
Cognitive Science minor. Related courses are Linguistics 150: Introduction to
Syntax and Linguistics 105: Introduction to Cognitive Science.

Fall 1996 Course Syllabus
Instructor:             Teaching Assistants:
Anthony Kroch           Alan Lee &Rashmi Prasad
614 Williams Hall       429 Williams Hall
898-3212                898-6050
Email addresses:
kroch@change.ling.upenn.edu
aleewk@babel.ling
rjprasad@babel.ling


Texts: 1. Steven Pinker. The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow.
       2. Course Bulkpack.




                                                                                                35
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

This course will introduce you to linguistics, one of the core disciplines of the
modern science of mind, from a mathematical point of view. Linguistics is a new
science, less than a century old; but we have learned much in this brief period of

scientific study about the uniquely human capacity to code abstract thought in
communicable form. In the course of this semester, we will explore some of the
results obtained in the scientific investigation of language, especially what has
been discovered regarding the formal structure of our linguistic capacities and
how our biologically endowed language faculty allows us to represent meanings
as structured sequences of words. The course will show you that it is possible to
study human language rigorously and scientifically and that such study leads to a
conception of language quite different from our everyday common-sense notions
of the subject.

Requirements for the course consist of a midterm and a final exam, as well as a
series of homework assignments, roughly one per week through the semester.
The course is organized around an interesting recent book by Steven Pinker
entitled "The Language Instinct." This book is not a textbook but it covers in a
readable way the basic aspects of the study of language. There will also be
readings to supplement the Pinker book, which are available in the course
bulkpack.

Topic 1: Introduction. How can there be a scientific study of language? How
are language and thought related? What grammar is and how we learn it. What
we can learn about language from the study of people who lose it or are kept by
circumstances from acquiring language in the normal way.

Reading: Steven Pinker. The Language Instinct. Chapters 1-3.
         Perlmutter. "The Language of the Deaf."
         Pullum. "The Great Eskimo Vocabulary Hoax."

Topic 2: Formal grammar - the mathematical structure of language.
This section of the course introduces the idea that languages can be analyzed
mathematically. We discuss the relationship between computer languages and
natural human languages and the notion of language complexity. We also
introduce the concept of a phrase structure grammar, which underlies both
computer and natural languages and the notion of a machine that automatically
recognizes and parses sentences.


                                                                                             36
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



Reading: Michael Sipser. Introduction to the Theory of Computation. Chapters
         0-2.

Topic 3: Syntax - the structure of sentences in natural language. This
section will be a focus of the course. Here we will learn how sentences are built
up out of words and why speakers are able to construct sentences of any degree
of complexity with a fixed vocabulary. We will also learn why linguists say that
language is a rule-governed system and what some of the linguistic rules are
that are used in the syntactic description of language. We will see how these
rules determine both the structure of sentences and the conditions under which
strings of words are interpretable as sentences of a language instead of being
meaningless word salad.

Reading: Pinker, chapter 4.

Topic 4: Semantics. What is 'meaning'? Lexical semantics.Truth conditional
semantics.The distinction between sense and reference.Semantic
compositionality.The relationship between syntax and semantics.

Reading: O'Grady, Dobrovolsky and Aronoff. Contemporary Linguistics: an
         Introduction. Chapter 6.
         Chierchia and McConnell-Ginet. Meaning and Grammar: An
         Introduction to Semantics. chapters 1-2.

Topic 5: Pragmatics. How do speakers take account of listeners' knowledge
and beliefs in formulating utterances? What inferences beyond literal meaning
listeners draw from what is said to them. What acts can be performed merely by
speaking. How context determines reference. The communicative effects of using
particular syntactic constructions.

Reading: Finegan and Besnier. Language, Its Structure and Use. chaps. 7, 10.

Topic 6: Language in real time. How do listeners understand what speakers
say? How do speakers convert thought into speech? How psycholinguists study
what goes on inside our heads without looking inside them.




                                                                                             37
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

Reading: Pinker, Chapters 7-8.
         Akmajian et al. Linguistics: an Introduction to Language and
         Communication. Chapter 10.
Assignment 1: Grammaticality
This assignment is due at the beginning of the Monday lecture class on
September 16. Be sure to write your TA's name and your recitation day on the
top of your assignment. All future assignments should also be handed in at the
beginning of the Monday lecture class in the week that they are due.
Assignments will be returned in recitation sections, beginning on the Friday after
they are handed in.
1. Prescriptive & Descriptive Grammar
In order to answer this question, you will have to be familiar with some notation
used in linguistics. An asterisk (*) is used to mark sentences which are
ungrammatical. Grammatical sentences are unmarked. Recall that
"ungrammatical" is used in the descriptive sense of interest to linguists, not in
the prescriptive sense. Pinker, whose use of the term we will follow, defines
"grammatical" as "well formed according to consistent rules in the dialect of [a]
speaker..." (p. 31). The dialect we have in mind here is the spoken language of
the general college-educated American population.

(A) Look at the sentences in (1a) and (1b). Why is (1b) ungrammatical?

      a. Two paintings are on the wall.
(1)
      b. *Two paintings is on the wall

(B) Now consider the sentences in (2). Do the grammaticality judgments
indicated correspond to your own? Assuming the correctness of these
judgments, what rule or rules could a speaker use to generate the grammatical
sentences (2a) and (2c) but not generate the ungrammatical sentence in (2b).
Keep in mind that your rule(s) should also be able to account for the sentences
in

(1). How does your description of the sentences in (1) and (2) differ from
prescriptions that govern standard usage?

(2) a. There are two paintings on the wall.
      b. *There is two paintings on the wall



                                                                                                38
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

   c. There's two paintings on the wall.




(C) The sentences in (3) show a similar pattern to the pattern seen in (1).
However, the pattern in (4) is different from the one in (2). Write a description
of the rules needed to generate the grammatical sentences in (3) and (4). Your
rules should not generate the ungrammatical sentences.

(3) a. A painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee are hanging on the wall.
   b. *A painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee is hanging on the wall


(4) a. *There are a painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee hanging on the
       wall
   b. There is a painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee hanging on the wall.
   c. There's a painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee hanging on the wall.


2. Grammaticality Judgments
(A) Decide whether the phrases in (1) - (15) are grammatical in your spoken
dialect. (If you are not a native speaker of English, consult a native speaker for
judgments). Mark ungrammatical sentences with an asterisk ( * ) and say briefly
what's wrong with them.
(1) To the bank.
(2) The rat the cat the dog bit chased ran.
(3) The cat the dog bit ran.
(4) Being so flat, the Dutch bicycle everywhere.
(5) Who do you wonder whether they will come.
(6) Ivan a tin of caviar ate quickly.
(7) Its mayor praised her village.
(8) If you go to school, there's an elephant on the corner.
(9) Susan told John that washing herself in public is a bad idea.
(10) The candy ate the boy.
(11) Immediately he opened the door he saw the murderer standing there.
(12) The police officer arrested Sam and I.



                                                                                              39
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

(13) Earlobe seven by hexed fruitless.
(14) Go take dog for a walk!
(15) Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.

(B) Often it is possible to make sense of an ungrammatical sentence. Likewise a
grammatical sentence can be incomprehensible. Mark the incomprehensible but
grammatical sentences in (A) with a number sign (#).

3. American Sign Language
Read the discussion of deaf sign language in Pinker and the Perlmutter article in
the bulk pack before answering this question.

Sign language users all over the world have been struggling for years to
eradicate the notion that because they do not use speech, their communications
systems are not "real" languages. One characteristic of languages in general is
that there is an arbitrary relation between words and what they represent. You
can't hear the French word 'chien,' for example, and know by its sound that it
refers to what the English word 'dog' refers to. Critics of sign languages have
often described them as "iconic," as a series of pictures and gestures for acting
out the real world -- and thus dismissed them as nothing more than complex
mime. Consider the issue of iconicity in American Sign Language (ASL) in light of
the following evidence.

(A) The signs for male and female:

original:
female: running thumb along jaw toward chin, mimicking bonnet strings
male: grasping an invisible cap near the forehead

current:
female: thumb on chin, with a hand shape as if thumbing your nose at someone
male: thumb on forehead, same handshape.
-- How have these signs changed over time? How does this development affect
the debate over whether signs are iconic or not?

(B) First person pronouns:
When hearing children are first learning to speak, they often display a charming
tendency to confuse the pronouns 'you' and 'me.' When asked, "Do you want


                                                                                             40
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

milk," they reply, "Yes, you want milk," believing that they are describing
themselves. In a curious parallel, deaf children who are learning to sign will
display, at the same age, the tendency to confuse the signs for 'you' and 'me.'

The adult will point to the child and ask a question, and the child will point at the
adult in reply, even though, once again, these children are describing
themselves.

-- Why do you think children make these mistakes? Based on the assumption
that ASL is a true language, would you expect hearing children (who are not
exposed to ASL) to make the same mistake as deaf children when responding to
pointing? Why or why not?

(C) Character placing:
When telling a story, an ASL signer is likely to name the characters at the
beginning (or whenever they appear) and in doing so, to "place" them at some
location in space (one to the left, and one to the right, for example). From that
point on, the signer will refer to those locations by pointing instead of repeating
the names.

-- Does these rules for pointing remind you of anything in spoken language?

(D) Handshapes:
While fingerspelling is not a grammatical part of ASL, many signs in ASL are
signed with the handshape of the first letter in the English word -- 'language' is
signed with the "l" shape, 'class' with the "c" shape, and 'water' with a "w". The
colors blue, purple, green, orange and yellow are all signed with the same
motion, shaking the initial letter (b, p, g, o, or y) back and forth. 'Apple' is an "a"
shape rotated at cheek level. At the same time, 'onion' is an "x" shape moved
the same way, so this pattern does not always not hold.

-- How do these facts impact upon the iconicity debate?

(E) Iconicity in spoken language:
There are iconic elements in ordinary spoken English. Give some examples. In
what ways are similar to and/or different from iconic features of ASL?




                                                                                              41
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)




Assignment 2: Language, Grammar, and Thought

1.Tongan Syntax
The following (simplified) sentences are from Tongan, a Polynesian language
spoken on the island of Tonga in the Pacific. Each sentence is glossed (directly
translated) and an English translation is also provided. The following
abbreviations are used: Pr=present tense, Pst =past tense, Nom=nominative
case, Acc=accusative case, 1ps=First person singular, 2ps=Second person
singular.
Answer the following questions based on your observations of sentences (1)-(4):
(a) what would you say is the main structural difference between Tongan and
English? (b) how is tense realized in Tongan? (c) are the nouns marked in any
particular way?

(1)   oku      ui     ehe- tamasiae-             tangata
      Pr       call Nom child Acc                man
       'The   child calls the man.'
(2)   oku      kai    ehe- fefine ae-             ufi
      Pr       eat Nom- womanAcc-                 yam
       'The   woman eats the yam.'
(3)   nae      ako ehe- tamasi ae-                lesoni
      Pst      study Nom- child Acc-              lesson
       'The   child studied the lesson.'
(4)   nae     haka ehe- fefine             ae-    ika
      Pst     boil     Nom- woman Acc- fish
       'The woman boiled the fish.'

How are the following sentences structurally different from the ones above? Also,
why is the 1ps pronoun in (5) different from the one in (8)? Similarly, why is the
2ps in (6) different from the one in (7)?
(5)   nae        ku         ui       ae-         tangata
      Pst        1ps        call     Acc-        man



                                                                                                  42
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

        'I called the man.'
(6)     oku      ke        kai      ae-       laise
        Pr       2ps       eat      Acc-      rice
        'You eat rice.'

(7)    oku      manako koe          ehe-     tangata
       Pr       like       2ps      Nom- man
        'The man likes you.'

(8)     nae      tokonia au         ehe-      kakai
        Pst      help      1ps      Nom- man
        'People helped me.'

If you have adequately analyzed the above sentences, you should now be able
to do some simple translations from English into Tongan! Try translating the
following:

The   man scares the child. ('scare' = 'fakailifiai')
The   woman saw me. ('see' = 'vakai')
You   ate the fish.
You   like me.

2. TheRelationship betweenLanguage andThought.
It is by now a well known fact (and those of you who have tried to learn a
foreign language will undoubtedly admit this) that certain things can be
expressed more conveniently in some languages than in others. While one
language may have a special word to refer exclusively to a particular object or
notion, in another language this object or notion can be described only by using
a whole phrase or sentence. For example, in Tuvaluan, a language spoken by the
Polynesian inhabitants of a group of islands in the Central Pacific, there are
different words to refer to many different types of coconut, which need to be
described at great length in English. Here are a few examples:
pii : drinking coconut, with little flesh and much water, at a stage when the
       water is maximally sweet
mukomuko: young coconut with some flesh in it, before it has become too solid
uto :         coconut at the stage when its husk can be chewed on and its water
              is still sweet
motomoto : same as mukomuko, but with firmer flesh

                                                                                                43
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

niu :        coconut ripe enough for its flesh to be grated
uttanu :     mature coconut whose sprout has already pierced through the husk
             and whose water has turned into an edible spongious solid kernel



How much can we conclude from examples like this one about the relationship
between the language that people speak and the way that they think? Do
examples like this one support the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis?.

3. Grammar of Non-Standard English
Study the following three groups of sentences of Appalachian English. The
sentences of each group share a grammatical feature not found in Standard
English. Describe these features.
(1) Boy, I started to runnin'.
     A vein in his nose bursted and he went to hemorragin'.
     She practically raised 'im 'til he got up to walkin'.
     Just recently, I had an aunt to come from Texas.
     Usually, I hafta have somebody else to do it.
     (note: 'went' and 'got' is roughly the equivalent of Standard English's
    'started')
(2) I'd go out and cut me a limb off a tree, get me a good straight one.
     We had us a cabin, built us a log cabin back over there.
     And then you'd get you a bowl of ice water.
     He wanted some straw to build him a house out of.
     I'm gonna write me a letter to the President.
(3) I got some kin people lived up there.
     He's the funny lookin' character plays baseball.
    'Cause there was this vampire that killed people come in the house.
     My grandma's got this thing tells me about when to plant.

4. Two puzzles
      (a). Included in your bulkpack there is an excerpt from an interview with
      Miss Manners. What point relevant to a linguistics course does she make
      in her discussion of ettiquette books?
      (b). Right after the Miss Manners interview there is a page with a
Domino's Pizza advertisement. Why do you think this page was included in the
course bulkpack?




                                                                                            44
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)



Assignment 3: Sentences as Structured Objects

1.Expressivity of Language
Consider the following sentences :

a) I hate war.
b) You know that I hate war.
c) He says that you know that I hate war.
Construct a sentence that includes sentence (c) and then construct another
sentence that includes your new sentence. Can you repeat the process again to
create an even longer sentence? How long do you think you can go on? Why?

2. Structural Ambiguity
Our syntactic knowledge goes beyond our ability to decide which strings are
grammatical and which are not. It accounts for the structural ambiguity of
expressions like synthetic buffalo hides. The ambiguity results from the fact that
synthetic can modify buffalo hides or simply hides to result in two different
interpretations. It is therefore due to the syntactic structure that the expression
has two meanings and not due to any ambiguous words. Paraphrase each of the
following sentences in two different ways to show that you understand the
ambiguity involved :
       a) Smoking grass can be nauseating.
       b) Rob finally decided on the boat.
       c) Old men and women are hard to live with.
       d) That sheepdog is too hairy to eat.
       e) Terry loves his wife and so do I.
       f) They said she would go yesterday.

Is the type of ambiguity in the above sentences different from the ambiguity in
the following sentences :
        g) I walked by the bank yesterday.
        h) Thomas Jefferson ate his cottage cheese with relish.

3. Mathematical exercises
Do exercises 3, 4, and 8 on page 24 at the end of chapter 0 of Sipser.




                                                                                             45
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

Assignment 4: Mathematical preliminaries

A.Alphabets, Strings and Languages
Let be the alphabet containing the symbols a and b. In other words: = {a, b}.
A language L contains all strings over which either begin with a and end with b,



or begin with b and end with a. State which of the following strings belong to L,
and which do not:
      i) ab
      ii) baa
      iii) abba
      iv) baba
      v) bubba
      vi) b

Concatenation is an operation on strings where one string is appended to the
end of another string. For example, if we have two strings xy and yx, we can do
the operation xy o yx (where 'o' is the symbol denoting concatenation) to yield a
new string xyyx. Now, when we concatenate certain strings from the language L,
we get a new string which still belongs to L. For example, the strings aab and
abb are valid strings in L. The operation aab o abb gives us the new string
aababb which still belongs to L (since it starts with an a and ends with ab).
However, the concatenation of two valid strings of L does not always yield a new
string which also belongs to L. Provide some counterexamples of when the
concatenation of two strings of L results in a new string which does NOT belong
to L.

B. Relations and Functions
The above diagram shows the maternal family tree of a certain family. F is the
set containing all the members of this family. Let M be the set containing all
mothers in F. Hence, M = {Eve, Jennifer, Mary, Kate}. Let the relation R = "is
the mother of". The statement mRf, where m is an element of set M and f is an
element of set F, simply says that m is the mother of f. We can show this
relationship diagramatically:

The above diagram shows a normal relation, where an element in the first group
can map onto more than one element in the second group.



                                                                                             46
(Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)

   a) Now, let S be the set containing all sons in F. List the set S.
   b) where R = "is the son of", s S, and f F.
   c) c) Briefly explain why this relation is a function.
   d) d) Let G be the set of siblings in F. List G. Is gRf a function, where R = "is
      the sibling of",
   e) G, and f F.

C. Finite State Automata
Do Exercises 1.1, 1.2, 1.3 and 1.4a (on page 75) from Chapter 1 of Sipser.

Assignment 5: Finite State Automata and Regular Grammars

1. Spot the Problem
A language L is described as follows:
L = all strings that begin with a 0, end with a 2, and contain at most three 1s
The alphabet of this language is {0, 1, 2}
a) Give an example of a string in this language which does not contain a 1.
b) Give an example of a string in this language of length 10.
c) Does the following finite state accept the language L? If not, how would you
change it so that it does accept exactly the language L? (Hint: You need to add
an extra state.)

2. Introduction to the trees program.
Download the Trees 2 program by following the instructions on the Trees web
page and the grammars G1, and G2, and G3 . For each of the grammars answer
the following questions.
a) Describe the language generated by the grammar.
b) Draw a finite state automaton that accepts the same language.

Assignment 6: More Machines and Grammars

1.More Finite Automata
Consider a finite automata whose input alphabet is S = {the, old, man, men, is,
are, here, and}.
a) Construct a state diagram for an automaton which accepts the following
language: {the man is here, the men are here}.




                                                                                             47
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets
Linguistics fact sheets

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

An Introduction to Morphology
An Introduction to MorphologyAn Introduction to Morphology
An Introduction to MorphologySorina Dragusanu
 
Linguistics Notes - 10/7/09
Linguistics Notes - 10/7/09Linguistics Notes - 10/7/09
Linguistics Notes - 10/7/09Daniel
 
Morphology and syntax kelompok
Morphology and syntax kelompokMorphology and syntax kelompok
Morphology and syntax kelompokCukup Saja
 
Structure of words: MORPHEMES
Structure of words: MORPHEMESStructure of words: MORPHEMES
Structure of words: MORPHEMESAlvin Vargas
 
Introduction English Morphology
Introduction English MorphologyIntroduction English Morphology
Introduction English MorphologyDzaki Jabbar Mahdi
 
Morphology, grammar
Morphology, grammarMorphology, grammar
Morphology, grammarSovanna Kakk
 
Morphemes, LANE 333- Dr. Shadia Yousef Banjar .pptx
Morphemes, LANE 333- Dr. Shadia Yousef Banjar .pptxMorphemes, LANE 333- Dr. Shadia Yousef Banjar .pptx
Morphemes, LANE 333- Dr. Shadia Yousef Banjar .pptxDr. Shadia Banjar
 
Morpheme + word formation
Morpheme  + word formationMorpheme  + word formation
Morpheme + word formationJunaid Amjed
 
Morphology ppt for blackboard
Morphology ppt for blackboardMorphology ppt for blackboard
Morphology ppt for blackboardcswstyle
 
Morphology Word and Its Parts (Root and Afixes)
Morphology Word and Its Parts (Root and Afixes)Morphology Word and Its Parts (Root and Afixes)
Morphology Word and Its Parts (Root and Afixes)Reni Riyani
 
What is morphology
What is morphologyWhat is morphology
What is morphologyrashid abbas
 
Bound and free morphemes
Bound and free morphemesBound and free morphemes
Bound and free morphemesLinda Julie
 
A Brief Introduction of Morphology
 A Brief Introduction of Morphology A Brief Introduction of Morphology
A Brief Introduction of Morphologyamna-shahid
 

Was ist angesagt? (20)

Ppt morphology
Ppt morphologyPpt morphology
Ppt morphology
 
An Introduction to Morphology
An Introduction to MorphologyAn Introduction to Morphology
An Introduction to Morphology
 
Linguistics Notes - 10/7/09
Linguistics Notes - 10/7/09Linguistics Notes - 10/7/09
Linguistics Notes - 10/7/09
 
Morphology and syntax kelompok
Morphology and syntax kelompokMorphology and syntax kelompok
Morphology and syntax kelompok
 
Morphology presentation
Morphology presentationMorphology presentation
Morphology presentation
 
Structure of words: MORPHEMES
Structure of words: MORPHEMESStructure of words: MORPHEMES
Structure of words: MORPHEMES
 
Introduction English Morphology
Introduction English MorphologyIntroduction English Morphology
Introduction English Morphology
 
Morphology, grammar
Morphology, grammarMorphology, grammar
Morphology, grammar
 
Morphemes, LANE 333- Dr. Shadia Yousef Banjar .pptx
Morphemes, LANE 333- Dr. Shadia Yousef Banjar .pptxMorphemes, LANE 333- Dr. Shadia Yousef Banjar .pptx
Morphemes, LANE 333- Dr. Shadia Yousef Banjar .pptx
 
Introduction to Morphology
Introduction to MorphologyIntroduction to Morphology
Introduction to Morphology
 
Morpheme + word formation
Morpheme  + word formationMorpheme  + word formation
Morpheme + word formation
 
Morphology
MorphologyMorphology
Morphology
 
Morphology ppt for blackboard
Morphology ppt for blackboardMorphology ppt for blackboard
Morphology ppt for blackboard
 
Morphology Word and Its Parts (Root and Afixes)
Morphology Word and Its Parts (Root and Afixes)Morphology Word and Its Parts (Root and Afixes)
Morphology Word and Its Parts (Root and Afixes)
 
Morphology
MorphologyMorphology
Morphology
 
Additive morpheme
Additive morphemeAdditive morpheme
Additive morpheme
 
What is morphology
What is morphologyWhat is morphology
What is morphology
 
Bound and free morphemes
Bound and free morphemesBound and free morphemes
Bound and free morphemes
 
A Brief Introduction of Morphology
 A Brief Introduction of Morphology A Brief Introduction of Morphology
A Brief Introduction of Morphology
 
Morphology
MorphologyMorphology
Morphology
 

Andere mochten auch

Top 8 production officer resume samples
Top 8 production officer resume samplesTop 8 production officer resume samples
Top 8 production officer resume samplesporichfergu
 
語言學概論Syntax
語言學概論Syntax語言學概論Syntax
語言學概論Syntax棠貝 白
 
語言學概論Morphology 2
語言學概論Morphology 2語言學概論Morphology 2
語言學概論Morphology 2Ja-Jun Liao
 
Exercise answers chapter 1, 2 & 3
Exercise answers chapter 1, 2 & 3Exercise answers chapter 1, 2 & 3
Exercise answers chapter 1, 2 & 3Hariz Mustafa
 
Identification Of Morphemes
Identification Of MorphemesIdentification Of Morphemes
Identification Of Morphemesnishantl
 
Characteristics of human language
Characteristics of human languageCharacteristics of human language
Characteristics of human languageAlan Bessette
 
Competence and Performance
Competence and PerformanceCompetence and Performance
Competence and PerformanceSahil Gupta
 

Andere mochten auch (11)

Top 8 production officer resume samples
Top 8 production officer resume samplesTop 8 production officer resume samples
Top 8 production officer resume samples
 
Exercise 6
Exercise 6Exercise 6
Exercise 6
 
2 - 3 Trees
2 - 3 Trees2 - 3 Trees
2 - 3 Trees
 
Syntax
SyntaxSyntax
Syntax
 
語言學概論Syntax
語言學概論Syntax語言學概論Syntax
語言學概論Syntax
 
語言學概論Morphology 2
語言學概論Morphology 2語言學概論Morphology 2
語言學概論Morphology 2
 
Biopharma Resume
Biopharma ResumeBiopharma Resume
Biopharma Resume
 
Exercise answers chapter 1, 2 & 3
Exercise answers chapter 1, 2 & 3Exercise answers chapter 1, 2 & 3
Exercise answers chapter 1, 2 & 3
 
Identification Of Morphemes
Identification Of MorphemesIdentification Of Morphemes
Identification Of Morphemes
 
Characteristics of human language
Characteristics of human languageCharacteristics of human language
Characteristics of human language
 
Competence and Performance
Competence and PerformanceCompetence and Performance
Competence and Performance
 

Ähnlich wie Linguistics fact sheets

Morpheme and its types in detail
Morpheme and its types in detailMorpheme and its types in detail
Morpheme and its types in detailDuaa Ahmed
 
Morphology Handout.pdf
Morphology Handout.pdfMorphology Handout.pdf
Morphology Handout.pdfArslanRaees
 
Vocabulary building
Vocabulary buildingVocabulary building
Vocabulary buildingTualha Rana
 
Grammatical Aspects of Language (1).pptx
Grammatical Aspects of Language (1).pptxGrammatical Aspects of Language (1).pptx
Grammatical Aspects of Language (1).pptxEmelitaCoronel2
 
Chapter 1 introduction 1
Chapter 1 introduction 1Chapter 1 introduction 1
Chapter 1 introduction 1llq4ever
 
Chapter 2 grammatical metalanguage
Chapter 2 grammatical metalanguageChapter 2 grammatical metalanguage
Chapter 2 grammatical metalanguageJesullyna Manuel
 
Morphology.....a major topic in Linguistics
Morphology.....a major topic in LinguisticsMorphology.....a major topic in Linguistics
Morphology.....a major topic in Linguisticssaroshzainab
 
Morphology and Word Formation in English
Morphology and Word Formation in EnglishMorphology and Word Formation in English
Morphology and Word Formation in EnglishHiraTariqButt
 
AREAS OF LANGUAGE.pptx
AREAS OF LANGUAGE.pptxAREAS OF LANGUAGE.pptx
AREAS OF LANGUAGE.pptxJovenLopez1
 
Morphology Son
Morphology SonMorphology Son
Morphology Sonfatmasima
 
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdflesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdfMOHAMEDAADDI
 
Classes of Words
Classes of WordsClasses of Words
Classes of WordsTutik SR
 

Ähnlich wie Linguistics fact sheets (20)

What is morphology
What is morphologyWhat is morphology
What is morphology
 
语法导论
语法导论语法导论
语法导论
 
导论
导论导论
导论
 
Morpheme and its types in detail
Morpheme and its types in detailMorpheme and its types in detail
Morpheme and its types in detail
 
Chapter 1 2
Chapter 1 2Chapter 1 2
Chapter 1 2
 
Morphology Handout.pdf
Morphology Handout.pdfMorphology Handout.pdf
Morphology Handout.pdf
 
Morphology.doc
Morphology.docMorphology.doc
Morphology.doc
 
Vocabulary building
Vocabulary buildingVocabulary building
Vocabulary building
 
Grammatical Aspects of Language (1).pptx
Grammatical Aspects of Language (1).pptxGrammatical Aspects of Language (1).pptx
Grammatical Aspects of Language (1).pptx
 
Chapter 1 introduction 1
Chapter 1 introduction 1Chapter 1 introduction 1
Chapter 1 introduction 1
 
Chapter 2 grammatical metalanguage
Chapter 2 grammatical metalanguageChapter 2 grammatical metalanguage
Chapter 2 grammatical metalanguage
 
Morphology.....a major topic in Linguistics
Morphology.....a major topic in LinguisticsMorphology.....a major topic in Linguistics
Morphology.....a major topic in Linguistics
 
Morphology and Word Formation in English
Morphology and Word Formation in EnglishMorphology and Word Formation in English
Morphology and Word Formation in English
 
Morphology
Morphology Morphology
Morphology
 
AREAS OF LANGUAGE.pptx
AREAS OF LANGUAGE.pptxAREAS OF LANGUAGE.pptx
AREAS OF LANGUAGE.pptx
 
Morphology Son
Morphology SonMorphology Son
Morphology Son
 
Week 2 morphology copy
Week 2 morphology copyWeek 2 morphology copy
Week 2 morphology copy
 
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdflesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
lesson 1 syntax 2021.pdf
 
Classes of Words
Classes of WordsClasses of Words
Classes of Words
 
Morphology
MorphologyMorphology
Morphology
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxVishalSingh1417
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural ResourcesEnergy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural ResourcesShubhangi Sonawane
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfChris Hunter
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDThiyagu K
 
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfAyushMahapatra5
 
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
PROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docxPROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docx
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docxPoojaSen20
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfPoh-Sun Goh
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxNikitaBankoti2
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfagholdier
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibitjbellavia9
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxDenish Jangid
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsMebane Rash
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhikauryashika82
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.christianmathematics
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptxUnit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
Unit-IV; Professional Sales Representative (PSR).pptx
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural ResourcesEnergy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
 
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
 
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
PROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docxPROCESS      RECORDING        FORMAT.docx
PROCESS RECORDING FORMAT.docx
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning ExhibitSociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
Sociology 101 Demonstration of Learning Exhibit
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
This PowerPoint helps students to consider the concept of infinity.
 

Linguistics fact sheets

  • 1. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) PART ONE: INTRODUCTION TO LINGUISTICS Every human knows at least one language, spoken or signed. Linguistics is the science of language, including the sounds, words, and grammar rules. Words in languages are finite, but sentences are not. It is this creative aspect of human language that sets it apart from animal languages, which are essentially responses to stimuli. The rules of a language, also called grammar, are learned as one acquires a language. These rules include phonology, the sound system, morphology, the structure of words, syntax, the combination of words into sentences, semantics, the ways in which sounds and meanings are related, and the lexicon, or mental dictionary of words. When you know a language, you know words in that language, i.e. sound units that are related to specific meanings. However, the sounds and meanings of words are arbitrary. Knowing a language encompasses this entire system, but this knowledge (called competence) is different from behavior (called performance.) You may know a language, but you may also choose to not speak it. Although you are not speaking the language, you still have the knowledge of it. However, if you don't know a language, you cannot speak it at all. There are two types of grammars: descriptive and prescriptive. Descriptive grammars represent the unconscious knowledge of a language. English speakers, for example, know that "me likes apples" is incorrect and "I like apples" is correct, although the speaker may not be able to explain why. Descriptive grammars do not teach the rules of a language, but rather describe rules that are already known. In contrast, prescriptive grammars dictate what a speaker's grammar should be and they include teaching grammars, which are written to help teach a foreign language. There are about 5,000 languages in the world right now (give or take a few thousand), and linguists have discovered that these languages are more alike than different from each other. There are universal concepts and properties that are shared by all languages, and these principles are contained in the Universal Grammar, which forms the basis of all possible human languages. 1
  • 2. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) PART TWO: MORPHOLOGY AND SYNTAX Morphemes are the minimal units of words that have a meaning and cannot be subdivided further. There are two main types: free and bound. Free morphemes can occur alone and bound morphemes must occur with another morpheme. An example of a free morpheme is "bad", and an example of a bound morpheme is "ly." It is bound because although it has meaning, it cannot stand alone. It must be attached to another morpheme to produce a word. Free morpheme: bad Bound morpheme: ly Word: badly When we talk about words, there are two groups: lexical (or content) and function (or grammatical) words. Lexical words are called open class words and include nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs. New words can regularly be added to this group. Function words, or closed class words, are conjunctions, prepositions, articles and pronouns; and new words cannot be (or are very rarely) added to this class. Affixes are often the bound morpheme. This group includes prefixes, suffixes, infixes, and circumfixes. Prefixes are added to the beginning of another morpheme, suffixes are added to the end, infixes are inserted into other morphemes, and circumfixes are attached to another morpheme at the beginning and end. Following are examples of each of these: Prefix: re- added to doproducesredo Suffix: -or added to edit produces editor Infix: -um- added to fikas (strong) produces fumikas (to be strong) in Bontoc Circumfix: ge- and -t to lieb (love) produces geliebt (loved) in German There are two categories of affixes: derivational and inflectional. The main difference between the two is that derivational affixes are added to morphemes to form new words that may or may not be the same part of speech and inflectional affixes are added to the end of an existing word for purely grammatical reasons. In English there are only eight total inflectional affixes: 2
  • 3. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) -s 3rd person singular present she waits -ed past tense she waited -ing progressive she's eating -en past participle she has eaten -s plural three apples -'s possessive Lori's son -er comparative you are taller -est superlative you are the shortest The other type of bound morphemes are called bound roots. These are morphemes (and not affixes) that must be attached to another morpheme and do not have a meaning of their own. Some examples are -ceive in perceive and mit in submit. English Morphemes A. Free 1. Open Class 2. Closed Class B. Bound 1. Affix a. Derivational b. Inflectional 2. Root 3. There are six ways to form new words. Compounds are a combination of words, acronyms are derived from the initials of words, back-formations are created from removing what is mistakenly considered to be an affix, abbreviations or clippings are shortening longer words, eponyms are created from proper nouns (names), and blending is combining parts of words into one. Compound: doghouse Acronym: NBA (National Basketball Association) or scuba(self-contained underwater breathing apparatus) Back-formation: edit from editor Abbreviation: phone from telephone Eponym: sandwich from Earl of Sandwich Blending: smog from smoke and fog 3
  • 4. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Grammar is learned unconsciously at a young age. Ask any five year old, and he will tell you that "I eat" and "you eat," but his "dog eats." But a human's syntactical knowledge goes farther than what is grammatical and what is not. It also accounts for ambiguity, in which a sentence could have two meanings, and enables us to determine grammatical relationships such as subject and direct object. Although we may not consciously be able to define the terms, we unconsciously know how to use them in sentences. Syntax, of course, depends on lexical categories (parts of speech.) You probably learned that there are 8 main parts of speech in grammar school. Linguistics takes a different approach to these categories and separates words into morphological and syntactic groups. Linguistics analyzes words according to their affixes and the words that follow or precede them. Hopefully, the following definitions of the parts of speech will make more sense and be of more use than the old definitions of grammar school books. Open Class Words _____ + plural endings Det. Adj. _____ (this is called a Noun Phrase) Nouns "dogs" "the big dog" _______ + tense endings Aux. ____ (this is called a Verb Phrase) Verbs "speaks" "have spoken" ______ + er / est Det. ____ Noun Adjectives "small" "the smaller child" Adj. + ly ____ Adj. or Verb or Adv. Adverbs "quickly" "quickly ran" 4
  • 5. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Closed Class Words a, an, the, this, that, these, _____________ Adj. Noun Determiners those, pronouns, quantities "this blue book" Auxiliary forms of be, have, may, NP __________________ VP Verbs can, shall "the girl is swimming" _____ NP (this is called a Prep. Phase) Prepositions at, in, on, under, over, of "in the room" N or V or Adj. ____________ N or V Conjunctions and, but, or or Adj. "apples and oranges" Sub categorization defines the restrictions on which syntactic categories (parts of speech) can or cannot occur within a lexical item. These additional specifications of words are included in our mental lexicon. Verbs are the most common categories that are subcategorized. Verbs can either be transitive or intransitive. Transitive verbs take a direct object, while intransitive verbs take an indirect object (usually they need a preposition before the noun). Transitive verb: to eat I ate an apple. (direct object) Intransitive: to sleep I was sleeping in the bed. (indirect object) Individual nouns can also be subcategorized. For example, the noun idea can be followed by a Prepositional Phrase or that and a sentence. But the noun compassion can only be followed by a Prepositional Phrase and not a sentence. (Ungrammatical sentences are marked with asterisks.) the idea of stricter laws his compassion for the animals the idea that stricter laws *his compassion that the animals are hurt are necessary 5
  • 6. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Phrase structure rules describe how phrases are formed and in what order. These rules define the following: Noun Phrase (NP) (Det.) (Adj.) Noun (PP) Verb Phrase (VP) Verb (NP) (PP) Prepositional Phrase (PP) Prep. NP Sentence (S) NP VP The parentheses indicate the categories are optional. Verbs don't always have to be followed by prepositional phrases and nouns don't always have to be preceded by adjectives. Passive Sentences The difference between the two sentences "Mary hired Bill" and "Bill was hired by Mary" is that the first is active and the second is passive. In order to change an active sentence into a passive one, the object of the active must become the subject of the passive. The verb in the passive sentence becomes a form of "be" plus the participle form of the main verb. And the subject of the active becomes the object of the passive preceded by the word "by." Active Passive Mary hired Bill. Bill was hired by Mary. Subject + Verb + Object Object + "be" + Verb + by + Subject 6
  • 7. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) PART THREE: PHONETICS AND PHONOLOGY There are three types of the study of the sounds of language. Acoustic Phonetics is the study of the physical properties of sounds. Auditory Phonetics is the study of the way listeners perceive sounds. Articulatory Phonetics (the type this lesson is concerned with) is the study of how the vocal tracts produce the sounds. The orthography (spelling) of words in misleading, especially in English. One sound can be represented by several different combinations of letters. For example, all of the following words contain the same vowel sound: he, believe, Lee, Caesar, key, amoeba, loudly, machine, people, and sea. The following poem illustrates this fact of English humorously (note the pronunciation of the bold words): I take it you already know of tough and bough and cough and dough? Some may stumble, but not you, on hiccough, thorough, slough, and through? So now you are ready, perhaps, to learn of less familiar traps? Beware of heard, a dreadful word, that looks like beard, but sounds like bird. And dead, it's said like bed, not bead; for goodness' sake, don't call it deed! Watch out for meat and great and threat. (They rhyme with suite and straight and debt.) A moth is not a moth in mother, nor both in bother, broth in brother. And here is not a match for there, nor dear and fear, for bear and pear. And then there's dose and rose and lose - just look them up - and goose and choose Andcork and work and card and ward and font and front and word and sword And do and go, then thwart and cart, come, come! I've hardly made a start. A dreadful language? Why man alive! I've learned to talk it when I was five. And yet to write it, the more I tried, I hadn't learned it at fifty-five. - Author Unknown The discrepancy between spelling and sounds led to the formation of the International Phonetics Alphabet (IPA.) The symbols used in this alphabet can be used to represent all sounds of all human languages. The following is the 7
  • 8. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) English Phonetic alphabet. You might want to memorize all of these symbols, as most foreign language dictionaries use the IPA. Phonetic Alphabet for English Pronunciation p pill d dill h heal ʌ but b bill n neal l leaf aj light m mill s seal r reef ɔ j boy f feel z zeal j you ɪ bit v veal č chill w witch ɛ bet θ thigh ǰ Jill i beet ʊ foot ð thy ʍ which e bait ɔ awe š shill k kill u boot a bar ž azure g gill o boat ə sofa t till ŋ ring æ bat aw cow Some speakers of English pronounce the words which and witch differently, but if you pronounce both words identically, just use w for both words. And the sounds /ʌ / and /ə/ are pronounced the same, but the former is used in stressed syllables, while the latter is used in unstressed syllables. This list does not even begin to include all of the phonetic symbols though. One other symbol is the glottal stop, ʔ which is somewhat rare in English. Some linguists in the United States traditionally use different symbols than the IPA symbols. These are listed below. U.S. IPA š ʃ ž ʒ č tʃ ǰ dʒ U ʊ 8
  • 9. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) The production of any speech sound involves the movement of air. Air is pushed through the lungs, larynx (vocal folds) and vocal tract (the oral and nasal cavities.) Sounds produced by using air from the lungs are called pulmonic sounds. If the air is pushed out, it is called egressive. If the air is sucked in, it is called ingressive. Sounds produced by ingressive airstreams are ejectives, implosives, and clicks. These sounds are common among African and American Indian languages. The majority of languages in the world use pulmonic egressive airstream mechanisms, and I will present only these types of sounds in this lesson. Consonants Consonants are produced as air from the lungs is pushed through the glottis (the opening between the vocal cords) and out the mouth. They are classified according to voicing, aspiration, nasal/oral sounds, places of articulation and manners of articulation. Voicing is whether the vocal folds vibrate or not. The sound /s/ is called voiceless because there is no vibration, and the sound /z/ is called voiced because the vocal folds do vibrate (you can feel on your neck if there is vibration.) Only three sounds in English have aspiration, the sounds /b/, /p/ and /t/. An extra puff of air is pushed out when these sounds begin a word or stressed syllable. Hold a piece of paper close to your mouth when saying the words pin and spin. You should notice extra air when you say pin. Aspiration is indicated in writing with a superscript h, as in /pʰ /. Nasal sounds are produced when the velum (the soft palate located in the back of the roof of the mouth) is lowered and air is passed through the nose and mouth. Oral sounds are produced when the velum is raised and air passes only through the mouth. Places of Articulation Bilabial: lips together Labiodental: lower lip against front teeth Interdental: tongue between teeth Alveolar: tongue near alveolar ridge on roof of mouth (in between teeth and hard palate) Palatal: tongue on hard palate Velar: tongue near velum Glottal: space between vocal folds 9
  • 10. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) The following sound is not found in the English language, although it is common in languages such as French and Arabic: Uvular: raise back of tongue to uvula (the appendage hanging down from the velum) Manners of Articulation Stop: obstruct airstream completely Fricative: partial obstruction with friction Affricate: stop airstream, then release Liquids: partial obstruction, no friction Glides: little or no obstruction, must occur with a vowel You should practice saying the sounds of the English alphabet to see if you can identify the places of articulation in the mouth. The sounds are described by voicing, place and then manner of articulation, so the sound /j/ would be called a voiced palatal glide and the sound /s/ would be called a voiceless alveolar fricative. Interdent Alveola Bilabial Labiodental Palatal Velar Glottal al r p t k Stop (oral) b d g Nasal m n ŋ (stop) f θ s š Fricative h v ð z ž č Affricate ǰ ʍ ʍ Glide h w j w Liquid lr 10
  • 11. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) For rows that have two consonants, the top consonant is voiceless and the bottom consonant is voiced. Nasal stops are all voiced, as are liquids. The sound /j/ is also voiced. If sounds are in two places on the chart, that means they can be pronounced either way. Vowels Vowels are produced by a continuous airstream and all are voiced. They are classified according to height of the tongue, part of tongue involved, and position of the lips. The tongue can be high, mid, or low; and the part of the tongue used can be front, central or back. Only four vowels are produced with rounded lips and only four vowels are considered tense instead of lax. The sound /a/ would be written as a low back lax unrounded vowel. Many languages also have vowels called diphthongs, a sequence of two sounds, vowel + glide. Examples in English include oy in boy and ow in cow. In addition, vowels can be nasalized when they occur before nasal consonants. A diacritic mark [~] is placed over the vowel to show this. The vowel sounds in bee and bean are considered different because the sound in bean is nasalized. Part of Tongue Front Central Back i u High ɪ ʊ Tongue e ə o Height Mid ɛ ʌ ɔ Low æ a The bold vowels are tense, and the italic vowels are rounded. English also includes the diphthongs: [aj] as in bite, [aw] as in cow, and [oj] as in boy. For the complete IPA chart with symbols for the sounds of every human language, please visit the International Phonetic Association's website. And you're looking for a way to type English IPA symbols online, please visit ipa.typeit.org 11
  • 12. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Major Classes of Sounds (Distinctive Features) All of the classes of sounds described above can be put into more general classes that include the patterning of sounds in the world's languages. Continuant sounds indicate a continuous airflow, while non-continuant sounds indicate total obstruction of the airstream. Obstruent sounds do not allow air to escape through the nose, while sonorant sounds have a relatively free airflow through the mouth or nose. The following table summarizes this information: Obstruent Sonorant Continuant fricatives liquids, glides, vowels oral stops, Non-Continuant nasal stops affricates Major Class Features [+ Lateral] [l] [+ Consonantal] consonants [- Lateral] [r] [- Consonantal] vowels [+ Delayed Release] affricates [č, ǰ ] [+Sonorant] nasals, liquids, glides, [- Delayed Release] stops [p, b, t, d, k, vowels g, ʔ ] [- Sonorant] stops, fricatives, affricates [+ Strident] “noisy” fricatives [f, v, s, z, (obstruents) š, ž] [+ Approximant] glides [j, w] [- Strident] [?, ð, h] [- Approximant] everything else Place Features Voice Features [Labial] involves lips [f, v, p, b, w] [+ Voice] voiced [Coronal] alveolar ridge to palate [θ, ð, [- Voice] voiceless s, z, t, d, š, ž, n, r, l] [+ Spread Glottis] aspirated [pʰ , tʰ , [+ Anterior] interdentals and true kʰ ] alveolars [- Spread Glottis] unaspirated [- Anterior] retroflex and palatals [š, ž, [+ Constricted Glottis] ejectives, č, ǰ , j] implosives [Dorsal] from velum back [k, g, ŋ] [- Constricted Glottis] everything else [Glottal] in larynx [h, ʔ ] Manner Features Vowels [+ Continuant] fricatives [f, v, s, z, š, ž, Height [± high] [± low] θ, ð] Backness [± back] [- Continuant] stops [p, b, t, d, k, g, Lip Rounding [± round] ʔ ] Tenseness [± tense] 12
  • 13. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) [+ Nasal] nasal consonants [m, n, ŋ] [- Nasal] all oral consonants Whereas phonetics is the study of sounds and is concerned with the production, audition and perception of speech sounds (called phones), phonology describes the way sounds function within a given language and operates at the level of sound systems and abstract sound units. Knowing the sounds of a language is only a small part of phonology. This importance is shown by the fact that you can change one word into another by simply changing one sound. Consider the differences between the words time and dime. The words are identical except for the first sound. [t] and [d] can therefore distinguish words, and are called contrasting sounds. They are distinctive sounds in English, and all distinctive sounds are classified as phonemes. Minimal Pairs Minimal pairs are words with different meanings that have the same sounds except for one. These contrasting sounds can either be consonants or vowels. The words pin and bin are minimal pairs because they are exactly the same except for the first sound. The words read and rude are also exactly the same except for the vowel sound. The examples from above, time and dime, are also minimal pairs. In effect, words with one contrastive sound are minimal pairs. Another feature of minimal pairs is overlapping distribution. Sounds that occur in phonetic environments that are identical are said to be in overlapping distribution. The sounds of [ɪ n] from pin and bin are in overlapping distribution because they occur in both words. The same is true for three and through. The sounds of [θr] is in overlapping distribution because they occur in both words as well. Free Variation Some words in English are pronounced differently by different speakers. This is most noticeable among American English speakers and British English speakers, as well as dialectal differences. This is evidenced in the ways neither, for example, can be pronounced. American English pronunciation is [niðər], while British English pronunciation is [najðər]. Phones and Allophones Phonemes are not physical sounds. They are abstract mental representations of the phonological units of a language. Phones are considered to be any single 13
  • 14. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) speech sound of which phonemes are made. Phonemes are a family of phones regarded as a single sound and represented by the same symbol. The different phones that are the realization of a phoneme are called allophones of that phoneme. The use of allophones is not random, but rule-governed. No one is taught these rules as they are learned subconsciously when the native language ] are allophones of the phoneme /ɪ /. Complementary Distribution If two sounds are allophones of the same phoneme, they are said to be in complementary distribution. These sounds cannot occur in minimal pairs and they cannot change the meaning of otherwise identical words. If you interchange the sounds, you will only change the pronunciation of the words, not the meaning. Native speakers of the language regard the two allophones as variations of the same sound. To hear this, start to say the word cool (your lips should be pursed in anticipation of /u/ sound), but then say kill instead (with your lips still pursed.) Your pronunciation of kill should sound strange because cool and kill are pronounced with different allophones of the phoneme /k/. Nasalized vowels are allophones of the ]. Yet in French, nasalized vowels are not allophones of the same phonemes. They are separate phonemes. The words beau [bo] and ] are not in complementary distribution because they are minimal pairs and have contrasting sounds. Changing the sounds changes the meaning of the words. This is just one example of differences between languages. Phonological Rules Assimilation: sounds become more like neighboring sounds, allowing for ease of articulation or pronunciation; such as vowels are nasalized before nasal consonants. Harmony: non-adjacent vowels become more similar by sharing a feature or set of features (common in Finnish) Gemination: sound becomes identical to an adjacent sound Regressive Assimilation: sound on left is the target, and sound on right is the trigger.Dissimilation: sounds become less like neighboring 14
  • 15. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) sounds; these rules are quite rare, but one example in English is [fɪ fθ] becoming [fɪ ft] (/f/ and /θ/ are both fricatives, but /t/ is a stop) Epenthesis: insertion of a sound, e.g. Latin "homre" became Spanish "hombre" Prothesis: insertion of vowel sound at beginning of word Anaptyxis: vowel sound with predictable quality is inserted word- internally Paragoge: insertion of vowel sound at end of word Excrescence: consonant sound inserted between other consonants (also called stop-intrusion) Deletion: deletion of a sound; e.g. French word-final consonants are deleted when the next word begins with a consonant (but are retained when the following word begins with a vowel) Aphaeresis: vowel sound deleted at beginning of word. Syncope: vowel sound is deleted word-internally. Apocope: vowel sound deleted at end of word. Metathesis: reordering of phonemes; in some dialects of English, the word asked is pronounced [æks]; children's speech shows many cases of metathesis such as aminal for animal. Lenition: consonant changes to a weaker manner of articulation; voiced stopbecomes a fricative, fricative becomes a glide, etc. Palatalization: sound becomes palatal when adjacent to a front vowel Compensatory Lengthening: sound becomes long as a result of sound loss, e.g. Latin "octo" became Italian "otto" Assimilation in English An interesting observation of assimilation rules is evidenced in the formation of plurals and the past tense in English. When pluralizing nouns, the last letter is pronounced as either [s], [z], or [əz]. When forming past tenses of verbs, the - ed ending is pronounced as either [t], [d], [əd]. 15
  • 16. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) If you were to sort words into three columns, you would be able to tell why certain words are followed by certain sounds: Plural nouns Hopefully, you can determine which consonants /s/ /z/ /əz/ produce which sounds. In the nouns, /s/ is added cats dads churches after voiceless consonants, and /z/ is added after voiced consonants. /əz/ is added after sibilants. For tips bibs kisses the verbs, /t/ is added after voiceless consonants, laughs dogs judges and /d/ is added after voiced consonants. /əd/ is Past Tense added after alveolar stops. The great thing about this is that no one ever taught you this in school. /t/ /d/ /əd/ But thanks to linguistics, you now know why there kissed loved patted are different sounds (because of assimilation rules, washed jogged waded the consonants become more like their neighboring coughed teased seeded consonants.) Writing Rules A general phonological rule is A → B / D __ E (said: A becomes B when it occurs between D and E) Other symbols in rule writing include: C = any obstruent, V = any vowel, Ø = nothing, # = word boundary, ( ) = optional, and { } = either/or. A deletion rule is A → Ø / E __ (A is deleted when it occurs after E) and an insertion rule is Ø → A / E __ (A is inserted when it occurs after E). Alpha notation is used to collapse similar assimilation rules into one. C → [Α voice] / __ [Α voice] (An obstruent becomes voiced when it occurs before a voiced obstruent AND an obstruent becomes voiceless when it occurs before a voiceless obstruent.) Similarly, it can be used for dissimilation rules too. C → [-Α voice] / __ [Α voice] (An obstruent becomes voiced when it occurs before a voiceless obstruent AND an obstruent becomes voiceless when it occurs before a voiced obstruent.) Gemination rules are written as C1C2 → C2C2 (for example, pd→dd) Syllable Structure 16
  • 17. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) There are three peaks to a syllable: nucleus (vowel), onset (consonant before nucleus) and coda (consonant after nucleus.) The onset and coda are both optional, meaning that a syllable could contain a vowel and nothing else. The nucleus is required in every syllable by definition. The order of the peaks is always onset - nucleus - coda. All languages permit open syllables (Consonant + Vowel), but not all languages allow closed syllables (Consonant + Vowel + Consonant). Languages that only allow open syllables are called CV languages. In addition to not allowing codas, some CV languages also have constraints on the number of consonants allowed in the onset. The sonority profile dictates that sonority must rise to the nucleus and fall to the coda in every language. The sonority scale (from most to least sonorous) is vowels - glides - liquids - nasals - obstruents. Sonority must rise in the onset, but the sounds cannot be adjacent to or share a place of articulation (except [s] in English) nor can there be more than two consonants in the onset. This explains why English allows some consonant combinations, but not others. For example, price [prajs] is a well-formed syllable and word because the sonority rises in the onset (p, an obstruent, is less sonorous than r, a liquid); however, rpice [rpajs] is not a syllable in English because the sonority does not rise in the onset. The Maximality Condition states that onsets are as large as possible up to the well-formedness rules of a language. Onsets are always preferred over codas when syllabifying words. There are also constraints that state the maximum number of consonants between two vowels is four; onsets and codas have two consonants maximally; and onsets and codas can be bigger only at the edges of words. PART FOUR: SEMANTICS AND PRAGMATICS Semantics Lexical semantics is concerned with the meanings of words and the meaning of relationships among words, while phrasal semantics is concerned with the meaning of syntactic units larger than the word. Pragmatics is the study of how context affects meaning, such as how sentences are interpreted in certain situations. 17
  • 18. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Semantic properties are the components of meanings of words. For example, the semantic property "human" can be found in many words such as parent, doctor, baby, professor, widow, and aunt. Other semantic properties include animate objects, male, female, countable items and non-countable items. The –nyms Homonyms: different words that are pronounced the same, but may or may not be spelled the same (to, two, and too) Polysemous: word that has multiple meanings that are related conceptually or historically (bear can mean to tolerate or to carry or to support) Homograph: different words that are spelled identically and possibly pronounced the same; if they are pronounced the same, they are also homonyms (pen can mean writing utensil or cage) Heteronym: homographs that are pronounced differently (dove the bird and dove the past tense of dive) Synonym: words that mean the same but sound different (couch and sofa) Antonym: words that are opposite in meaning. Complementary pairs: alive and dead Gradable pairs: big and small (no absolute scale) Hyponym: set of related words (red, white, yellow, blue are all hyponyms of "color") Metonym: word used in place of another to convey the same meaning (jock used for athlete, Washington used for American government, crown used for monarchy) Retronym: expressions that are no longer redundant (silent movie used to be redundant because a long time ago, all movies were silent, but this is no longer true or redundant) Thematic Roles Thematic roles are the semantic relationships between the verbs and noun phrases of sentences. The following chart shows the thematic roles in relationship to verbs of sentences: Thematic Description Example Role Agent the one who performs an action Maria ran 18
  • 19. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) the person or thing that undergoes an Theme Mary called John action Location the place where an action takes place It rains in Spain Put the cat on the Goal the place to which an action is directed porch the place from which an action He flew from Source originates Chicago to LA the means by which an action is He cuts his hair Instrument performed with scissors She heard Bob Experiencer one who perceives something play the piano The wind Causative a natural force that causes a change destroyed the house The tail of the cat Possessor one who has something got caught Recipient one who receives something I gave it to the girl Sentential Meaning The meaning of sentences is built from the meaning of noun phrases and verbs. Sentences contain truth conditions if the circumstances in the sentence are true. Paraphrases are two sentences with the same truth conditions, despite subtle differences in structure and emphasis. The ball was kicked by the boy is a paraphrase of the sentence the boy kicked the ball, but they have the same truth conditions - that a boy kicked a ball. Sometimes the truth of one sentence entails or implies the truth of another sentence. This is called entailment and the opposite of this is called contradiction, where one sentence implies the falseness of another. He was assassinated entails that he is dead. He was assassinated contradicts with the statement he is alive. Pragmatics Pragmatics is the interpretation of linguistic meaning in context. Linguistic context is discourse that precedes a sentence to be interpreted and situational context is knowledge about the world. In the following sentences, the kids have 19
  • 20. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) eaten already and surprisingly, they are hungry, the linguistic context helps to interpret the second sentence depending on what the first sentence says. Maxims of Conversation Grice's maxims for conversation are conventionsof speech such as the maxim of quantity that states a speaker should be as informative as is required and neither more nor less. The maxim of relevance essentially states a speaker should stay on the topic, and the maxim of manner states the speaker should be brief and orderly, and avoid ambiguity. The fourth maxim, the maxim of quality, states that a speaker should not lie or make any unsupported claims. Performative Sentences In these types of sentences, the speaker is the subject who, by uttering the sentence, is accomplishing some additional action, such as daring, resigning, or nominating. These sentences are all affirmative, declarative and in the present tense. An informal test to see whether a sentence is performative or not is to insert the words I hereby before the verb.I hereby challenge you to a match or I hereby fine you $500 are both performative, but I hereby know that girl is not. Other performative verbs are bet, promise, pronounce, bequeath, swear, testify, and dismiss. Presuppositions These are implicit assumptions required to make a sentence meaningful. Sentences that contain presuppositions are not allowed in court because accepting the validity of the statement mean accepting the presuppositions as well. Have you stopped stealing cars?is not admissible in court because no matter how the defendant answers, the presupposition that he steals cars already will be acknowledged. Have you stopped smoking?Implies that you smoke already. Deixis Deixis is reference to a person, object, or event which relies on the situational context. First and second person pronouns such as my, mine, you, your, yours, we, ours and us are always deictic because their reference is entirely dependent on context. Demonstrative articles like this, that, these and those and expressions of time and place are always deictic as well. In order to understand what specific times or places such expressions refer to, we also need to know when or where the utterance was said. If someone says "I'm over here!" you 20
  • 21. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) would need to know who "I" referred to, as well as where "here" is. Deixis marks one of the boundaries of semantics and pragmatics. PART FIVE: NEUROLINGUISTICS The human brain consists of 10 billion nerve cells (neurons) and billions of fibers that connect them. These neurons or gray matter form the cortex, the surface of the brain, and the connecting fibers or white matter form the interior of the brain. The brain is divided into two hemispheres, the left and right cerebral hemispheres. These hemispheres are connected by the corpus callosum. In general, the left hemisphere of the brain controls the right side of the body and vice versa. The auditory cortex receives and interprets auditory stimuli, while the visual cortex receives and interprets visual stimuli. The angular gyrus converts the auditory stimuli to visual stimuli and vice versa. The motor cortex signals the muscles to move when we want to talk and is directed by Broca's area. The nerve fiber connecting Wernicke's and Broca's area is called the arcuate fasciculus. Lateralization refers to any cognitive functions that are localized to one side of the brain or the other. Language is said to be lateralized and processed in the left hemisphere of the brain. Paul Broca first related language to the left side of the brain when he noted that damage to the front part of the left hemisphere (now called Broca's area) resulted in a loss of speech, while damage to the right side did not. He determined this through autopsies of patients who had acquired language deficits following brain injuries. A language disorder that follows a brain lesion is called aphasia, and patients with damage to Broca's area have slow and labored speech, loss of function words, and poor word order, yet good comprehension. Carl Wernicke also used studies of autopsies to describe another type of aphasia that resulted from lesions in the back portion of the left hemisphere (now called Wernicke's area.) Unlike Broca's patients, Wernicke's spoke fluently and with good pronunciation, but with many lexical errors and a difficulty in comprehension. Broca's and Wernicke's area are the two main regions of the cortex of the brain related to language processing. 21
  • 22. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Aphasics can suffer from anomia, jargon aphasia, and acquired dyslexia. Anomia is commonly referred to as "tip of the tongue" phenomenon and many aphasics experience word finding difficulty on a regular basis. Jargon aphasia results in the substitution of one word or sound for another. Some aphasics may substitute similar words for each other, such as table for chair, or they may substitute completely unrelated words, such as chair for engine. Others may pronounce table as sable, substituting an s sound for a t sound. Aphasics who became dyslexic after brain damage are called acquired dyslexics. When reading aloud words printed on cards, the patients produced the following substitutions: Stimuli Response One Response Two Act Play Play South East West Medicine Heal Pain The substitution of phonologically similar words, such as pool and tool, also provides evidence that a human's mental lexicon is organized by both phonology and semantics. Broca's aphasics and some acquired dyslexics are unable to read function words, and when presented with them on the cards, the patients say no, as shown in the following example: Stimuli One Response Stimuli Two Response Witch Witch Which no! Hour Time Our no! Wood Wood Would no! The patient's errors suggest our mental dictionary is further organized into parts consisting of major content words (first stimuli) and grammatical words (second stimuli.) In addition, split-brain patients (those who have had their corpus callosum severed) provide evidence for language lateralization. If an object is placed in the left hand of split-brain patient whose vision is cut off, the person cannot 22
  • 23. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) name the object, but will know how to use it. The information is sent to the right side of the brain, but cannot be relayed to the left side for linguistic naming. However, if the object is placed in the person's right hand, the person can immediately name it because the information is sent directly to the left hemisphere. Dichotic listening is another experimental technique, using auditory signals. Subjects hear a different sound in each ear, such as boy in the left ear and girl in the right ear or water rushing in the left ear and a horn honking in the right ear. When asked to state what they heard in each ear, subjects are more frequently correct in reporting linguistic stimuli in the right ear (girl) and nonverbal stimuli in the left ear (water rushing.) This is because the left side of the brain is specialized for language and a word heard in the right ear will transfer directly to the left side of the body because of the contralateralization of the brain. Furthermore, the right side of the brain is specialized for nonverbal stimuli, such as music and environmental sounds, and a noise heard in the left ear will transfer directly to the right side of the brain. PART SIX: CHILD LANGUAGE ACQUISITION AND SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION Linguistic competence develops in stages, from babbling to one word to two word, then telegraphic speech. Babbling is now considered the earliest form of language acquisition because infants will produce sounds based on what language input they receive. One word sentences (holophrastic speech) are generally monosyllabic in consonant-vowel clusters. During two word stage, there are no syntactic or morphological markers, no inflections for plural or past tense, and pronouns are rare, but the intonation contour extends over the whole utterance. Telegraphic speech lacks function words and only carries the open class content words, so that the sentences sound like a telegram. Three theories The three theories of language acquisition: imitation, reinforcement and analogy, do not explain very well how children acquire language. Imitation does not work because children produce sentences never heard before, such as "cat stand up table." Even when they try to imitate adult speech, children cannot generate the same sentences because of their limited grammar. And children 23
  • 24. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) who are unable to speak still learn and understand the language, so that when they overcome their speech impairment they immediately begin speaking the language. Reinforcement also does not work because it actually seldomly occurs and when it does, the reinforcement is correcting pronunciation or truthfulness, and not grammar. A sentence such as "apples are purple" would be corrected more often because it is not true, as compared to a sentence such as "apples is red" regardless of the grammar. Analogy also cannot explain language acquisition. Analogy involves the formation of sentences or phrases by using other sentences as samples. If a child hears the sentence, "I painted a red barn," he can say, by analogy, "I painted a blue barn." Yet if he hears the sentence, "I painted a barn red," he cannot say "I saw a barn red." The analogy did not work this time, and this is not a sentence of English. Acquisitions Phonology: A child's error in pronunciation is not random, but rule-governed. Typical phonological rules include: consonant cluster simplification (spoon becomes poon), devoicing of final consonants (dog becomes dok), voicing of initial consonants (truck becomes druck), and consonant harmony (doggy becomes goggy, or big becomes gig.) Morphology: An overgeneralization of constructed rules is shown when children treat irregular verbs and nouns as regular. Instead of went as the past tense of go, children use goed because the regular verbs add an -ed ending to form the past tense. Similarly, children use gooses as the plural of goose instead of geese, because regular nouns add an -s in the plural. The "Innateness Hypothesis" of child language acquisition, proposed by Noam Chomsky, states that the human species is prewired to acquire language, and that the kind of language is also determined. Many factors have led to this hypothesis such as the ease and rapidity of language acquisition despite impoverished input as well as the uniformity of languages. All children will learn a language, and children will also learn more than one language if they are exposed to it. Children follow the same general stages when learning a language, although the linguistic input is widely varied. The poverty of the stimulus states that children seem to learn or know the aspects of grammar for which they receive no information. In addition, children 24
  • 25. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) do not produce sentences that could not be sentences in some human language. The principles of Universal Grammar underlie the specific grammars of all languages and determine the class of languages that can be acquired unconsciously without instruction. It is the genetically determined faculty of the left hemisphere, and there is little doubt that the brain is specially equipped for acquisition of human language. The "Critical Age Hypothesis" suggests that there is a critical age for language acquisition without the need for special teaching or learning. During this critical period, language learning proceeds quickly and easily. After this period, the acquisition of grammar is difficult, and for some people, never fully achieved. Cases of children reared in social isolation have been used for testing the critical age hypothesis. None of the children who had little human contact were able to speak any language once reintroduced into society. Even the children who received linguistic input after being reintroduced to society were unable to fully develop language skills. These cases of isolated children, and of deaf children, show that humans cannot fully acquire any language to which they are exposed unless they are within the critical age. Beyond this age, humans are unable to acquire much of syntax and inflectional morphology. Second Language Acquisition Teaching Methods Grammar-translation: The student memorizes words, inflected words, and syntactic rules and uses them to translate from native to target language and vice versa; most commonly used method in schools because it does not require teacher to be fluent; however, least effective method of teaching. Direct method: The native language is not used at all in the classroom, and the student must learn the new language without formal instruction; based on theories of first language acquisition. Audio-lingual: Heavy use of dialogs and audio, based on the assumption that language learning is acquired mainly through imitation, repetition, and reinforcement; influenced by psychology. 25
  • 26. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Natural Approach: Emphasis on vocabulary and not grammar; focus on meaning, not form; use of authentic materials instead of textbook Silent Way: Teachers remain passive observers while students learn, which is a process of personal growth; no grammatical explanation or modeling by the teacher. Total Physical Response: Students play active role as listener and performer, must respond to imperative drills with physical action. Suggestopedia: Students always remain comfortable and relaxed and learn through memorization of meaningful texts, although the goal is understanding Community Language Learning: Materials are developed as course progresses and teacher understands what students need and want to learn; learning involves the whole person and language is seen as more than just communication. Community Language Teaching: Incorporates all components of language and helps students with various learning styles; use of communication-based activities with authentic materials, needs of learner are taken into consideration when planning topics and objectives. Four skill areas The four skill areas of learning a foreign language need to be addressed consistently and continually. Good lesson plans incorporate all four: Listening, Speaking, Reading (and Vocabulary), and Writing (and Grammar). Native speakers do not learn the skill areas separately, nor do they use them separately, so they shouldn’t be taught separately. However, it is easy to fall into the trap of teaching about the language, instead of actually teaching the language. Most textbooks resort to teaching grammar and vocabulary lists and nothing more. 26
  • 27. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) PART SEVEN: SOCIOLINGUISTICS A dialect is a variety of language that is systematically different from other varieties of the same language. The dialects of a single language are mutually intelligible, but when the speakers can no longer understand each other, the dialects become languages. Geographical regions are also considered when dialects become languages. Swedish, Norwegian, and Danish are all considered separate languages because of regular differences in grammar and the countries in which they are spoken, yet Swedes, Norwegians, and Danes can all understand one another. Hindi and Urdu are considered mutually intelligible languages when spoken, yet the writing systems are different. On the other hand, Mandarin and Cantonese are mutually unintelligible languages when spoken, yet the writing systems are the same. A dialect is considered standard if it is used by the upper class, political leaders, in literature and is taught in schools as the correct form of the language. Overt prestige refers to this dominant dialect. A non-standard dialect is associated with covert prestige and is an ethnic or regional dialect of a language. These non- standard dialects are just as linguistically sophisticated as the standard dialect, and judgments to the inferiority of them are based on social or racist judgments. African-American English contains many regular differences of the standard dialect. These differences are the same as the differences among many of the world's dialects. Phonological differences include r and l deletion of words like poor (pa) and all (awe.) Consonant cluster simplification also occurs (passed pronounced like pass), as well as a loss of interdental fricatives. Syntactic differences include the double negative and the loss of and habitual use of the verb "be." He late means he is late now, but he be late means he is always late. A lingua franca is a major language used in an area where speakers of more than one language live that permits communication and commerce among them. English is called the lingua franca of the whole world, while French used to be the lingua franca of diplomacy. 27
  • 28. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) A pidgin is a rudimentary language of few lexical items and less complex grammatical rules based on another language. No one learns a pidgin as a native language, but children do learn creoles as a first language. Creoles are defined as pidgins that are adopted by a community as its native tongue. Besides dialects, speakers may use different styles or registers (such as contractions) depending on the context. Slang may also be used in speech, but is not often used in formal situations or writing. Jargon refers to the unique vocabulary pertaining to a certain area, such as computers or medicine. Words or expressions referring to certain acts that are forbidden or frowned upon are considered taboo. These taboo words produce euphemisms, words or phrases that replace the expressions that are being avoided. The use of words may indicate a society's attitude toward sex, bodily functions or religious beliefs, and they may also reflect racism or sexism in a society. Language itself is not racist or sexist, but the society may be. Such insulting words may reinforce biased views, and changes in society may be reflected in the changes in language. 28
  • 29. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) PART EIGHT: HISTORICAL LINGUISTICS Languages that evolve from a common source are genetically related. These languages were once dialects of the same language. Earlier forms of Germanic languages, such as German, English, and Swedish were dialects of Proto- Germanic, while earlier forms of Romance languages, such as Spanish, French, and Italian were dialects of Latin. Furthermore, earlier forms of Proto-Germanic and Latin were once dialects of Indo-European. Linguistic changes like sound shift are found in the history of all languages, as evidenced by the regular sound correspondences that exist between different stages of the same language, different dialects, and different languages. Words, morphemes, and phonemes may be altered, added or lost. The meaning of words may broaden, narrow or shift. New words may be introduced into a language by borrowing, or by coinage, blends and acronyms. The lexicon may also shrink as older words become obsolete. Change comes about as a result of the restructuring of grammar by children learning the language. Grammars seem to become simple and regular, but these simplifications may be compensated for by more complexities. Sound changes can occur because of assimilation, a process of ease of articulation. Some grammatical changes are analogic changes, generalizations that lead to more regularity, such as sweeped instead of swept. The study of linguistic change is called historical and comparative linguistics. Linguists identify regular sound correspondences using the comparative method among the cognates (words that developed from the same ancestral language) of related languages. They can restructure an earlier protolanguage and this allows linguists to determine the history of a language family. Old English, Middle English, Modern English Old English 499-1066 CE Beowulf Middle English 1066-1500 CE Canterbury Tales 29
  • 30. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Modern English 1500-present Shakespeare Phonological change: Between 1400 and 1600 CE, the Great Vowel Shift took place. The seven long vowels of Middle English underwent changes. The high vowels [i] and [u] became the diphthongs [aj] and [aw]. The long vowels increased tongue height and shifted upward, and [a] was fronted. Many of the spelling inconsistencies of English are because of the Great Vowel Shift. Our spelling system still reflects the way words were pronounced before the shift took place. Morphological change: Many Indo-European languages had extensive case endings that governed word order, but these are no longer found in Romance languages or English. Although pronouns still show a trace of the case system (he vs. him), English uses prepositions to show the case. Instead of the dative case (indirect objects), English usually the words to or for. Instead of the genitive case, English uses the word ofor 's after a noun to show possession. Other cases include the nominative (subject pronouns), accusative (direct objects), and vocative. Syntactic change: Because of the lack of the case system, word order has become more rigid and strict in Modern English. Now it is strictly Subject - Verb - Object order. Orthographic change: Consonant clusters have become simplified, such as hlaf becoming loaf and hnecca becoming neck. However, some of these clusters are still written, but are no longer pronounced, such as gnaw, write, and dumb. Lexical change: Old English borrowed place names from Celtic, army, religious and educational words from Latin, and everyday words from Scandinavian. Angle and Saxon (German dialects) form the basis of Old English phonology, morphology, syntax and lexicon. 30
  • 31. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) PART NINE: CLASSIFICATION OF LANGUAGES Indo-European family of languages Italic (Latin) o Romance  Catalan  French  Italian  Occitan (Provençal)  Portuguese  Rhaeto-Romansch  Romanian  Spanish Germanic o North Germanic  Danish  Faroese  Icelandic  Norwegian  Swedish o East Germanic  Gothic (extinct) o West Germanic  Afrikaans  Dutch  English  Flemish  Frisian  German  Yiddish Slavic o Western  Czech  Polish 31
  • 32. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)  Slovak  Sorbian o Eastern  Belarusian  Russian  Ukrainian o Southern  Bulgarian  Croatian  Macedonian  Old Church Slavonic  Serbian  Slovene Baltic o Latvian o Lithuanian o Old Prussian (extinct) Celtic o Brythonic  Breton  Cornish (extinct)  Gaulish (extinct)  Welsh o Goidelic  Irish  Manx Gaelic (extinct)  Scots Gaelic Hellenic (Greek) Albanian Armenian Anatolian (extinct) Tocharian (extinct) Indo-Iranian o Indo-Aryan (Indic)  Assamese  Bengali  Bihari  Gujarati 32
  • 33. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.)  Hindi-Urdu  Marathi  Punjabi  Romani  Sanskrit  Sindhi  Singhalese o Iranian  Avestan  Balochi  Farsi (Persian)  Kurdish  Pashtu (Afghan)  Sogdian Uralic (or Finno-Ugric) is the other major family of languages spoken on the European continent. Finnish, Estonian and Hungarian are examples. Afro-Asiatic languages are spoken in Northern Africa and the Middle East. They include Berber, Egyptian, Omotic and Cushitic languages (Somali, Iraqw) as well as the modern Semitic languages of Hebrew, Arabic and Amharic, in addition to languages spoken in biblical times, such as Aramaic, Akkadian, Babylonian, Canaanite, and Phoenician. The Altaic languages are classified as Japanese and Korean, though some linguists separate these languages into their own groups. Sino-Tibetan languages include Mandarin, Hakka, Wu, Burmese, Tibetan, and all of the Chinese "dialects." Austro-tai languages include Indonesian, Javanese and Thai; while the Asiatic group includes Vietnamese. The Dravidian languages of Tamil and Telugu are spoken in southeastern India and Sri Lanka. The Caucasian language family consists of 40 different languages, and is divided into Cartvelian (south Caucasian), North-West Caucasian and North-East 33
  • 34. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Caucasian language groups. Some languages are Georgian, Megrelian, Chechen, Ingush Avarian, Lezgian and Dargin. These languages are mostly spoken in Georgia, Turkey, Syria, Iran, Jordan and parts of the Russian federation. The Niger-Congo family includes most of the African languages. About 1,500 languages belong to this group, including the Bantu languages of Swahili, Tswana, Xhosa, Zulu, Kikuyu, and Shona. Other languages are Ewe, Mina, Yoruba, Igbo, Wolof, Kordofanian and Fulfulde. Other African language groups are Nilo-Saharan, which includes 200 languages spoken in Central and Eastern Africa; and Khoisan, the click languages of southern Africa. The Khoisan group only contains about 30 languages, most of which are spoken in Namibia and Botswana. The Austronesian family also contains about 900 languages, spoken all over the globe. Hawaiian, Maori, Tagalog, and Malay are all representatives of this language family. Many languages are, or were, spoken in North and South America by the native peoples before the European conquests. Knowledge of these languages is limited, and because many of the languages are approaching extinction, linguists have little hope of achieving a complete understanding of the Amerindian language families. 34
  • 35. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Linguistics 101: Introduction to Linguistics I - Fall 1996 Anthony Kroch 614 Williams Hall 898-3212 kroch@change.ling.upenn.edu Linguistics 101 is an introduction to linguistics from the point of view of its logical and mathematical foundations. Its goal is to teach the basic structural properties of natural language sentences in their syntactic, semantic and pragmatic aspects, as well as the elements of automata theory and logic needed to describe natural language precisely. The course begins with a discussion of the philosophical and psychological foundations of modern linguistics and moves from there to more technical matters. Linguistics 101 has no prerequisites and fulfills the formal reasoning requirement in SAS. It is also a basic part of the Linguistics major and of the interdisciplinary Cognitive Science minor. Related courses are Linguistics 150: Introduction to Syntax and Linguistics 105: Introduction to Cognitive Science. Fall 1996 Course Syllabus Instructor: Teaching Assistants: Anthony Kroch Alan Lee &Rashmi Prasad 614 Williams Hall 429 Williams Hall 898-3212 898-6050 Email addresses: kroch@change.ling.upenn.edu aleewk@babel.ling rjprasad@babel.ling Texts: 1. Steven Pinker. The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow. 2. Course Bulkpack. 35
  • 36. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) This course will introduce you to linguistics, one of the core disciplines of the modern science of mind, from a mathematical point of view. Linguistics is a new science, less than a century old; but we have learned much in this brief period of scientific study about the uniquely human capacity to code abstract thought in communicable form. In the course of this semester, we will explore some of the results obtained in the scientific investigation of language, especially what has been discovered regarding the formal structure of our linguistic capacities and how our biologically endowed language faculty allows us to represent meanings as structured sequences of words. The course will show you that it is possible to study human language rigorously and scientifically and that such study leads to a conception of language quite different from our everyday common-sense notions of the subject. Requirements for the course consist of a midterm and a final exam, as well as a series of homework assignments, roughly one per week through the semester. The course is organized around an interesting recent book by Steven Pinker entitled "The Language Instinct." This book is not a textbook but it covers in a readable way the basic aspects of the study of language. There will also be readings to supplement the Pinker book, which are available in the course bulkpack. Topic 1: Introduction. How can there be a scientific study of language? How are language and thought related? What grammar is and how we learn it. What we can learn about language from the study of people who lose it or are kept by circumstances from acquiring language in the normal way. Reading: Steven Pinker. The Language Instinct. Chapters 1-3. Perlmutter. "The Language of the Deaf." Pullum. "The Great Eskimo Vocabulary Hoax." Topic 2: Formal grammar - the mathematical structure of language. This section of the course introduces the idea that languages can be analyzed mathematically. We discuss the relationship between computer languages and natural human languages and the notion of language complexity. We also introduce the concept of a phrase structure grammar, which underlies both computer and natural languages and the notion of a machine that automatically recognizes and parses sentences. 36
  • 37. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Reading: Michael Sipser. Introduction to the Theory of Computation. Chapters 0-2. Topic 3: Syntax - the structure of sentences in natural language. This section will be a focus of the course. Here we will learn how sentences are built up out of words and why speakers are able to construct sentences of any degree of complexity with a fixed vocabulary. We will also learn why linguists say that language is a rule-governed system and what some of the linguistic rules are that are used in the syntactic description of language. We will see how these rules determine both the structure of sentences and the conditions under which strings of words are interpretable as sentences of a language instead of being meaningless word salad. Reading: Pinker, chapter 4. Topic 4: Semantics. What is 'meaning'? Lexical semantics.Truth conditional semantics.The distinction between sense and reference.Semantic compositionality.The relationship between syntax and semantics. Reading: O'Grady, Dobrovolsky and Aronoff. Contemporary Linguistics: an Introduction. Chapter 6. Chierchia and McConnell-Ginet. Meaning and Grammar: An Introduction to Semantics. chapters 1-2. Topic 5: Pragmatics. How do speakers take account of listeners' knowledge and beliefs in formulating utterances? What inferences beyond literal meaning listeners draw from what is said to them. What acts can be performed merely by speaking. How context determines reference. The communicative effects of using particular syntactic constructions. Reading: Finegan and Besnier. Language, Its Structure and Use. chaps. 7, 10. Topic 6: Language in real time. How do listeners understand what speakers say? How do speakers convert thought into speech? How psycholinguists study what goes on inside our heads without looking inside them. 37
  • 38. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Reading: Pinker, Chapters 7-8. Akmajian et al. Linguistics: an Introduction to Language and Communication. Chapter 10. Assignment 1: Grammaticality This assignment is due at the beginning of the Monday lecture class on September 16. Be sure to write your TA's name and your recitation day on the top of your assignment. All future assignments should also be handed in at the beginning of the Monday lecture class in the week that they are due. Assignments will be returned in recitation sections, beginning on the Friday after they are handed in. 1. Prescriptive & Descriptive Grammar In order to answer this question, you will have to be familiar with some notation used in linguistics. An asterisk (*) is used to mark sentences which are ungrammatical. Grammatical sentences are unmarked. Recall that "ungrammatical" is used in the descriptive sense of interest to linguists, not in the prescriptive sense. Pinker, whose use of the term we will follow, defines "grammatical" as "well formed according to consistent rules in the dialect of [a] speaker..." (p. 31). The dialect we have in mind here is the spoken language of the general college-educated American population. (A) Look at the sentences in (1a) and (1b). Why is (1b) ungrammatical? a. Two paintings are on the wall. (1) b. *Two paintings is on the wall (B) Now consider the sentences in (2). Do the grammaticality judgments indicated correspond to your own? Assuming the correctness of these judgments, what rule or rules could a speaker use to generate the grammatical sentences (2a) and (2c) but not generate the ungrammatical sentence in (2b). Keep in mind that your rule(s) should also be able to account for the sentences in (1). How does your description of the sentences in (1) and (2) differ from prescriptions that govern standard usage? (2) a. There are two paintings on the wall. b. *There is two paintings on the wall 38
  • 39. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) c. There's two paintings on the wall. (C) The sentences in (3) show a similar pattern to the pattern seen in (1). However, the pattern in (4) is different from the one in (2). Write a description of the rules needed to generate the grammatical sentences in (3) and (4). Your rules should not generate the ungrammatical sentences. (3) a. A painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee are hanging on the wall. b. *A painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee is hanging on the wall (4) a. *There are a painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee hanging on the wall b. There is a painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee hanging on the wall. c. There's a painting by Picasso and a painting by Klee hanging on the wall. 2. Grammaticality Judgments (A) Decide whether the phrases in (1) - (15) are grammatical in your spoken dialect. (If you are not a native speaker of English, consult a native speaker for judgments). Mark ungrammatical sentences with an asterisk ( * ) and say briefly what's wrong with them. (1) To the bank. (2) The rat the cat the dog bit chased ran. (3) The cat the dog bit ran. (4) Being so flat, the Dutch bicycle everywhere. (5) Who do you wonder whether they will come. (6) Ivan a tin of caviar ate quickly. (7) Its mayor praised her village. (8) If you go to school, there's an elephant on the corner. (9) Susan told John that washing herself in public is a bad idea. (10) The candy ate the boy. (11) Immediately he opened the door he saw the murderer standing there. (12) The police officer arrested Sam and I. 39
  • 40. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) (13) Earlobe seven by hexed fruitless. (14) Go take dog for a walk! (15) Colorless green ideas sleep furiously. (B) Often it is possible to make sense of an ungrammatical sentence. Likewise a grammatical sentence can be incomprehensible. Mark the incomprehensible but grammatical sentences in (A) with a number sign (#). 3. American Sign Language Read the discussion of deaf sign language in Pinker and the Perlmutter article in the bulk pack before answering this question. Sign language users all over the world have been struggling for years to eradicate the notion that because they do not use speech, their communications systems are not "real" languages. One characteristic of languages in general is that there is an arbitrary relation between words and what they represent. You can't hear the French word 'chien,' for example, and know by its sound that it refers to what the English word 'dog' refers to. Critics of sign languages have often described them as "iconic," as a series of pictures and gestures for acting out the real world -- and thus dismissed them as nothing more than complex mime. Consider the issue of iconicity in American Sign Language (ASL) in light of the following evidence. (A) The signs for male and female: original: female: running thumb along jaw toward chin, mimicking bonnet strings male: grasping an invisible cap near the forehead current: female: thumb on chin, with a hand shape as if thumbing your nose at someone male: thumb on forehead, same handshape. -- How have these signs changed over time? How does this development affect the debate over whether signs are iconic or not? (B) First person pronouns: When hearing children are first learning to speak, they often display a charming tendency to confuse the pronouns 'you' and 'me.' When asked, "Do you want 40
  • 41. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) milk," they reply, "Yes, you want milk," believing that they are describing themselves. In a curious parallel, deaf children who are learning to sign will display, at the same age, the tendency to confuse the signs for 'you' and 'me.' The adult will point to the child and ask a question, and the child will point at the adult in reply, even though, once again, these children are describing themselves. -- Why do you think children make these mistakes? Based on the assumption that ASL is a true language, would you expect hearing children (who are not exposed to ASL) to make the same mistake as deaf children when responding to pointing? Why or why not? (C) Character placing: When telling a story, an ASL signer is likely to name the characters at the beginning (or whenever they appear) and in doing so, to "place" them at some location in space (one to the left, and one to the right, for example). From that point on, the signer will refer to those locations by pointing instead of repeating the names. -- Does these rules for pointing remind you of anything in spoken language? (D) Handshapes: While fingerspelling is not a grammatical part of ASL, many signs in ASL are signed with the handshape of the first letter in the English word -- 'language' is signed with the "l" shape, 'class' with the "c" shape, and 'water' with a "w". The colors blue, purple, green, orange and yellow are all signed with the same motion, shaking the initial letter (b, p, g, o, or y) back and forth. 'Apple' is an "a" shape rotated at cheek level. At the same time, 'onion' is an "x" shape moved the same way, so this pattern does not always not hold. -- How do these facts impact upon the iconicity debate? (E) Iconicity in spoken language: There are iconic elements in ordinary spoken English. Give some examples. In what ways are similar to and/or different from iconic features of ASL? 41
  • 42. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Assignment 2: Language, Grammar, and Thought 1.Tongan Syntax The following (simplified) sentences are from Tongan, a Polynesian language spoken on the island of Tonga in the Pacific. Each sentence is glossed (directly translated) and an English translation is also provided. The following abbreviations are used: Pr=present tense, Pst =past tense, Nom=nominative case, Acc=accusative case, 1ps=First person singular, 2ps=Second person singular. Answer the following questions based on your observations of sentences (1)-(4): (a) what would you say is the main structural difference between Tongan and English? (b) how is tense realized in Tongan? (c) are the nouns marked in any particular way? (1) oku ui ehe- tamasiae- tangata Pr call Nom child Acc man 'The child calls the man.' (2) oku kai ehe- fefine ae- ufi Pr eat Nom- womanAcc- yam 'The woman eats the yam.' (3) nae ako ehe- tamasi ae- lesoni Pst study Nom- child Acc- lesson 'The child studied the lesson.' (4) nae haka ehe- fefine ae- ika Pst boil Nom- woman Acc- fish 'The woman boiled the fish.' How are the following sentences structurally different from the ones above? Also, why is the 1ps pronoun in (5) different from the one in (8)? Similarly, why is the 2ps in (6) different from the one in (7)? (5) nae ku ui ae- tangata Pst 1ps call Acc- man 42
  • 43. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) 'I called the man.' (6) oku ke kai ae- laise Pr 2ps eat Acc- rice 'You eat rice.' (7) oku manako koe ehe- tangata Pr like 2ps Nom- man 'The man likes you.' (8) nae tokonia au ehe- kakai Pst help 1ps Nom- man 'People helped me.' If you have adequately analyzed the above sentences, you should now be able to do some simple translations from English into Tongan! Try translating the following: The man scares the child. ('scare' = 'fakailifiai') The woman saw me. ('see' = 'vakai') You ate the fish. You like me. 2. TheRelationship betweenLanguage andThought. It is by now a well known fact (and those of you who have tried to learn a foreign language will undoubtedly admit this) that certain things can be expressed more conveniently in some languages than in others. While one language may have a special word to refer exclusively to a particular object or notion, in another language this object or notion can be described only by using a whole phrase or sentence. For example, in Tuvaluan, a language spoken by the Polynesian inhabitants of a group of islands in the Central Pacific, there are different words to refer to many different types of coconut, which need to be described at great length in English. Here are a few examples: pii : drinking coconut, with little flesh and much water, at a stage when the water is maximally sweet mukomuko: young coconut with some flesh in it, before it has become too solid uto : coconut at the stage when its husk can be chewed on and its water is still sweet motomoto : same as mukomuko, but with firmer flesh 43
  • 44. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) niu : coconut ripe enough for its flesh to be grated uttanu : mature coconut whose sprout has already pierced through the husk and whose water has turned into an edible spongious solid kernel How much can we conclude from examples like this one about the relationship between the language that people speak and the way that they think? Do examples like this one support the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis?. 3. Grammar of Non-Standard English Study the following three groups of sentences of Appalachian English. The sentences of each group share a grammatical feature not found in Standard English. Describe these features. (1) Boy, I started to runnin'. A vein in his nose bursted and he went to hemorragin'. She practically raised 'im 'til he got up to walkin'. Just recently, I had an aunt to come from Texas. Usually, I hafta have somebody else to do it. (note: 'went' and 'got' is roughly the equivalent of Standard English's 'started') (2) I'd go out and cut me a limb off a tree, get me a good straight one. We had us a cabin, built us a log cabin back over there. And then you'd get you a bowl of ice water. He wanted some straw to build him a house out of. I'm gonna write me a letter to the President. (3) I got some kin people lived up there. He's the funny lookin' character plays baseball. 'Cause there was this vampire that killed people come in the house. My grandma's got this thing tells me about when to plant. 4. Two puzzles (a). Included in your bulkpack there is an excerpt from an interview with Miss Manners. What point relevant to a linguistics course does she make in her discussion of ettiquette books? (b). Right after the Miss Manners interview there is a page with a Domino's Pizza advertisement. Why do you think this page was included in the course bulkpack? 44
  • 45. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Assignment 3: Sentences as Structured Objects 1.Expressivity of Language Consider the following sentences : a) I hate war. b) You know that I hate war. c) He says that you know that I hate war. Construct a sentence that includes sentence (c) and then construct another sentence that includes your new sentence. Can you repeat the process again to create an even longer sentence? How long do you think you can go on? Why? 2. Structural Ambiguity Our syntactic knowledge goes beyond our ability to decide which strings are grammatical and which are not. It accounts for the structural ambiguity of expressions like synthetic buffalo hides. The ambiguity results from the fact that synthetic can modify buffalo hides or simply hides to result in two different interpretations. It is therefore due to the syntactic structure that the expression has two meanings and not due to any ambiguous words. Paraphrase each of the following sentences in two different ways to show that you understand the ambiguity involved : a) Smoking grass can be nauseating. b) Rob finally decided on the boat. c) Old men and women are hard to live with. d) That sheepdog is too hairy to eat. e) Terry loves his wife and so do I. f) They said she would go yesterday. Is the type of ambiguity in the above sentences different from the ambiguity in the following sentences : g) I walked by the bank yesterday. h) Thomas Jefferson ate his cottage cheese with relish. 3. Mathematical exercises Do exercises 3, 4, and 8 on page 24 at the end of chapter 0 of Sipser. 45
  • 46. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) Assignment 4: Mathematical preliminaries A.Alphabets, Strings and Languages Let be the alphabet containing the symbols a and b. In other words: = {a, b}. A language L contains all strings over which either begin with a and end with b, or begin with b and end with a. State which of the following strings belong to L, and which do not: i) ab ii) baa iii) abba iv) baba v) bubba vi) b Concatenation is an operation on strings where one string is appended to the end of another string. For example, if we have two strings xy and yx, we can do the operation xy o yx (where 'o' is the symbol denoting concatenation) to yield a new string xyyx. Now, when we concatenate certain strings from the language L, we get a new string which still belongs to L. For example, the strings aab and abb are valid strings in L. The operation aab o abb gives us the new string aababb which still belongs to L (since it starts with an a and ends with ab). However, the concatenation of two valid strings of L does not always yield a new string which also belongs to L. Provide some counterexamples of when the concatenation of two strings of L results in a new string which does NOT belong to L. B. Relations and Functions The above diagram shows the maternal family tree of a certain family. F is the set containing all the members of this family. Let M be the set containing all mothers in F. Hence, M = {Eve, Jennifer, Mary, Kate}. Let the relation R = "is the mother of". The statement mRf, where m is an element of set M and f is an element of set F, simply says that m is the mother of f. We can show this relationship diagramatically: The above diagram shows a normal relation, where an element in the first group can map onto more than one element in the second group. 46
  • 47. (Source: An Introduction to Language by Victoria Fromkin and Robert Rodman, 6th Ed.) a) Now, let S be the set containing all sons in F. List the set S. b) where R = "is the son of", s S, and f F. c) c) Briefly explain why this relation is a function. d) d) Let G be the set of siblings in F. List G. Is gRf a function, where R = "is the sibling of", e) G, and f F. C. Finite State Automata Do Exercises 1.1, 1.2, 1.3 and 1.4a (on page 75) from Chapter 1 of Sipser. Assignment 5: Finite State Automata and Regular Grammars 1. Spot the Problem A language L is described as follows: L = all strings that begin with a 0, end with a 2, and contain at most three 1s The alphabet of this language is {0, 1, 2} a) Give an example of a string in this language which does not contain a 1. b) Give an example of a string in this language of length 10. c) Does the following finite state accept the language L? If not, how would you change it so that it does accept exactly the language L? (Hint: You need to add an extra state.) 2. Introduction to the trees program. Download the Trees 2 program by following the instructions on the Trees web page and the grammars G1, and G2, and G3 . For each of the grammars answer the following questions. a) Describe the language generated by the grammar. b) Draw a finite state automaton that accepts the same language. Assignment 6: More Machines and Grammars 1.More Finite Automata Consider a finite automata whose input alphabet is S = {the, old, man, men, is, are, here, and}. a) Construct a state diagram for an automaton which accepts the following language: {the man is here, the men are here}. 47