SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 125
Running
head:
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
1
Research Proposal
Ashleigh Barber
Australian Catholic University
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
2
Research Proposal
Background
Australia’s Ageing population
At present, Australia’s population is aged, as indicated by the
large proportion of older
adult residents, and . This would at least be partially
attributable to 100 years of progressively
greater longevities (Howat & Stoneham, 2011). With increasing
longevity come several
challenges, for individuals and the health care sector if a
number individuals later life is spent in
a state of morbidity. While normal ageing research is not
without contention (e.g., about the
underpinning processes in cognitive decline), evidence suggests
that the process of normal
ageing appears to involve a decline in such higher order
cognitive domains as memory and
information processing REF, which are crucial for living
autonomously. While many cognitive
functions are studied in relation to ageing, memory impairment
topic of research in older
adulthood, as it comprises multiple cognitive abilities that are
each fundamental to in successful
ageing.
The Ageing Process
A normal ageing trajectory human beings represents a latent,
time-dependent biological
process (Moody & Sasser, 2014), which albeit it is not
pathological, is often characterised by
functional degradation in one or more of physical, sensory, or
cognitive abilities (Desjardins,
2012). Many adults experience some age-related cognitive
decline (Deary et al., 2009), but the
explicit causal underpinnings of such changes are not robustly
understood. The idea of negative
learning was proposed in Mahncke et al.’s (2006) framework,
which argues that normal
cognitive changes stem from the combined effect of physical
ageing of the cortices and negative
cortical plasticity (i.e., plasticity with negative consequences)
(Mahncke, Bronstone, and
Merzenich, 2006).
Briefly outsine the scaffolding idea and negative cortical
plasticity
Normal ageing
The maximum upper limit of human lifespan
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
3
The maximum human lifespan is indicated to have a fixed
theoretical upper limit of 120 years
(Moody & Sasser, 2014). This upper bound has remained
unchanged over time, while the life
expectancy continues to climb.
Promoting optimal development right across the lifespan is a
crucial task of developmental
psychology
Compression of morbidity hypothesis
Discuss briefly in conjunction: max lifespan, compression of
morbidity theory in relation
to health goals and the possibility of human beings, as
prevention strategies implemented with
healthy participants fosters the healthy trajectory not the one of
morbidity.
“With this concept of limit in mind, compression of morbidity is
attractive because
delaying dysfunction would enhance the quality of life, extend
life expectancy, and reduce health
care costs (Butler, 1995). A compression-of-morbidity strategy
would move life “
Prospective Memory
What is PM (related to RM, what do they encompass?)
Qualitative study: Subjective complaints of particcipants were
all PM and some
RM, some really dangerous (community-dwelling older adults,
focus group)
Types of PM, cued.
Cortical Plasticity
Evidence of maintaining plasticity throughout lifespan
Negative learning (NCP)
Conclusions of OA misbeliefs (negative learning)
Cognitive training programs
Extant research
Process vs. system designs
Virtual week
Original, P. R.
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
4
Computerised, 2000
VW14, computerised with graded difficulty etc.
Rationale.
Gaps in literature study aims to address
Firstly, relatively few studies include control groups when
running studies with cognitive
training (REF), yet it is crucial for being able to make causal
inferences about its efficacy.
Secondly, many cognitive training programs are conducted in
laboratories, which limits
ecological validity. The design of the study has participants
carrying out the tasks in their own
home, without the experimenter there was they play, and the
tasks are relevant to functioning
outside of the study. Finally, the adherence of participants to
the prescribed training program
and protocol is not a prioritised assessment in training studies.
Current Study
Aims and objectives.
The two primary objectives of this pilot study are to examine
both the usability and
assess the preliminary efficacy of 6-weeks of Virtual Week
Training 2014 (VWT14) program for
improving prospective memory in healthy Australian older
adults. In addition, this study will aim
to analyse changes in subjective reports of memory functioning
following 6-weeks of VWT14
program.
VWT14 may represent one way of decelerating normal
prospective memory decline in
older adulthood, and as such, it is hypothesised that participants
who have completed the training
program will have significant improvement in their prospective
memory compared to waitlist
controls, based on Memory for Intentions Screening Test
(MIST) (S. Raskin, Buckheit, &
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
5
Sherrod, 2010) performance. It is also hypothesised that when
all participants’ posttest data are
pooled, there will significant changes from pre- to posttest
performance on the MIST. Finally, it
is hypothesised that subjective reports of memory, based on the
Memory Function Questionnaire
Revised (MFQ-R, Gilewski, Zelinski, & Schaie, 1990), will
improve from pre- to posttest.
Research Methodology
Participants
A B C D E F
1
Recruitment
phase
Prospective
participants
2
Make contact
(phone/email)
Pre-screen
checklist
First assessment
3 Arrange in-person meeting
Cog screen; BG
survey; MFQ
4
Participants
assigned to 1/2
conditions
5
Stratified by
age, gender,
ACE-III score
6 Pretesting phase
Figure 1: Participant Selection Map (coordinates referred to
throughout research methods section)
Study design
This pilot study will be a pre-post-test waitlist control design
with both intervention and
comparison groups, so as to pilot the VWT14 program as a
means of improving prospective
memory in healthy older adults.
Inclusion/exclusion criteria
Participants will be deemed eligible to partake in the study if
they meet all of the criteria
examined during the screening assessment (see Appendix 1).
Measures
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
6
Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination III ipad version
(ACEmobile) (Newman, Noad,
& Hodges, 2014)
The ACE-III (Mathuranath, Nestor, Berrios, Rakowicz, &
Hodges, 2000) is a sensitive
and specific screening tool for general cognitive functioning
that takes appropriately 15 minutes
to administer. Nineteen brief subtests comprise the following
five dimensions:
attention/orientation, memory, verbal fluency, language, and
visuo-spatial, which give five sub-
scores and a total score out of 100. The ACE-III has the
capacity to screen for cognitive
impairment and diagnoses, severity and profile of cognitive
functioning, and has three Australian
alternate forms for assessing change and comparing scores
between groups. ACEmobile, an
electronic version of ACE-III was released for the iPad June
2014, and will be used in this study.
It enhances administration through on-screen instructions,
requires no training, reduces error by
scoring automatically, and provides results instantly on-screen
and via email to the researcher.
Memory Functioning Questionnaire Revised REF
DESCRIBE TEST
Memory for Intentions Screening Test
The Memory for Intentions Screening Test (MIST) (S. A.
Raskin, 2009) is a standardised
tool for measuring prospective memory that takes approximately
30 minutes to administer. The
MIST is reported to have sound psychometric properties (Woods
et al., 2008), and be sensitive to
mild impairment (Karantzoulis, Troyer, & Rich, 2009).
Participants are provided with an eight-
task set of tests at the outset of a testing session and must
remember, throughout the session, to
perform both time-based tasks (‘in exactly x minutes, please
alert me that it is break time’) and
event-based tasks (‘when I pass you the blue pen, may you write
your name on this sheet of
paper’). Between these PM tasks, participants solve word
puzzles, while the researcher observes
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
7
whether the PM tasks are remembered and carried out. Correct
and error responses are both
recorded, while variables and error scores can be combined to
discern different types of
prospective memory fail
Recruitment
Participants will be sourced from the community (Catholic
homes, advertisements in
relevant public locations, word-of-mouth) (A1, 2, 3).
Prospective participants will be asked to
contact the researcher via either email or telephone if they are
interested in partaking or to find
out more information about the study (B1, B2, 3). The
researcher will then screen these
individuals over the phone using a checklist (C2).
Procedure
After eligible participants have been explained the details of the
study (C2) and provided
informed consent (D2), they will then be asked to fill in a
background information survey, the
Memory Functions Questionnaire (Revised), and be screened on
global cognitive ability to
ensure healthy cognitive functioning on the ACE-III (D3). Both
the intervention and waitlist
control groups will partake in the VWT14 program, but the
immediate intervention group will
commence the 6-week training within the first two weeks of
screening (C2), while the waitlist
control will commence their 6-week block around 8 weeks after
screening (C2). Given the data
collection and intervention implementation procedures are
conducted one participant at a time
rather than altogether in a group, so long as each participant has
the appropriate intervals across
screening, pre-test, and post-test for their assigned condition
(i.e., immediate intervention or
waitlist control), the waitlist control group does not necessarily
have to wait until each
immediate intervention participant has completed the training.
Specifically, so long as the
waitlist control has had a 6-week no-contact interval after
screening, they can be pre-tested and
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
8
commence training as soon as this 6-week period is over (see
Appendix 2 for visual
representation).
The Memory for Intentions Test (MIST) (S. Raskin et al., 2010)
will be administered at
two time points for each participant: for the immediate
intervention group, first time (pretest)
will be within two weeks following the first meeting between
participant and researcher (D2, 3),
and two weeks before the start of the VWT14 program, and
second time (posttest) will be within
a week of completing VWT14. For the waitlist control group,
their pretest will be 6 weeks after
the first meeting between participant and researcher, and this
pretest data will serve as a
comparison point for intervention effects.
Planned Statistical Analyses
The following table outlines how data will be analysed in order
to address the aims and
objectives of the study. Due to the nature of a pilot study, the
sample size is projected to be
relatively small so until distributions are analysed, it is
unknown whether non- or parametric
statistical analyses will be more appropriate, so both are
recorded below where necessary.
Table 1
Planned Statistical Analyses
Assessing By: Variables Statistics
Efficacy (a)
Of VW at
improving PM
performance on
MIST (control
comparison)
Change scores on
MIST from pre- to
posttest: between
groups difference
(independent
samples)
IV1 group:
intervention /control
IV2 time point:
pretest/ posttest
DV change scores:
MIST
Hodges-Lehmann estimate of
median differences (estimates the
median difference between the
groups’ change scores from pre-post
test)
Efficacy (b) Of VW at
improving PM
performance on
MIST (pooled
comparison)
Scores on MIST,
difference from pre-
to posttest: within
groups difference
(paired sample)
IV1 time point:
pretest/ posttest
DV scores: MIST
Paired samples t-test or Wilcoxon
test (changes in the same
participants’ scores across two time
points)
Efficacy (c) Of VW at Scores on MFQ IV1 time point: Paired
samples t-test or Wilcoxon
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
9
improving
subjective
perceptions of
memory (pooled
comparison)
difference from pre-
to posttest: within
groups difference
pretest/ posttest
DV scores: MFQ
test (changes in the same
participants’ scores across two time
points)
Usability
Of VW training
program for
older adults
Scores on VWUQ-
14, an adapted
version of Lewis’
1995 questionnaire
DV scores: VWUQ-
14
Descriptive statistics: means,
standard deviations, confidence
intervals, and % ≥ or ≤ given scoring
ranges (i.e., difficulty: easy, med,
hard) according to author’s
recommended scoring.
Adherence/
compliance
To training
program’s
prescribed
protocol
Rate of sessions
completed per week
(χ/4) (& total χ/24)
DV rate: of
completed sessions
out of 4
Descriptive statistics: means, SDs,
CIs, n’s and % rates of completed
sessions per week, total over
program, and % of participants
completing all prescribed sessions.
Usability of VWT14
A usability questionnaire (adapted from Lewis, 1995) will be
administered via email or
post within one week of each participant’s final VWT14 session
(as shown by automated data).
Participants rated how strongly they agreed with each item on a
scale of 1(Strongly Disagree) to
7 (Strongly Agree) (see Appendix 3).
Efficacy of VWT14
The primary efficacy outcome measure was performance on the
MIST (S. Raskin et al.,
2010) (with control comparison), which aims to objectively
measure prospective memory.
The secondary efficacy outcome measures were performance on
the MIST (without
control comparison), and responses on the MFQ, a self-report
questionnaire that assessed
subjective perceptions of memory functioning.
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
10
Appendix 1:
Pre-Screen and Screener Assessments
The following will be asked/assessed during phone calls with
prospective participants (pre-
screen)
Inclusion Criteria
No. Question Y N
1 Are you aged 60 or older
2 Are you retired
3 Do you have a windows computer?
4 Are you comfortable using the computer
5 Are you living independently in the community (i.e. without
assistance)
6 Are you comfortable reading and understanding written
instructions in English
Exclusion Criteria
1 Do you have any neuro/psychiatric disorders or medical
conditions that could affect your ability
to play a computer game e.g., epilepsy, blindness
2 Are you involved in another research study at this time
The following will be information will be collected during the
first face-to-face meeting
(Screener)
1. Cognitive functioning using Addenbrooke’s Cognitive
Examination V.3 (ACE-III) ipad
version
2. Background information using a demographic questionnaire
3. Subjective memory functioning using the MFQ
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
11
Appendix 2:
Data Collection and Intervention Timelines for Two Groups
The following two hypothetical timelines are displaying one of
two pathways each participant
can take depending on which group they are assigned.
1
2 ! !
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
In
te
rv
en
ti
on
w
ee
ks
o
f
as
se
ss
m
en
t a
nd
in
te
rv
en
ti
on
POSTTEST
In
te
rv
en
ti
on
PRETESTPOSTTEST
N
o contact
PRETEST
Immediate
Intervention
Waitlist
Control
SCREENER
PRESCREEN
PARTICIPANT RECRUITMENT
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
12
Appendix 3:
Usability Questionnaire
The following questionnaire will be given at the completion of
the 6-week training period; it is
adapted from Lewis (1995).
Virtual Week 14 Usability Questionnaire (VWUQ-14)
For each of the statements below, please circle the rating of
your choice.
1 Overall, I am satisfied with the ease of completing the Virtual
Week program
Strongly
AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly
DISAGREE
2 I am satisfied with the availability of support information
(phone calls, messages,
instructions/documentation) for completing this task
Strongly
AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly
DISAGREE
3 I am satisfied with how clear/easy to understand
instructions/documentation for
completing this task was
Strongly
AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly
DISAGREE
4 I enjoyed playing Virtual Week
Strongly
AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly
DISAGREE
5 It was easy to learn to play/use Virtual Week
Strongly
AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly
DISAGREE
6 I felt comfortable using the Virtual Week program
Strongly
AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly
DISAGREE
7 I would recommend Virtual Week to family and friends
Strongly
AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly
DISAGREE
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
13
Appendix 4:
Memory Functioning Questionnaire-Revised
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
14
110
Se riousness of Fo rgetting
When you actually forget in these situations, how serious of a
problem do you consider the
memory to be?
very serious somewhat serious not serious
a. names 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
b. faces 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
c. where you put things (e.g., keys) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
d. directions to places 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
e. beginning to do something and
forgetting what you were doing 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
_____________________________________________________
_________________________
R et rospective F unctioning
How is your memory compared to the way is was?
much worse same much better
a. 1 year ago? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
b. 5 years ago? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
c. 10 years ago? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
d. 20 years ago? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
e. when you were 18? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
_____________________________________________________
_________________________
M nemonics Usage
How often do you use these techniques to remind yourself about
things?
always sometimes never
a. keep an appointment book 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
b. write yourself reminder notes 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
c. make lists of things to do 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
d. make grocery lists 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
e. plan your daily schedule
in advance 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
f. mental repetition 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
g. associations with other things 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
h. keep things you need to do in a
prominent place where you will
notice them 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
_____________________________________________________
_________________________
RESEARCH
PROPOSAL
15
References
Deary, I. J., Corley, J., Gow, A. J., Harris, S. E., Houlihan, L.
M., Marioni, R. E., . . . Starr, J. M.
(2009). Age-associated cognitive decline. British Medical
Bulletin, 92(1), 135-152.
Desjardins, R. (2012). Ageing and skills: a review and analysis
of skill gain and skill loss over
the lifespan and over time.
Gilewski, M. J., Zelinski, E. M., & Schaie, K. W. (1990). The
Memory Functioning
Questionnaire for assessment of memory complaints in
adulthood and old age.
Psychology and aging, 5(4), 482.
Howat, P., & Stoneham, M. (2011). Why sustainable population
growth is a key to climate
change and public health equity. Health Promotion Journal of
Australia, 22(4), 34-38.
Karantzoulis, S., Troyer, A. K., & Rich, J. B. (2009).
Prospective memory in amnestic mild
cognitive impairment. Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society, 15(3),
407-415. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090596
Lewis, J. R. (1995). IBM computer usability satisfaction
questionnaires: Psychometric evaluation
and instructions for use. International Journal of Human-
Computer Interaction, 7(1), 57-
78.
Mathuranath, P. S., Nestor, P. J., Berrios, G. E., Rakowicz, W.,
& Hodges, J. R. (2000). A brief
cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer's disease and
frontotemporal dementia.
Neurology, 55(11), 1613-1620.
Moody, H. R., & Sasser, J. R. (2014). Aging: Concepts and
Controversies: SAGE Publications.
Newman, C., Noad, R., & Hodges, J. (2014). ACEmobile
(Version iPad): Neuroscience
Research Australia & Plymouth University. Retrieved from
http://www.acemobile.org
Raskin, S., Buckheit, C., & Sherrod, C. (2010). Memory for
intentions test (MIST). Lutz, FL:
Psychological Assessment Resources.
Raskin, S. A. (2009). Memory for intentions screening test:
Psychometric properties and clinical
evidence. Brain Impairment, 10(1), 23-33. doi:
10.1375/brim.10.1.23
Woods, S. P., Moran, L. M., Dawson, M. S., Carey, C. L.,
Grant, I., Atkinson, J. H., . . . Vaida,
F. (2008). Psychometric characteristics of the memory for
intentions screening test.
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22(5), 864-878. doi:
10.1080/13854040701595999
Prospective memory
Successful prospective memory depends on retrospective
(episodic) memory processes (e.g., encoding, retrieval), as well
as controlled attention/executive processes associated with
planning, working memory, and task switching
As such, having good prospective memory may be especially
important for older adults, who often have health-related
prospective memory demands, such as remembering to take
medication. Prospective memory (PM) refers to memory for
future intentions, such as remembering to take medication, to
turn off appliances, pay bills and keep appointments. It involves
“remembering to carry out actions” (Huppert & Beardsall,
1993), or “remembering to do things in the future” (McDaniel,
Einstein, & Rendell, 2008). PM involves recognising that a task
needs to be undertaken in the future, encoding the task and the
cue to perform the task, storing the task and the cue, monitoring
for and recognising the cue while engaged in other ongoing
tasks, with retrieval of the content of the task when the cue is
recognised. Thus PM is not a single cognitive process but
contains elements of multiple cognitive processes including
attention, working memory, retrospective memory and executive
functioning (Shum & Fleming, 2009).
This ability is crucial to the maintenance of functional
independence, which is a fundamental concern for older adults
(Chasteen, Park, & Schwarz, 2001). Problems with PM cause
more deficits in activities of daily living, instrumental activities
of daily living and caregiver burden than do retrospective
memory (RM) failures (Smith, Della Sala, Logie, & Maylor,
2000). It is therefore of considerable concern that PM is often
disrupted in the context of normal adult ageing (Henry,
MacLeod, Phillips, & Crawford, 2004; Kvavilashvili, Kornbrot,
Mash, Cockburn, & Milne, 2009)and to an even greater extent
in dementia, even in the mild (Martins & Damasceno, 2008) and
preclinical stages (Duchek, Balota, & Cortese, 2006; Jones,
Livner, & Backman, 2006). As will be discussed, there is
evidence of PM impairment in individuals with Mild Cognitive
Impairment (MCI), a condition in which cognitive difficulties
are present, but diagnostic criteria for dementia are not met
(Petersen, 2004).
In contrast to PM, retrospective memory (RM) refers to memory
of past knowledge and events. Most memory assessment and
research to date has focused on this type of memory function,
using tests of learning, recall and recognition such as the
Wechsler Memory Scales (Wechsler, 1945, 1997b) and Rey
Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; Rey, 1964). Normal
adult ageing is associated with decline in some cognitive
abilities (Rabbitt & Lowe, 2000), including RM (Ivnik, Malec,
Smith, Tangalos, Petersen, Kokmen, & Kurland, 1992; Kral,
Cahn, & Mueller, 1964).
Memory training (cognitive training to enhance functioning or
postpone decline)
Training programs designed to enhance memory among older
adults have proliferated in recent years. Memory training
programs exist within the larger context of cognitive training,
which is based on the notion that cognition is plastic in older
age (Acevedo & Lowenstein, 2007; Rebok, Carlson, &
Langbaum, 2007). Through cognitive plasticity and reserve
(Stern, 2002), mentally stimulating activities like memory
training help maintain and improve cognitive and functional
abilities in daily life (Hertzog et al., 2008). Memory training
interventions facilitate plasticity by teaching various strategies
that help encode and retrieve information (Gross & Rebok,
2011; McDaniel, Einstein, & Jacoby, 2008; Rebok et al., 2007;
Verhaeghen, Marcoen, & Goossens, 1992). In addition to
improving objective memory performance, research shows
interventions can improve self-rated subjective memory (Floyd
& Scogin, 1997).
Framework for prospective memory and ageing
There is some evidence that prospective memory deteriorates
with age (Huppert et al., 2000). This age difference is more
apparent when using laboratory assessments as opposed to more
naturalistic assessment paradigms (Henry et al., 2004). It is
difficult to ascertain if prospective memory or retrospective
memory is more affected by age, because of heterogeneous
methodologies (Maylor et al., 2002). Prospective memory
errors, however, may be more concerning and salient than
retrospective memory errors (Maylor et al., 2002). Hence why
the individual components of memory (PM, RM, WM) will be
measured.
Prospective memory has been studied most extensively with
regard to its functioning in older adults (typically those over
60 years of age). Craik (1986) stimulated an early and abiding
interest in examining prospective memory in older adults with
his seminal framework on age-related memory deficits. Craik
suggested that memory tasks could be ordered in terms of the
amount of self-initiated processes required to retrieve the target
information. Generally, the fewer the cues provided by the
memory task, the more retrieval is dependent on processes
initiated by the individual, such as generating possible cues,
generating potential targets, and implementing any other
strategies that will help bring the desired information to mind.
E.g., recognition is considered to have low self-initiated
retrieval demands, because the recognition task provides the
target item. In this scheme, prospective memory is thought to
have the highest self-initiated retrieval demands because not
only is there an absence of cues, but also one has to remember
to remember.
According to Craik's framework, because self-initiated retrieval
presumably requires extensive processing resources and because
processing resources decline with age, age-related deficits in
memory should be a function of the amount of self-initiated
retrieval required by the memory task. Because prospective
memory is assumed to require the most self-initiated retrieval,
this compelling theory makes the strong prediction that
prospective memory tasks would be especially difficult for
older adults.
Virtual Week Memory Training Program (computerised
Australian version, 2014)
A test that has shown considerable promise in research studies
of PM is Virtual Week (Rendell & Craik, 2000; Rendell &
Henry, 2009). Virtual Week takes the structure of a board game,
in which participants move through each day as they circuit the
board, with a series of events occurring and tasks to be
remembered each day. The test can be computerised to facilitate
data capture. Virtual Week has evidence of good reliability and
validity, with reliability estimates from .84 to .94 (Rose,
Rendell, & McDaniel, 2007) and validity established through
controlled studies showing group differences with poorer
performance in the aged (Rendell & Craik, 2000; Rose et al.,
2007) and in clinical populations including schizophrenia
(Henry et al., 2007), multiple sclerosis (Rendell et al., 2007b)
and substance abuse (Rendell et al., 2007a). As with Virtual
Street, Virtual Week has been argued to be more ecologically
valid than more conventional laboratory assessments, in that it
mirrors the types of PM tasks that occur in daily life (Rendell &
Henry, 2009).
One of the most important advantages of Virtual Week as a
measure of PM is the inclusion of time and event-based PM
cues and regular and irregular tasks. A unique and important
aspect of this test is that a differentiated profile of impairment
on Virtual Week may therefore be informative of the degree of
PM impairment per se, and also of the particular circumstances
in which PM impairment is more likely to arise (and
consequently the manner in which rehabilitation efforts should
be targeted).
While a number of promising measures of PM have now been
developed, of these, Virtual Week is notable for its
psychometric properties, for including tasks that vary in their
relative demands, and having documented sensitivity to both the
effects of normal adult ageing, as well as psychiatric and
neurological illness. Thus Virtual week was the test chosen for
a group comparison of PM performance in Study 1.
Training Protocol
In the training protocol, we used the original Virtual Week
paradigm and extended it to a process-based training version to
examine whether older adults’ prospective memory could be
improved and if training gains could transfer to other cognitive
and everyday life tasks (Rose et al., 2012). The study consisted
of three experimental groups, one group performed the Virtual
Week training; the second group, an active control group,
performed a music training; the third group was a waitlist, no-
contact control group. In total, 50 older adults between 60 and
80 years participated in the study.
It is anticipated the first training session will take 40-45 mins.
At the training session the patient participant will be provided
with a USB version of Virtual Week to use on their home
computer over six weeks. They will be advised to use the
training program four times per week over six weeks. Each
training session will take approximately 20 to 30 minutes to
complete each day. On insertion of the USB into the
participant’s computer, it will bring up prompts to begin the
training at the appropriate level and limit the number of
sessions that can be completed on each day.
The training version of Virtual Week has the critical factor of
adaptive difficulty where the task starts at a relatively easy
level and then is progressively more difficult, adapting to the
performance of the participant. Another key feature of this
program is the distributed practice of the cognitive training, in
this case over six weeks, rather than massed practice in one or
two long sessions.
A final key training feature, is that Virtual Week has provision
to provide feedback at the conclusion of each 'virtual day'
(which will occur twice per session) so that participants are
aware of their performance on each day and of their progression
through the difficulty levels. The program also has provision to
record performance. At the first training session the RA will
provide face-to-face support to the patient participant in using
the computer program.
An automatic emailing system will also be set up by the RA to
send participant's training data automatically at the completion
of each session so that the participant’s involvement in the
restorative memory training program can be monitored. In
addition, the RA will make contact with participants by
telephone twice a week to offer support and monitor progress.
The RA will also be accessible to participants by telephone four
days a week, during business hours to answer queries or resolve
issues relating to the program. Face-to-face support will be
arranged where needed. After six weeks of using the memory
training program the RA will arrange for the USB to be
returned.
1 23
Psychological Research
An International Journal of Perception,
Attention, Memory, and Action
ISSN 0340-0727
Psychological Research
DOI 10.1007/s00426-014-0566-4
Prospective memory training in older
adults and its relevance for successful aging
Alexandra Hering, Peter G. Rendell,
Nathan S. Rose, Katharina
M. Schnitzspahn & Matthias Kliegel
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer-
Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. This e-offprint is
for personal use only and shall not be self-
archived in electronic repositories. If you wish
to self-archive your article, please use the
accepted manuscript version for posting on
your own website. You may further deposit
the accepted manuscript version in any
repository, provided it is only made publicly
available 12 months after official publication
or later and provided acknowledgement is
given to the original source of publication
and a link is inserted to the published article
on Springer's website. The link must be
accompanied by the following text: "The final
publication is available at link.springer.com”.
R E V I E W
Prospective memory training in older adults and its relevance
for successful aging
Alexandra Hering • Peter G. Rendell •
Nathan S. Rose • Katharina M. Schnitzspahn •
Matthias Kliegel
Received: 31 May 2013 / Accepted: 22 March 2014
� Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014
Abstract In research on cognitive plasticity, two
training approaches have been established: (1) training of
strategies to improve performance in a given task (e.g.,
encoding strategies to improve episodic memory perfor-
mance) and (2) training of basic cognitive processes
(e.g., working memory, inhibition) that underlie a range
of more complex cognitive tasks (e.g., planning) to
improve both the training target and the complex transfer
tasks. Strategy training aims to compensate or circum-
vent limitations in underlying processes, while process
training attempts to augment or to restore these pro-
cesses. Although research on both approaches has pro-
duced some promising findings, results are still
heterogeneous and the impact of most training regimes
for everyday life is unknown. We, therefore, discuss
recent proposals of training regimes aiming to improve
prospective memory (i.e., forming and realizing delayed
intentions) as this type of complex cognition is highly
relevant for independent living. Furthermore, prospective
memory is associated with working memory and exec-
utive functions and age-related decline is widely repor-
ted. We review initial evidence suggesting that both
training regimes (i.e., strategy and/or process training)
can successfully be applied to improve prospective
memory. Conceptual and methodological implications of
the findings for research on age-related prospective
memory and for training research in general are
discussed.
Introduction
Aging is associated with cognitive decline causing indi-
vidual and social burdens. Lawton et al. (1999), for
example, showed how threatening cognitive decline can
be for older adults. They asked 600 older adults how
long they would desire to live under the condition of
functional limitations (e.g., being in a bedridden state),
cognitive impairment (e.g., being confused) or pain.
Above 60 % of the participants aged 70 years and older
did not wish to live any longer under any condition of
cognitive impairment. Indeed, cognitive decline was
deemed more threatening for older adults’ quality of life
than functional impairment or pain. These results illus-
trate the importance of cognitive wellbeing and inde-
pendence in old age and reveal the need for interventions
aimed at maintaining a high level of cognitive func-
tioning as long as possible. Research following this need
faces two challenges: (1) developing effective training
interventions that protect or enhance cognitive functions;
(2) ensuring that these interventions actually transfer to
everyday life and impact quality of life in older adults.
The present review aims to discuss and connect existing
literature on cognitive training to everyday functioning in
older adults. To do so, we focus on a key everyday
memory skill, prospective memory, and its application to
cognitive training.
A. Hering (&) � K. M. Schnitzspahn � M. Kliegel
Faculté de Psychologie et Sciences de l’Education, Université
de
Genève, 40, Boulevard du Pont-d’-Arve, 1211 Geneva,
Switzerland
e-mail: [email protected]
P. G. Rendell
Australian Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia
N. S. Rose
Rotman Research Institute of Baycrest, Toronto, Canada
123
Psychological Research
DOI 10.1007/s00426-014-0566-4
Author's personal copy
Prospective memory and aging
Prospective memory describes the ability to plan and
successfully execute delayed intentions in the future (e.g.,
Einstein & McDaniel, 1990; for an overview see Kliegel,
McDaniel, & Einstein 2008). Typical prospective memory
tasks in everyday life are to take one’s medication at the
right time, to call friends on their birthday or to pay bills
on time. These examples show how important intact
prospective remembering is for an individual’s everyday
and social life. Furthermore, studies show that forgetting
planned intentions (e.g., forgetting to make a phone call)
belongs to the most frequent memory errors in everyday
life (Crovitz & Daniel, 1984; Kliegel & Martin, 2003).
Moreover, prospective memory is predictive of a wide
range of everyday activities in older adults (Woods,
Weinborn, Velnoweth, Rooney, & Bucks, 2012). Specif-
ically, Wood and colleagues found that prospective
memory deficits in older adults were associated with
problems and greater self-reported dependence in instru-
mental activities of daily living (e.g., housekeeping,
shopping). Hence, an intact prospective memory is a
crucial factor for maintaining independence and auton-
omy. It may therefore be the ideal target for cognitive
interventions in old age.
This seems to hold especially, because developmental
studies have shown that it may be a particular challenge to
keep an intact prospective memory with increasing age.
Research on aging and prospective memory reveal a gen-
eral decline with increasing age in laboratory-based studies
compared to young adults (e.g., Altgassen, Kliegel,
Brandimonte, & Filippello, 2010; Bisiacchi, Tarantino, &
Ciccola, 2008; Einstein, McDaniel, Manzi, Cochran, &
Baker, 2000; Park, Hertzog, Kidder, Morrell, & Mayhorn,
1997). Including 26 studies, Henry, MacLeod, Phillips, and
Crawford (2004) reported in their meta-analysis on pro-
spective memory a substantial age-related deficit (rs = -
0.39 to -0.34) indicating that young adults outperform old
adults in laboratory-based prospective memory tasks (see
Ihle, Hering, Mahy, Bisiacchi, & Kliegel, 2013; Kliegel,
Phillips, & Jäger, 2008 for further meta-analytic evidence
confirming those findings). Obviously, the robust age-
related decline in older adults raised the question which
underlying mechanisms could explain those findings.
While there is an ongoing debate over the precise cognitive
processes that are associated with age-related decline in
prospective memory, there is some consensus that it can
partly be linked to the age-related decline of attentional
resources and cognitive control (Craik & Byrd, 1982;
Kliegel, Ramuschkat, & Martin, 2003; Martin, Kliegel, &
McDaniel, 2003; Maylor, Smith, Della Sala, & Logie,
2002; Rose, Rendell, McDaniel, Aberle, & Kliegel, 2010).
In particular, age-related decline in working memory,
inhibition and shifting has been associated with decline in
prospective memory (Gonneaud et al., 2011; Rose et al.,
2010; Schnitzspahn, Stahl, Zeintl, Kaller, & Kliegel, 2013).
For example, Schnitzspahn et al. (2013) tested 175 young
and 110 older adults on a battery of different prospective
memory tasks and cognitive tests assessing shifting,
updating, inhibition, working memory, and speed. Aiming
to disentangle the role of different executive functions on
age-related prospective memory performance, the authors
found age-related declines in performance on both the
prospective memory and executive function measures.
Furthermore, they showed that inhibition and shifting play
an especially important role in predicting prospective
memory performance.
Taken together, prospective memory is of high rele-
vance for older adults, yet there is an age-related decline in
prospective memory performance. As a result, it is not
surprising that, recently, there has been increasing interest
in training interventions that may have the potential to
enhance prospective memory performance in older adults
and maybe even prevent age-related declines (McDaniel &
Bugg, 2012). However, so far, very little has been pub-
lished in this area. The present review, therefore, aims to
offer some insights on a possible rationale for future
research in the area of prospective memory training. In the
following, we briefly outline a distinction made in the
available literature on cognitive training in general that will
guide our rationale.
Cognitive training: general remarks and two ways to go
The approach of training cognitive resources is generally
motivated by the concept of cognitive plasticity, also
described as the malleability of behavior (Greenwood &
Parasuraman, 2010; Lövdén, Bäckman, Lindenberger,
Schaefer, & Schmiedek, 2010; Willis & Schaie, 2009). In
more detail, cognitive plasticity refers to the general
potential within a person to change as a function of expe-
rience in the sense of improvements (Baltes, 1987).
Empirical evidence suggests that cognitive plasticity is still
possible in late adulthood (e.g., Carretti, Borella, Zavagnin,
& de Beni, 2013; Karbach & Kray, 2009; Singer, Lin-
denberger, & Baltes, 2003; Verhaeghen, Marcoen, &
Goossens, 1992) and also in very late adulthood in indi-
viduals aged over 80 years (e.g., Borella, Carretti, Zanoni,
Zavagnin, & De Beni, 2013; Buschkuehl et al., 2008;
Fernandez-Ballesteros et al., 2012; Zinke, Zeintl, Eschen,
Herzog, & Kliegel, 2012; Zinke et al., 2013). Furthermore,
the capability to learn principally remains intact even in
patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s
disease (e.g., Carretti, Borella, Fostinelli, & Zavagnin,
2013; Fernandez-Ballesteros et al., 2012).
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
Considering the available literature, two main approa-
ches exist in the area of cognitive interventions: strategy-
oriented training and process-based training. Strategy
training aims to compensate or circumvent limitations in
underlying processes, while process training aims to aug-
ment or to restore the underlying processes (Reichman,
Fiocco, & Rose, 2010).
Strategy-oriented training interventions typically (but
not exclusively) train the use of a mnemonic strategy (e.g.,
rehearsal, imagery, method of loci) to enhance particular
functions as encoding or retrieval from memory in a
compensatory way (Morrison & Chein, 2011). Meta-anal-
yses by Verhaeghen et al. (1992) and Gross et al. (2012)
show substantial effect size for pre-post change scores,
indicating large benefits for the training groups compared
to control groups. However, these training gains are mostly
limited to the training task itself. Although it remains
questionable if transfer of training improvements to other
new and untrained tasks is possible (McDaniel & Bugg,
2012; Morrison & Chein, 2011), there is some promising
positive evidence (for benefits to working memory see,
e.g., Carretti, Borella, & De Beni, 2007; for benefits to
everyday activities see, e.g., Cavallini, Pagnin, & Vecchi,
2003). In any case, memory strategies have important
practical value. They are comparably easy to teach, train
and to use in everyday life. However, the scientific chal-
lenge is to find the appropriate strategy targeting the rele-
vant cognitive difficulties in older adults’ everyday life. For
example, as discussed by McDaniel and Bugg (2012),
instructing older adults to use a mnemonic strategy to learn
a list of items (e.g., grocery list) is fairly easy to do, but
might not be used in everyday life because of other com-
pensational strategies or technical devices (e.g., using
mobile phone to record a list). Furthermore, other critical
aspects that influence possible transfer need to be taken
into account, such as the role of homework, the training
material, and the transfer tasks (McDaniel & Bugg, 2012).
A possibility to extend the benefits of strategy-oriented
training could be to combine strategy use with additional
metacognitive interventions. For instance, Cavallini, Dun-
losky, Bottiroli, Hertzog, and Vecchi (2010) found greater
transfer effects in older adults when teaching them mne-
monic strategies combined with an instruction phase on
how to use these strategies for other tasks. Participants
learned two strategies: imagery and sentence generation to
learn paired associates and lists of words. One half of the
group received additional instructions on how they could
use the strategies for other tasks besides the training
including two of the transfer tasks; the other half performed
only the training. Transfer was measured for four different
tasks, two of them were mentioned as examples on how the
trained strategies could be used for them, and the other two
transfer tasks were not mentioned. One major result
showed transfer effects to a non-mentioned transfer task
(text learning), which were greater for the instruction phase
group than the training only group (see also Craik & Rose,
2012).
Process-based training regimes (sometimes called
restorative training) aim to improve a particular cognitive
ability (e.g., working memory) by repetitively exercising
the underlying core mechanisms (e.g., updating) in a cog-
nitively intensive way, typically by adaptively increasing
the difficulty based on the participant’s individual perfor-
mance level (Morrison & Chein, 2011). The rationale
behind process-based training is the assumption that the
improvement in central or core neuro-cognitive resources
should also stimulate benefits in associated cognitive
functions (e.g., planning, intelligence). Therefore, the out-
come of process-based training should result in broader
effects such as near and far transfer.
Importantly for present purposes, what is often criticized
in the training literature is the lack of everyday transfer of
such process-based training regimes (Craik & Rose, 2012;
McDaniel & Bugg, 2012). There exist a few studies tar-
geting this issue so far; however, some promising results
were found in self-reported, but not objective measures of
everyday functioning (Brehmer, Westerberg, & Bäckman,
2012; Willis et al., 2006) or in reading comprehension
(e.g., Chein & Morrison, 2010; Loosli, Buschkuehl, Perrig,
& Jaeggi, 2012). For example, Brehmer et al. (2012)
investigated the effectiveness of a computerized adaptive
working memory training compared to a control training.
The control group worked with the same task material but
practiced on a low level of difficulty throughout the
intervention. The training program was applied to younger
and older adults for 5 weeks. Besides near and far cogni-
tive transfer measures, they also included a self-rating scale
for subjective cognitive functioning in everyday life. The
training led to near and far transfer effects for both age
groups. More interestingly, the results showed that both
training groups reduced their subjective memory com-
plaints more than the active control groups and that this
benefit was even maintained across a follow-up interval of
3 months. A limitation of this study is that this everyday
transfer is restricted to only self-report improvements.
Thus, the question remains if this subjective impression
holds true on an objective performance level.
Trainability of prospective memory
Taken together, to this date, evidence of far transfer effects
as well as everyday transfer is limited, for both strategy-
and process-based training approaches. In this context,
McDaniel and Bugg (2012) suggested that this may partly
be due to a lack of theory-driven hypotheses for transfer.
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
For example, the authors criticized memory training
interventions that train older adults to perform a strategy or
task in the lab, but fail to generalize the training to
everyday memory. In their opinion, training should be
training for transfer that rests on clear conceptual predic-
tions on the possible overlap between training and transfer
tasks, particularly with respect to transfer to everyday
functioning. Specifically, they suggested that training pro-
grams should aim for a better fit between the trained
strategies or processes in the lab and their application in
everyday life. Authors argued that it is important to iden-
tify everyday problems and develop and train strategies and
processes that are realistically applicable and useful for
those problems in everyday life. As stated in the beginning
of this article, prospective memory can be seen as such a
target of cognitive interventions (c.f., Kliegel & Bürki,
2012). Also, from a more fundamental cognitive training
perspective, prospective memory may be considered an
interesting target, as it is associated to executive functions
such as task switching and working memory on the one
hand and to episodic memory demands on the other hand.
As stated earlier, available evidence shows that these
associated processes seem to be trainable. In conclusion,
this should also be the case for prospective memory.
To guide such interventions, Kliegel, Altgassen, Hering,
and Rose (2011) recently proposed a framework for a
theory-driven training approach in the area of prospective
memory. Specifically, Kliegel et al. (2011) recommended
disentangling the process of prospective remembering by
its different phases to identify the critical components of
prospective memory in need of training in a given popu-
lation. Remembering delayed intentions at the appropriate
moment in the future is conceptualized as a four-phase
process (Ellis, 1996; Kliegel, Martin, McDaniel, & Ein-
stein, 2002; Kliegel, McDaniel, & Einstein, 2000). In the
first phase—intention formation—the individual has to
formulate and plan the intention. This first phase relies
mainly on encoding and planning processes (Kliegel et al.,
2002; Kliegel et al., 2000). After successfully forming an
intention, there is the intention retention phase. Typically,
the individual performs other ‘‘ongoing’’ tasks during a
delay. This temporal delay until the initiation of the
intention can vary in its length from a few minutes (e.g., to
remember to take the pizza out of the oven) up to several
days or months (e.g., to remember to pay a monthly bill).
During this phase, the intention is maintained in long-term
memory. Important for this phase is a more or less elabo-
rate representation of the intended action (e.g., a strong
association between intention and content; Marsh, Hicks,
Cook, Hansen, & Pallos, 2003)—to facilitate later memory
retrieval; and working memory capacity (Martin & Schu-
mann-Hengsteler, 2001) since the intention may need to be
refreshed or updated in memory while being engaged in
other activities. The last two phases refer to the intention
initiation and the intention execution. The individual has to
detect the appropriate moment indicated by the prospective
memory cue (event-based prospective memory) or after an
elapsed time (time-based prospective memory) and retrieve
the intention from memory to successfully execute it.
Although these two phases are sometimes considered
together as they occur close in real time, Kliegel et al.
(2002) describe fine conceptual differences. The intention
initiation phase involves the initiation of the planned
intention at the appropriate moment. The involved under-
lying executive processes include monitoring (to detect the
appropriate situation by noticing the cue or monitoring the
time), cognitive flexibility (to switch between ongoing and
intended activity), and inhibition (to inhibit the ongoing
activity). The intention execution phase refers to the
completion of intentions and depends also on cognitive
flexibility. There are two possible outcomes: the correctly
retrieved intention could be executed correctly or an
incorrect action could be executed. Performance is influ-
enced by the previously formed plan and situational factors
that occur while performing the intended tasks. Besides the
relevance of these cognitive functions for each phase of
prospective remembering, non-cognitive factors such as
metacognitive abilities play a role in prospective memory
performance as well (e.g., Kliegel, Altgassen, Hering, &
Rose, 2011; Meeks, Hicks, & Marsh, 2007; Schnitzspahn,
Zeintl, Jaeger, & Kliegel, 2011). To which extent someone
rates his or her ability to remember intentions as high or
low might also influence which strategies and how many
cognitive resources were allocated to the execution of the
intentions. A recent study by Rummel and Meiser (2013) in
young adults showed that the allocation of attention and
monitoring in a prospective memory task was influenced
by the metacognitive beliefs about the task demands.
To conclude, successful prospective memory depends
on retrospective (episodic) memory processes (e.g.,
encoding, retrieval), as well as controlled attention/execu-
tive processes associated with planning, working memory,
and task switching. Given this theoretical process-based
framework, a more tailor-made approach could be applied
to the development of training programs where different
training aspects fit to the different cognitive requirements
that underlie prospective remembering (Kliegel & Bürki,
2012). But what is actually known about the malleability of
prospective memory?
So far, there is only a small literature on prospective
memory training (Fleming, Shum, Strong, & Lightbody,
2005; Kinsella et al., 2009; Radford, Lah, Thayer, Say, &
Miller, 2012; Raskin & Sohlberg, 1996; Shum, Fleming,
Gill, Gullo, & Strong, 2011), which focuses on clinical
samples only [e.g., traumatic brain injury (TBI), mild
cognitive impairment] and implements interventions more
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
in a neuropsychological rehabilitation setting (for a reviews
see Fish, Wilson, & Manly, 2010; Piras, Borella, Incoccia,
& Carlesimo, 2011). While most of the studies investigate
small sample groups or single cases, a study by Shum et al.
(2011) used a more systematic approach. The authors
compared four different training regimes. TBI patients
(aged between 18 and 60 years) participated in each of the
four training groups. All training regimes consisted of two
parts. The first part of the training comprised either 2
weeks of self-awareness training for prospective memory
or 2 weeks of a control condition (for details see below).
The second part of the training consisted of either 6 weeks
of a so-called compensatory prospective memory training
or 6 weeks of a so-called remedial prospective memory
training. Combining the two versions of the first part with
the two versions of the second part formed the four training
groups (i.e., self-awareness ? compensatory training;
control ? compensatory training; self-awareness ? reme-
dial training; control ? remedial training). The self-
awareness training introduced the concept of prospective
memory and exercised self-prediction and monitoring of
performance on various prospective memory tasks. The
corresponding control training contained the discussion of
autobiographical memories and life events as well as
education about attention and concentration. The com-
pensatory prospective memory training aimed to train
strategies to compensate for prospective memory problems
in daily life such as training of note-taking and maximizing
the use of diaries and organizational devices. The remedial
training aimed to restore lost functions. In the case of
prospective memory, training involved increasing the time
delay for remembering to perform the prospective memory
tasks continuously. First, participants’ initial prospective
memory capacity (i.e., the longest time period for which
they could successfully remember the given intentions)
was assessed. During the training, the time delay between
instruction of the prospective memory task and the
appropriate moment to execute the task was increased
always by 1 min for the next task if they performed the task
successfully. Outcome measures for all training groups
were performance on a standardized prospective memory
test, a questionnaire to assess the frequency of prospective
memory lapses in everyday life, and a rating of everyday
functioning by a relative of the training participant. These
measures were assessed before and after the training per-
iod. Results showed that only both compensatory training
groups profited from the training showing larger change
scores for the standardized prospective memory test. In
contrast, there were found no training effects for the
remedial training. Furthermore, the groups with the self-
awareness training did not differ concerning their training
gains from those without self-awareness training. There
were neither effects on the questionnaire nor the everyday
functioning ratings. Taken together, results are promising,
as they show that patients can benefit from compensatory
prospective memory training. However, the studies repor-
ted so far did not include far transfer measures or examine
healthy individuals. Further research is clearly needed to
examine training effects in healthy older adults using big-
ger samples and to investigate prospective memory inter-
ventions from a more theory-driven perspective. In the
following sections, we present some evidence from our
own work examining the possibilities for prospective
memory training following the distinction of strategy and
process-based training outlined above.
Strategy-oriented prospective memory training
In the following section, we will discuss external and
internal strategy trainings on prospective memory. External
strategy trainings comprise the use of external memory
aids such as clocks and phones. Several studies used
technological devices as external reminders to overcome
problems with self-initiated retrieval (e.g., setting an alarm
for an appointment). As already indicated in the study by
Shum et al. (2011) and also investigated in other studies in
rehabilitation settings, electronic devices indeed supported
successfully remembering delayed intentions (for mobile
phones and smartphones e.g., DePompei et al., 2008;
Thöne-Otto & Walther, 2003; Wade & Troy, 2001; for
pager e.g., Wilson, Emslie, Quirk, & Evans, 2001; Wilson,
Evans, Emslie, & Malinek, 1997). For example, Thöne-
Otto and Walther (2003) compared the efficacy of two
electronic devices, mobile phone and palm organizer in 12
brain injured patients. First, participants received some
naturalistic, experimenter-given tasks to perform without
an electronic device and they should send back a list with
all everyday intentions they forgot to the experimenter. In
this first phase, participants learned also, how they could
use one of the electronic devices. In the second phase, the
experimenter gave some naturalistic prospective memory
tasks that were already entered in the device and partici-
pants could enter their everyday intentions themselves.
Again, they had to send back the list of forgotten inten-
tions. Phase 3 and 4 repeated this procedure but for the
second device. Results revealed that participants forgot
fewer intentions when using the electronic devices and this
was especially pronounced for their self-entered everyday
tasks. Although electronic devices do not improve pro-
spective memory capacity per se, they help to keep or
regain a sufficient level of prospective memory perfor-
mance from an outcome perspective.
Considering the process-model of prospective memory,
internal strategies should play a beneficial role, especially
for the first phases (intention formation, intention
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
retention), which rely on planning abilities, encoding and
memory storage. There is some empirical evidence from
cross-sectional designs comparing the effectiveness of
different planning and encoding strategies on later pro-
spective memory performance in young and older adults
(Brom et al., 2013; Henry, Rendell, Phillips, Dunlop, &
Kliegel, 2012; Kliegel, Martin, McDaniel, Einstein, &
Moor, 2007; Liu & Park, 2004; Park, Gutchess, Meade, &
Stine-Morrow, 2007).
Kliegel et al. (2007) found that older adults’ prospective
memory performance benefited from different planning
aids (e.g., structuring of plans), which facilitated intention
initiation. The authors used the modified six-elements task
as a complex prospective memory task. The task is a
delayed multi-tasking paradigm where participants have to
plan the order how they intend to initiate and execute the
six delayed tasks while respecting the time limit and other
constraining rules concerning the order of the subtasks
(Kliegel et al., 2002; Kliegel et al., 2000). To be successful
in the task, it is necessary to plan how to proceed, to initiate
the tasks appropriately, and to switch between the tasks
according to the rules. First, participants received the
instruction and had to plan how to perform the six subtasks,
which was followed by a delay period with filler tasks.
Finally, participants had to self-initiate the execution of the
modified six-elements task. Studies using this paradigm
show that older adults tend to make less detailed plans
compared to young adults and that a less detailed plan is
associated with impairment of later realization of delayed
intentions (Kliegel et al., 2002; Kliegel et al., 2000).
To overcome this performance deficit in older adults,
Kliegel et al. (2007) provided different planning aids to
foster different aspects of the planning process of the task
in an attempt to improve later prospective memory per-
formance. The authors investigated young and old adults
and compared the effectiveness of planning aids with a no-
aid condition in both age groups. The aids helped partici-
pants to plan in three ways. First, participants were asked to
include information about the appropriate moment to ini-
tially start the task (self-initiation of intention). Second,
participants were asked to include specific cues in their
plans that described when and how they wanted to switch
between tasks (task switching). Third, a general planning
aid helped participants structure and schedule the tasks
appropriately (planning). To do so, participants received a
flow chart to help plan the order of the six subtasks in
advance. In the first two experiments of the study, partic-
ipants received either a combination of the first and second
planning aid or no planning aid. The results showed that
participants’ prospective memory performance benefited
from planning aids. Both age groups in the planning aid
condition were more likely to initiate the six-elements task
at the appropriate moment compared to participants in the
no-aid condition. Furthermore, older adults also profited
from the planning aid that benefited their task switching
between the subtasks. In the third experiment, the authors
compared the effect of three conditions in young and old
adults: no planning aid versus the general planning aid
versus a combined version of the general and the switching
aid. Results showed no influence on self-initiation of
intentions (1) but the combined aid of structuring the plans
(3) and encouraging task switching (2) improved realiza-
tion of delayed intentions with a greater effect for old
adults than young adults.
A recent study on the influence of reminders on pro-
spective memory performance also shows the benefits of
compensatory approaches for older and younger adults
(Henry et al., 2012). Henry et al. (2012) found that pro-
spective memory performance of both age groups could be
enhanced using reminders either self-initiated or presented
by the experimenter. In sum, the findings show that already
simpler internal strategies, such as basic planning strategies
or provision of reminders, could be a useful approach to
foster prospective memory.
A more complex strategy successfully used in prospec-
tive memory research is implementation intentions (e.g.,
Chasteen, Park, & Schwarz, 2001; Liu & Park, 2004;
McFarland & Glisky, 2011; Park et al., 2007; Rummel,
Einstein, & Rampey, 2012; Schnitzspahn & Kliegel, 2009;
Zimmermann & Meier, 2010). The implementation inten-
tions strategy is a goal-directed verbalization of intentions
in a ‘‘If x arises, then I will perform y’’ manner (Gollwitzer,
1993, 1999). It is assumed that formulating implementation
intentions creates a strong connection between the situation
in the ‘‘if’’-part and the intended action formulated in the
‘‘then’’-part. When the specific situation is encountered, the
planned action is automatically activated and, therefore,
more easily retrievable. When applied to prospective
memory, there is promising evidence showing that older
adults’ prospective memory benefits from formulating
implementation intentions (e.g., Chasteen et al., 2001;
Schnitzspahn & Kliegel, 2009; Zimmermann & Meier,
2010). Interestingly, Zimmermann and Meier (2010) found
the beneficial effect of implementation intentions only for
the older adults’ group but not in younger adults or ado-
lescents. Thus, implementation intentions seem to be
especially useful in older adults but the explaining mech-
anisms are still under debate. Two hypotheses are currently
discussed. One assumption is that implementation inten-
tions should improve the encoding of prospective inten-
tions, thereby reducing the need for strategic monitoring.
As a result, more automatic detection of the prospective
memory cue as well as a more spontaneous retrieval of the
intention should occur (Gollwitzer, 1999; McDaniel &
Scullin, 2010; Rummel et al., 2012). Alternatively, it has
also been argued that implementation intentions strengthen
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
the perceived importance of the intention and foster more
monitoring processes at the costs of the ongoing task
(Meeks & Marsh, 2010).
We recently conducted a study investigating the effect
of implementation intentions in performing health behav-
iors in older adults (Brom et al., 2013). The aims of the
study were to investigate the effectiveness of implemen-
tation intentions in a real-life prospective memory task and
to examine possible moderators (e.g., fluid intelligence,
conscientiousness) of the effectiveness of the strategy. The
study follows up on results by Liu and Park (2004) who
also examined the beneficial influence of implementation
intentions on prospective memory in health behavior. Liu
and Park used implementation intentions to enhance glu-
cose monitoring but in a small sample of older participants
(n = 10). We decided to use an alternative everyday health
task that could be of importance for older adults: remem-
bering to measure one’s blood pressure at three times per
day for five consecutive days of a week. The treatment
consisted of one session where participants were first
instructed on how to use a blood pressure monitor and then
they were trained to formulate the implementation inten-
tion about how they were going to control their blood
pressure. Furthermore, standardized tests and question-
naires were used to assess cognitive resources (e.g., fluid
intelligence) as well as other possible environmental (e.g.,
everyday stress) and motivational moderators (e.g., self-
efficacy).
In total, 39 healthy older adults with a mean age of
68.8 years (SD = 4.99) who had no experience with
monitoring blood pressure were included in the study.
Participants were randomly assigned to two treatment
groups: implementation intention instruction (n = 19) and
a control instruction (n = 20). The groups did not differ in
age or intelligence. Participants in the implementation
intention condition were asked to think of specific times
when they could perform the blood pressure monitoring,
where it will take place, and how they will do it. They had
to write down all this information on how to perform the
blood pressure monitoring during the next 5 days. In a
second step, participants transformed this information
through the use of the implementation intentions strategy
using prepared worksheets (e.g., ‘‘If I am in the living room
next week from Monday to Friday at 8 a.m., 12 p.m. and
6 p.m., then I will check my blood pressure with the blood
pressure monitor’’). In a last step, the participants were
asked to mentally visualize how they would enact their
intentions. Participants in the control group were asked to
write down the times when they wanted to check their
blood pressure and read an article about blood pressure to
ensure equal treatment durations.
The dependent variable was the rate of forgotten blood
pressure tests. Comparing the implementation intention
group with the control group, a significant main effect of
treatment of large effect size emerged. Participants using
implementation intentions forgot to check their blood
pressure at the intended times considerably less often than
participants of the control group. In fact, participants in the
implementation intentions training group performed almost
perfectly. Furthermore, and extending previous work by
Liu and Park (2004), there was a significant interaction
between the treatment group and level of fluid intelligence
(based on a median split in high and low fluid intelligence).
Control participants with a lower level of fluid intelligence
forgot more often to check their blood pressure than par-
ticipants with higher fluid intelligence; however, for the
implementation intention group, there was no difference
between those with low or high fluid intelligence.
These results provide an example that older adults’
prospective memory can benefit from a strategy interven-
tion. More precisely, the moderation effect indicates that
implementation intentions benefited prospective remem-
bering by compensating for low fluid intelligence. Fur-
thermore, the study shows the effectiveness of a strategy
intervention for an everyday health behavior that is highly
relevant for many older adults. However, one limitation of
the study is that maintenance effects were not assessed. It
would be of interest to know, if participants applied the
learned strategy also on other everyday behavior. In terms
of future directions, it should be acknowledged that,
although the implementation intention effect in the study
by Brom et al. (2013) was strong, the strategy was very
specific to the task that should benefit from it. It would be
of conceptual and applied interest to promote prospective
memory also in a more general way and to test transfer to
other prospective memory tasks as well as related cognitive
abilities. In the next section, we give an example for a
process-based approach to train prospective memory that
investigated the possibility of transfer from prospective
memory training.
Process-based prospective memory training
The training described below (Rose et al., 2012) was based
on the Virtual Week paradigm (Rendell & Craik, 2000).
Virtual Week is a computerized laboratory prospective
memory task in the style of a board game but with
everyday life content to increase ecological validity. Par-
ticipants roll a dice and move a token around a board that
symbolizes a virtual day from 7 a.m. to 10 p.m. During this
fictitious day, participants pass several event squares that
represent different everyday events (e.g., breakfast, shop-
ping, going to the library, dinner). For each of these events,
participants have to make choices about how the event
would end and what they would do in real life (e.g.,
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
deciding what to eat for breakfast or deciding what to buy
in the shopping mall). Two clocks are displayed in the
middle of the board, one indicating the virtual day time
(calibrated to the token position on the board), the other
displaying a stop clock that measures the actual play time
in minutes and seconds.
The prospective memory tasks are embedded in the
story of the game. At the beginning, participants learn
different health tasks, which they have to remember to
simulate performing over the course of the whole game.
For example, participants have to remember to take anti-
biotics each virtual day at breakfast and dinner events or
they have to remember to use an asthma inhaler at 11 a.m.
and 9 p.m. These health-related tasks have to be performed
regularly—that is, on every virtual day (i.e., one circuit of
the board). There are also irregular prospective memory
tasks that are instructed for the current virtual day (e.g.,
pick up dry cleaning at the shopping mall, return book at
the library, call the restaurant for a dinner reservation at
5 p.m.). These tasks have to be encoded at the beginning of
each new virtual day and during the day. The third type of
tasks are stop-clock tasks, in which participants have to
perform a certain task after a specific time has elapsed, for
example, testing blood sugar when the stop clock reaches 2
and 4 min. Participants are not obliged to perform the
regular, irregular and stop-clock prospective memory tasks
in real life. They are to simulate performing the task at the
appropriate moments, either time-cued or event-cued, by
clicking a ‘‘perform task’’ button and selecting the right
task from a list. In the original version by Rendell and
Craik (2000), participants had to perform 10 tasks per
virtual day for 7 virtual days indicating Monday to Sunday.
Previous research has demonstrated that Virtual Week is
a useful and reliable tool for prospective memory research
with good psychometric properties (Rendell & Henry,
2009). The different task types vary in their demands
requiring automatic or strategic processing and working
memory abilities (McDaniel & Einstein, 2000; Rose et al.,
2010). The distinction between regular and irregular tasks
reflects low versus high retrospective memory demands
(Foster, Rose, McDaniel, & Rendell, 2013; Mioni, Rendell,
Henry, Cantagallo, & Stablum, 2013). Differences based
on the cue of the prospective memory task (event-based,
time-based and stop clock) result in varying demands on
monitoring processes, with time-based and stop-clock tasks
being more demanding as they require checking the clocks,
whereas event-based tasks require attending to the events
which pop up when the participant passes an event-square
(Rendell & Henry, 2009; Rose et al., 2010). Furthermore,
Rose et al. (2010) showed that individual differences in
working memory were predictive of prospective memory
performance in the Virtual Week game, especially for
high-demanding tasks. Moreover, Virtual Week is an age-
sensitive task: Research on aging shows that older adults
perform worse compared to young adults for the irregular
tasks compared to regular tasks (Aberle, Rendell, Rose,
McDaniel, & Kliegel, 2010; Rendell & Craik, 2000; Rose
et al., 2010) and also for time-based tasks compared to
event-based tasks (e.g., Henry et al., 2012; Rendell et al.,
2011).
In the training protocol, we used the original Virtual
Week paradigm and extended it to a process-based training
version to examine whether older adults’ prospective
memory could be improved and if training gains could
transfer to other cognitive and everyday life tasks (Rose
et al., 2012). The study consisted of three experimental
groups, one group performed the Virtual Week training; the
second group, an active control group, performed a music
training; the third group was a waitlist, no-contact control
group. In total, 50 older adults between 60 and 80 years
participated in the study.
All participants performed a pre- and post-testing ses-
sion using a battery of established prospective memory
measures, neuropsychological tasks and everyday compe-
tence tests. We also developed and assessed performance
on a novel real-life prospective memory task—the ‘‘call-
back task’’ (Rose et al., 2012). While participants were at
home engaged in their daily activities, they received a
phone call from an experimenter and they were to call the
experimenter back in exactly 10, 25, 35 and 40 min. For
everyday competence, we administered the timed version
of instrumental activities of daily living (Owsley, Sloane,
McGwin, & Ball, 2002). In this standard measure of
instrumental activities of daily living, participants had to
perform short everyday tasks as fast as possible (e.g.,
counting money, reading out loud ingredients, finding
telephone numbers in a phone book). Performance is typ-
ically used to assess one‘s fitness for independent living.
Between the pre- and post-test there was a delay of 4
weeks. Participants in the no-contact control group per-
formed only the pre- and the post-testing. Participants in
the active control group received fifteen 1-h music lessons
over 4 weeks. Subjects in the Virtual Week training group
participated in twelve 1-h training sessions, three times per
week for 4 weeks on the prospective memory training. The
Virtual Week training consisted of 24 different levels. One
level corresponded to one virtual day that was one circuit
of the board. To implement a process-based training
design, the levels increased in difficulty in an adaptive
manner. We varied the number of tasks to train the retro-
spective component. The prospective component was also
trained by changing the cues across the training and dis-
playing visible but also hidden stop clocks for the stop-
clock tasks to put increasing demands on cue detection,
monitoring, and cognitive flexibility. Participants had to
play two virtual days per training session. After each level,
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
they received feedback about their performance. Partici-
pants saw whether they performed each task correctly or
not. They had to achieve at least 75 % correct to pass the
level or else they had to repeat and practice the day with
the same events. However, the game routine differed
depending on the rolling of the dice. After each week of
training participants filled in a questionnaire on how they
performed the different types of tasks during the game.
Over the course of the training, participants increased
their performance while the numbers of day repetitions
decreased. Pre-post comparisons indicated a training ben-
efit for the prospective memory training group in the ori-
ginal Virtual Week task compared to the control groups.
Likewise, participants in the Virtual Week training group
tended to show greater improvement in the real-life pro-
spective memory task, the call-back task, after the training
compared to both control groups, hinting at some near
transfer to performing prospective memory tasks in the real
world. Furthermore, participants in the Virtual Week
training group showed a greater pre- to post-test
improvement in the timed instrumental activities of daily
living after the training compared to both control groups,
indicating far transfer. Qualitative analyses of the post-
training questionnaires further indicated that participants
used either no or poor strategies (e.g., ‘‘concentrate’’) at the
beginning of the training, but some improved to using
better strategies (e.g., ‘‘visualize’’) over the course of the
training. Taken together, Rose et al. (2012) showed that
older adults could improve their prospective memory per-
formance on the Virtual Week paradigm relative to con-
trols. Furthermore, there were some trends for near and far
transfer to everyday naturalistic tasks relative to controls.
Prospective memory training among cognitive training
interventions
The examples discussed above show that there is evidence
that prospective memory is a suitable candidate for both
strategy-oriented and process-based training approaches.
However, prospective memory is also a construct that
breaks ranks among the other target cognitive abilities in
previous training research. The focus of process-based
training in the literature lays especially on working mem-
ory. The typical training of working memory targets basic
cognitive processes constituting working memory by very
specific tasks. For example, participants have to perform
visuospatial span tasks or n-back tasks with increasing
difficulty in a repetitive way (e.g., Jaeggi, Buschkuehl,
Jonides, & Perrig, 2008; Klingberg, Forssberg, & Wester-
berg, 2002). Also, verbal working memory trainings
showed promising effects (Borella, Carretti, Riboldi, & De
Beni, 2010; Borella et al., 2013; Carretti, Borella,
Fostinelli, & Zavagnin, 2013). The question resulting from
this approach is if it is really possible to bridge the gap
between the highly specific tasks used in training inter-
ventions to improvements in everyday life for older adults
(e.g., making financial decisions, health behavior, house-
keeping). If cognitive training is intended to improve older
adults’ cognition and increase their independence and
wellbeing, then this should be the primary aim. In our
opinion, training of prospective memory could actually
connect the applied perspective of an attempt to improve
everyday life functioning in older adults with the basic
research on plasticity and its process-based approach. Of
course, this is an empirical question and future research
will have to systematically test this proposition.
However, so far, only a few studies have examined the
trainability of prospective memory. We discussed exam-
ples for both training approaches that showed encouraging
results for this line of research. Taken together, we dem-
onstrated the effectiveness of a strategy-oriented approach
to improve prospective memory in everyday life (Brom
et al., 2013). The second example showed the possibility to
train prospective memory with a process-based training
regime (Rose et al., 2012). This initial evidence leads to
further important research questions: how can we maxi-
mize benefits of prospective memory training and how can
we ensure robust transfer to the daily requirements of older
adults? As discussed previously, each of the two training
approaches has its strengths and weaknesses. For training
prospective memory, one possibility might be to combine
both approaches and perhaps profit from the advantages of
each approach and to overcome some of their limitations.
For example, in future studies both trainings could be
combined. The strategy of implementation intentions could
be implemented in the Virtual Week training as well. The
training of implementation intentions on the realistic con-
tent of Virtual Week could stimulate transfer to everyday
tasks of the participants. On the other hand, the adaptive
training of prospective memory abilities could be boostered
by the strategy. Another route for future research may be to
consider the benefits of strategies that help ‘‘train for
transfer’’. For example, if we aim to improve older adults’
everyday functioning and wellbeing (Craik & Rose, 2012),
training the use of strategies for varying stimuli and situ-
ations over the course of the intervention could help
achieve transfer effects of the training regimes applied.
Another crucial point within this discussion is the aim for
long-term effects. When improving cognitive functioning
by training interventions, the second aim, besides reliable
transfer, should be the maintenance of these effects (e.g.,
Borella et al., 2013; Brehmer et al., 2012; Li et al., 2008;
Zinke et al., 2013). For instance, Zinke et al. (2013) found
that their working memory training for older adults pro-
duced stable training effects as well as near transfer effects
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
at a 9-month follow-up assessment. However, the results of
Melby-Lervag and Hulme (2013) were more sobering. In
their meta-analysis, they found long-term effects for
working memory training on visual working memory tasks,
but not on verbal tasks or far transfer measures (e.g., word
reading, attention, reasoning). One main problem of eval-
uating long-term effects rests on the fact that the majority
of training studies did not include follow-up assessments.
Further training research is clearly needed to determine
factors responsible for maintenance effects. Moreover, a
limitation of the meta-analysis for the present purpose is,
that only a few training studies on older adults were
included.
The characterization of prospective memory as a multi-
phase process (Kliegel et al., 2002; Kliegel et al., 2000)
provides several different targets for combined cognitive
interventions. Compensational strategies like implementa-
tion intentions are especially helpful for encoding inten-
tions and should, therefore, be especially helpful when
deficits are likely to be due to the intention formation
phase (e.g., such as in Parkinson patients, see Kliegel
et al., 2011). In terms of transfer, implementation inten-
tions seem to be fruitful as they are a rather general
strategy with a broad range of possible applications com-
pared to the typical mnemonic strategies that tend to be
more specific to retrieval processes (e.g., method of loci).
Complementary, traditional process-based training regimes
may be especially indicated when the improvement of
cognitive components underlying prospective memory is
the key target of the intervention. For example, training of
working memory or inhibition should reduce deficit levels
in these resources (training improvement) and subse-
quently drive improvements in those prospective memory
phases where they are most involved (i.e., intention initi-
ation and execution; far transfer). Finally, training all
phases of the prospective memory process at once with a
tool such as the Virtual Week game actually provides a
learning environment to apply the principles of training,
stabilize training gains, and possibly stimulate transfer to
other cognitive and everyday life functions. In addition,
prospective memory could also be a suitable candidate for
a multi-domain training approach. Multi-domain training
targets several cognitive domains as an ensemble, for
example, simultaneously training memory, reasoning, and
attention in one training regime (e.g., Cheng et al., 2012;
Oswald, Gunzelmann, Rupprecht, & Hagen, 2006).
Whereas single-domain training targets one cognitive
function, such as working memory or set shifting. In fact,
prospective memory is itself a multi-phase, multi-domain
process that is closely related to executive functions and
memory. For instance, prospective memory tasks by defi-
nition target a full set of functions, such as retrospective
memory (for the intention content), monitoring (for the
cues) or inhibition (of the ongoing task before initiating
the prospective memory task). For example, in a possible
training, the episodic memory component could be adap-
tively increased in difficulty by systemically increasing
number, length and/or complexity of the to-be-executed
intention and at the same time the difficulty to find the cue
and the inhibitory demands of interrupting the ongoing
task may be adapted by more and more hiding the cue in
the ongoing task and increasing its task demands, respec-
tively. Therefore, using prospective memory tasks as
training targets could prove to be a fruitful variant of
recent suggestions to use multi-domain training interven-
tions in contrast to single-domain regimes (see also Insel,
Einstein, Morrow, & Hepworth, 2013, for a similar pro-
posal; for related conceptualizations in skill acquisition see
the review by Schmidt & Bjork, 1992). Cheng et al. (2012)
found that both training approaches, single- and multi-
domain training, showed immediate training gains. Fur-
thermore, they demonstrated that multi-domain training
especially has advantages for maintenance effects of
training. Although they included booster sessions after
6 months for both trainings, Cheng et al. showed better
long-term effects up to 12 months for multi-domain
training in healthy older adults.
To conclude on the presented literature, a tailor-made
approach of a holistic prospective memory training com-
bining specific strategy- and process-based components
could be of interest for further research. The combination
of these two general training traditions in prospective
memory training could provide the possibility to not only
target one but most of the corresponding processes of
prospective memory as an ensemble. Therefore, future
studies running this combined approach could be promising
attempts to ensure improvements of functioning and well-
being in older adults’ everyday life through prospective
memory training.
Acknowledgments The preparation of this article was supported
by
a grant by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG).
References
Aberle, I., Rendell, P. G., Rose, N. S., McDaniel, M. A., &
Kliegel,
M. (2010). The age prospective memory paradox: young adults
may not give their best outside of the lab. Developmental
Psychology, 46(6), 1444–1453. doi:10.1037/A0020718.
Altgassen, M., Kliegel, M., Brandimonte, M., & Filippello, P.
(2010).
Are older adults more social than younger adults? social
importance increases older adults’ prospective memory perfor-
mance. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 17(3), 312–328.
doi:10.1080/13825580903281308.
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span
develop-
mental-psychology—on the dynamics between growth and
decline. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 611–626. doi:10.
1037/0012-1649.23.5.611.
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0020718
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13825580903281308
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.611
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.611
Bisiacchi, P. S., Tarantino, V., & Ciccola, A. (2008). Aging and
prospective memory: the role of working memory and monitor-
ing processes. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research,
20(6),
569–577.
Borella, E., Carretti, B., Riboldi, F., & De Beni, R. (2010).
Working
memory training in older adults evidence of transfer and
maintenance effects. Psychology and Aging, 25(4), 767–778.
doi:10.1037/A0020683.
Borella, E., Carretti, B., Zanoni, G., Zavagnin, M., & De Beni,
R.
(2013). Working memory training in old age: an examination of
transfer and maintenance effects. Archives of Clinical Neuro-
psychology, 28(4), 331–347. doi:10.1093/Arclin/Act020.
Brehmer, Y., Westerberg, H., & Bäckman, L. (2012). Working-
memory training in younger and older adults: training gains,
transfer, and maintenance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
6:63. doi:10.3389/Fnhum.2012.00063.
Brom, S. S., Schnitzspahn, K. M., Melzer, M., Hagner, F.,
Bernhard,
A., & Kliegel, M. (2013). Fluid mechanics moderate the effect
of
implementation intentions on a health prospective memory task
in older adults. European Journal of Ageing, 19, 1–10. doi:10.
1007/s10433-013-0288-2.
Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., Hutchison, S., Perrig-Chiello,
P.,
Däpp, C., Müller, M., et al. (2008). Impact of working memory
training on memory performance in old–old adults. Psychology
and Aging, 23(4), 743–753. doi:10.1037/A0014342.
Carretti, B., Borella, E., & De Beni, R. (2007). Does strategic
memory
training improve the working memory performance of younger
and older adults? Experimental Psychology, 54(4), 311–320.
doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.311.
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Fostinelli, S., & Zavagnin, M. (2013a).
Benefits of training working memory in amnestic mild cognitive
impairment: specific and transfer effects. International Psych-
ogeriatrics, 25(4), 617–626. doi:10.1017/S1041610212002177.
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Zavagnin, M., & de Beni, R. (2013b).
Gains
in language comprehension relating to working memory training
in healthy older adults. International Journal of Geriatric
Psychiatry, 28(5), 539–546. doi:10.1002/gps.3859.
Cavallini, E., Dunlosky, J., Bottiroli, S., Hertzog, C., & Vecchi,
T.
(2010). Promoting transfer in memory training for older adults.
Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 22(4), 314–323.
doi:10.3275/6704.
Cavallini, E., Pagnin, A., & Vecchi, T. (2003). Aging and
everyday
memory: the beneficial effect of memory training. Archives of
Gerontology and Geriatrics, 37(3), 241–257. doi:10.1016/
S0167-4943(03)00063-3.
Chasteen, A. L., Park, D. C., & Schwarz, N. (2001).
Implementation
intentions and facilitation of prospective memory.
Psychological
Science, 12(6), 457–461. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00385.
Chein, J. M., & Morrison, A. B. (2010). Expanding the mind’s
workspace: training and transfer effects with a complex working
memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(2),
193–199. doi:10.3758/Pbr.17.2.193.
Cheng, Y., Wu, W. Y., Feng, W., Wang, J. Q., Chen, Y., Shen,
Y.,
et al. (2012). The effects of multi-domain versus single-domain
cognitive training in non-demented older people: a randomized
controlled trial. BMC Medicine, 10:30. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-
10-30.
Craik, F. I. M., & Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive
deficits: the role
of attentional resources. In F. I. M. Craik & S. E. Trehub (Eds.),
Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 191–211). New York:
Plenum.
Craik, F. I. M., & Rose, N. S. (2012). Training cognition:
parallels
with physical fitness? Journal of Applied Research in Memory
and Cognition, 1(1), 51–52. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2011.12.001.
Crovitz, H. F., & Daniel, W. F. (1984). Measurements of
everyday
memory—toward the prevention of forgetting. Bulletin of the
Psychonomic Society, 22(5), 413–414.
DePompei, R., Gillette, Y., Goetz, E., Xenopoulos-Oddsson, A.,
Bryen, D., & Dowds, M. (2008). Practical applications for use
of
PDAs and smartphones with children and adolescents who have
traumatic brain injury. Neurorehabilitation, 23(6), 487–499.
Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and
prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology-
Learning Memory and Cognition, 16(4), 717–726. doi:10.1037/
0278-7393.16.4.717.
Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Manzi, M., Cochran, B., &
Baker,
M. (2000). Prospective memory and aging: forgetting intentions
over short delays. Psychology and Aging, 15(4), 671–683.
doi:10.1037//0882-7974.15.4.671.
Ellis, J. (1996). Prospective memory or the realization of
delayed
intentions: A conceptual framework for research. In M.
A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.),
Prospective memory: theory and applications (pp. 1–22).
Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Fernandez-Ballesteros, R., Botella, J., Zamarron, M. D.,
Molina, M.
A., Cabras, E., Schettini, R., et al. (2012). Cognitive plasticity
in
normal and pathological aging. Clinical Interventions in Aging,
7, 15–25. doi:10.2147/Cia.S27008.
Fish, J., Wilson, B. A., & Manly, T. (2010). The assessment and
rehabilitation of prospective memory problems in people with
neurological disorders: a review. Neuropsychological Rehabili-
tation, 20(2), 161–179. doi:10.1080/09602010903126029.
Fleming, J. M., Shum, D., Strong, J., & Lightbody, S. (2005).
Prospective memory rehabilitation for adults with traumatic
brain injury: a compensatory training programme. Brain Injury,
19(1), 1–10. doi:10.1080/02699050410001720059.
Foster, E. R., Rose, N. S., McDaniel, M. A., & Rendell, P. G.
(2013).
Prospective memory in Parkinson disease during a virtual week:
effects of both prospective and retrospective demands. Neuro-
psychology, 27(2), 170–181. doi:10.1037/A0031946.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: the role of
intentions.
European Review of Social Psychology, 4(1), 141–185. doi:10.
1080/14792779343000059.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions—strong
effects
of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493–503. doi:10.
1037//0003-066x.54.7.493.
Gonneaud, J., Kalpouzos, G., Bon, L., Viader, F., Eustache, F.,
&
Desgranges, B. (2011). Distinct and shared cognitive functions
mediate event- and time-based prospective memory impairment
in normal ageing. Memory, 19(4), 360–377. doi:10.1080/
09658211.2011.570765.
Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (2010). Neuronal and
cognitive plasticity: a neurocognitive framework for ameliorat-
ing cognitive aging. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2:150.
doi:10.3389/fnagi.2010.00150.
Gross, A. L., Parisi, J. M., Spira, A. P., Kueider, A. M., Ko, J.
Y.,
Saczynski, J. S., et al. (2012). Memory training interventions
for
older adults: a meta-analysis. Aging & Mental Health, 16(6),
722–734. doi:10.1080/13607863.2012.667783.
Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & Crawford, J. R.
(2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and
aging. Psychology and Aging, 19(1), 27–39. doi:10.1037/0882-
7974.19.1.27.
Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., Phillips, L. H., Dunlop, L., &
Kliegel, M.
(2012). Prospective memory reminders: a laboratory investiga-
tion of initiation source and age effects. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 65(7), 1274–1287. doi:10.1080/
17470218.2011.651091.
Ihle, A., Hering, A., Mahy, C. E. V., Bisiacchi, P. S., & Kliegel,
M.
(2013). Adult age differences, response management, and cue
focality in event-based prospective memory: a meta-analysis on
the role of task order specificity. Psychology and Aging, 28(3),
714–720. doi:10.1037/A0033653.
Psychological Research
123
Author's personal copy
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0020683
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/Arclin/Act020
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/Fnhum.2012.00063
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-013-0288-2
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-013-0288-2
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0014342
http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.311
http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1041610212002177
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/gps.3859
http://dx.doi.org/10.3275/6704
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4943(03)00063-3
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4943(03)00063-3
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00385
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/Pbr.17.2.193
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-10-30
http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-10-30
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2011.12.001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.15.4.671
http://dx.doi.org/10.2147/Cia.S27008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602010903126029
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699050410001720059
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0031946
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14792779343000059
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14792779343000059
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.54.7.493
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.54.7.493
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.570765
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.570765
http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/Fnagi.2010.00150
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2012.667783
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.27
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.27
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.651091
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.651091
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0033653
Insel, K. C., Einstein, G. O., Morrow, D. G., & Hepworth, J. T.
(2013). A multifaceted prospective memory intervention to
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx
Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Ähnlich wie Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx

The less adults sleep, the faster their. new group presentation
The less adults sleep, the faster their. new group presentationThe less adults sleep, the faster their. new group presentation
The less adults sleep, the faster their. new group presentationAlex Taremwa
 
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebralesMeditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebralesRAUL TAYA PEREZ
 
Walden University NURS 6050 Polic
 Walden University   NURS 6050 Polic Walden University   NURS 6050 Polic
Walden University NURS 6050 PolicMoseStaton39
 
Long term effects of cognitive training
Long term effects of cognitive trainingLong term effects of cognitive training
Long term effects of cognitive trainingMiriamTrapagaO
 
Flak2014 article the_memoryaidstudyprotocolforar
Flak2014 article the_memoryaidstudyprotocolforarFlak2014 article the_memoryaidstudyprotocolforar
Flak2014 article the_memoryaidstudyprotocolforarsonia483890
 
Launching of cognitive training in ain shams university
Launching of cognitive training in ain shams universityLaunching of cognitive training in ain shams university
Launching of cognitive training in ain shams universityHeba Tawfik
 
Why older people engage in physical activity an exploratory study of partici...
Why older people engage in physical activity  an exploratory study of partici...Why older people engage in physical activity  an exploratory study of partici...
Why older people engage in physical activity an exploratory study of partici...Darren John Capalb
 
Statistical methods for cardiovascular researchers
Statistical methods for cardiovascular researchersStatistical methods for cardiovascular researchers
Statistical methods for cardiovascular researchersRamachandra Barik
 
Mental Illness Prediction based on Speech using Supervised Learning
Mental Illness Prediction based on Speech using Supervised LearningMental Illness Prediction based on Speech using Supervised Learning
Mental Illness Prediction based on Speech using Supervised LearningIRJET Journal
 
A systematic review of interventions for children with cerebral palsy state ...
A systematic review of interventions for children with cerebral palsy  state ...A systematic review of interventions for children with cerebral palsy  state ...
A systematic review of interventions for children with cerebral palsy state ...MaradelRocioHuertaTe
 
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity Giuseppe Fattori
 
High School Start time paper
High School Start time paperHigh School Start time paper
High School Start time paperSarah Hashmi
 
LongitudinalAttrtionLitReviewNov09
LongitudinalAttrtionLitReviewNov09LongitudinalAttrtionLitReviewNov09
LongitudinalAttrtionLitReviewNov09Kathryn Ashton
 
Supporting Procedural and Perceptual Learning in Laparoscopic Surgery
Supporting Procedural and Perceptual Learning in Laparoscopic SurgerySupporting Procedural and Perceptual Learning in Laparoscopic Surgery
Supporting Procedural and Perceptual Learning in Laparoscopic SurgeryYiman Lou
 
Daytime Impairment due to College Student Technology Use during S(1)
Daytime Impairment due to College Student Technology Use during S(1)Daytime Impairment due to College Student Technology Use during S(1)
Daytime Impairment due to College Student Technology Use during S(1)Rachel Feragne
 
Bilingualism And Dementia Essay
Bilingualism And Dementia EssayBilingualism And Dementia Essay
Bilingualism And Dementia EssayKate Subramanian
 
Enrichment effects on adult
Enrichment effects on adultEnrichment effects on adult
Enrichment effects on adultMiriamTrapagaO
 

Ähnlich wie Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx (20)

The less adults sleep, the faster their. new group presentation
The less adults sleep, the faster their. new group presentationThe less adults sleep, the faster their. new group presentation
The less adults sleep, the faster their. new group presentation
 
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebralesMeditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
Meditacion ayuda a la resitencia de enfermedades cerebrales
 
Walden University NURS 6050 Polic
 Walden University   NURS 6050 Polic Walden University   NURS 6050 Polic
Walden University NURS 6050 Polic
 
Long term effects of cognitive training
Long term effects of cognitive trainingLong term effects of cognitive training
Long term effects of cognitive training
 
Flak2014 article the_memoryaidstudyprotocolforar
Flak2014 article the_memoryaidstudyprotocolforarFlak2014 article the_memoryaidstudyprotocolforar
Flak2014 article the_memoryaidstudyprotocolforar
 
Launching of cognitive training in ain shams university
Launching of cognitive training in ain shams universityLaunching of cognitive training in ain shams university
Launching of cognitive training in ain shams university
 
Why older people engage in physical activity an exploratory study of partici...
Why older people engage in physical activity  an exploratory study of partici...Why older people engage in physical activity  an exploratory study of partici...
Why older people engage in physical activity an exploratory study of partici...
 
Statistical methods for cardiovascular researchers
Statistical methods for cardiovascular researchersStatistical methods for cardiovascular researchers
Statistical methods for cardiovascular researchers
 
Disfagia
DisfagiaDisfagia
Disfagia
 
Mental Illness Prediction based on Speech using Supervised Learning
Mental Illness Prediction based on Speech using Supervised LearningMental Illness Prediction based on Speech using Supervised Learning
Mental Illness Prediction based on Speech using Supervised Learning
 
A systematic review of interventions for children with cerebral palsy state ...
A systematic review of interventions for children with cerebral palsy  state ...A systematic review of interventions for children with cerebral palsy  state ...
A systematic review of interventions for children with cerebral palsy state ...
 
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity The Cochrane Library:   Web 2.0 & phisical activity
The Cochrane Library: Web 2.0 & phisical activity
 
High School Start time paper
High School Start time paperHigh School Start time paper
High School Start time paper
 
LongitudinalAttrtionLitReviewNov09
LongitudinalAttrtionLitReviewNov09LongitudinalAttrtionLitReviewNov09
LongitudinalAttrtionLitReviewNov09
 
Supporting Procedural and Perceptual Learning in Laparoscopic Surgery
Supporting Procedural and Perceptual Learning in Laparoscopic SurgerySupporting Procedural and Perceptual Learning in Laparoscopic Surgery
Supporting Procedural and Perceptual Learning in Laparoscopic Surgery
 
Daytime Impairment due to College Student Technology Use during S(1)
Daytime Impairment due to College Student Technology Use during S(1)Daytime Impairment due to College Student Technology Use during S(1)
Daytime Impairment due to College Student Technology Use during S(1)
 
Bilingualism And Dementia Essay
Bilingualism And Dementia EssayBilingualism And Dementia Essay
Bilingualism And Dementia Essay
 
Course project ntr_5503
Course project ntr_5503Course project ntr_5503
Course project ntr_5503
 
Enrichment effects on adult
Enrichment effects on adultEnrichment effects on adult
Enrichment effects on adult
 
Efficacy of Dual Task Training to Improve Functional Gait Performance in Idio...
Efficacy of Dual Task Training to Improve Functional Gait Performance in Idio...Efficacy of Dual Task Training to Improve Functional Gait Performance in Idio...
Efficacy of Dual Task Training to Improve Functional Gait Performance in Idio...
 

Mehr von agnesdcarey33086

Sample Summaries of Emily Raine’s Why Should I Be Nice to You.docx
Sample Summaries of Emily Raine’s Why Should I Be Nice to You.docxSample Summaries of Emily Raine’s Why Should I Be Nice to You.docx
Sample Summaries of Emily Raine’s Why Should I Be Nice to You.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
SAMPLEExecutive Summary The following report is an evalua.docx
SAMPLEExecutive Summary The following report is an evalua.docxSAMPLEExecutive Summary The following report is an evalua.docx
SAMPLEExecutive Summary The following report is an evalua.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample Student Industry AnalysisExecutive SummaryCom.docx
Sample Student Industry AnalysisExecutive SummaryCom.docxSample Student Industry AnalysisExecutive SummaryCom.docx
Sample Student Industry AnalysisExecutive SummaryCom.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
sample.sql-- START-- SETUP Create userCREATE USER .docx
sample.sql-- START-- SETUP Create userCREATE USER .docxsample.sql-- START-- SETUP Create userCREATE USER .docx
sample.sql-- START-- SETUP Create userCREATE USER .docxagnesdcarey33086
 
SAMPLING MEAN DEFINITION The term sampling mean is.docx
SAMPLING MEAN  DEFINITION  The term sampling mean is.docxSAMPLING MEAN  DEFINITION  The term sampling mean is.docx
SAMPLING MEAN DEFINITION The term sampling mean is.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
SAMPLING MEANDEFINITIONThe term sampling mean is a stati.docx
SAMPLING MEANDEFINITIONThe term sampling mean is a stati.docxSAMPLING MEANDEFINITIONThe term sampling mean is a stati.docx
SAMPLING MEANDEFINITIONThe term sampling mean is a stati.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
sampleReportt.docxPower Electronics Contents.docx
sampleReportt.docxPower Electronics            Contents.docxsampleReportt.docxPower Electronics            Contents.docx
sampleReportt.docxPower Electronics Contents.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample Workflow of Answering a Telephone in an OfficeInform .docx
Sample Workflow of Answering a Telephone in an OfficeInform .docxSample Workflow of Answering a Telephone in an OfficeInform .docx
Sample Workflow of Answering a Telephone in an OfficeInform .docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample Investment PropertyAverage InlandSan Diego HomeASSUMPTION.docx
Sample Investment PropertyAverage InlandSan Diego HomeASSUMPTION.docxSample Investment PropertyAverage InlandSan Diego HomeASSUMPTION.docx
Sample Investment PropertyAverage InlandSan Diego HomeASSUMPTION.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
SAMPLE Project (Answers and explanations are in red)I opened t.docx
SAMPLE Project (Answers and explanations are in red)I opened t.docxSAMPLE Project (Answers and explanations are in red)I opened t.docx
SAMPLE Project (Answers and explanations are in red)I opened t.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample Questions to Ask During an Informational Interview .docx
Sample Questions to Ask During an Informational Interview  .docxSample Questions to Ask During an Informational Interview  .docx
Sample Questions to Ask During an Informational Interview .docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample Table.pdfTopic RatingPatients Goal Able to walk .docx
Sample Table.pdfTopic RatingPatients Goal Able to walk .docxSample Table.pdfTopic RatingPatients Goal Able to walk .docx
Sample Table.pdfTopic RatingPatients Goal Able to walk .docxagnesdcarey33086
 
SAMPLE QUESTIONExercise 1 Consider the functionf (x,C).docx
SAMPLE QUESTIONExercise 1 Consider the functionf (x,C).docxSAMPLE QUESTIONExercise 1 Consider the functionf (x,C).docx
SAMPLE QUESTIONExercise 1 Consider the functionf (x,C).docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample PowerPoint Flow Week 5Select a current product with which.docx
Sample PowerPoint Flow Week 5Select a current product with which.docxSample PowerPoint Flow Week 5Select a current product with which.docx
Sample PowerPoint Flow Week 5Select a current product with which.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample Of assignmentIntroductionComment by Jane Summers Introd.docx
Sample Of assignmentIntroductionComment by Jane Summers Introd.docxSample Of assignmentIntroductionComment by Jane Summers Introd.docx
Sample Of assignmentIntroductionComment by Jane Summers Introd.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample Access Control Policy1.Purpose2.Scope3.Pol.docx
Sample Access Control Policy1.Purpose2.Scope3.Pol.docxSample Access Control Policy1.Purpose2.Scope3.Pol.docx
Sample Access Control Policy1.Purpose2.Scope3.Pol.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
SAMPLE GED 501 RESEARCH PAPERTechnology Based Education How.docx
SAMPLE GED 501 RESEARCH PAPERTechnology Based Education How.docxSAMPLE GED 501 RESEARCH PAPERTechnology Based Education How.docx
SAMPLE GED 501 RESEARCH PAPERTechnology Based Education How.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample Action Research Report 1 Effect of Technol.docx
Sample Action Research Report 1    Effect of Technol.docxSample Action Research Report 1    Effect of Technol.docx
Sample Action Research Report 1 Effect of Technol.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Sample Case with a report Dawit Zerom, Instructor Cas.docx
Sample Case with a report Dawit Zerom, Instructor  Cas.docxSample Case with a report Dawit Zerom, Instructor  Cas.docx
Sample Case with a report Dawit Zerom, Instructor Cas.docxagnesdcarey33086
 
Salkind_datasetsCrab Scale Results.savSalkind_datasetsLess.docx
Salkind_datasetsCrab Scale Results.savSalkind_datasetsLess.docxSalkind_datasetsCrab Scale Results.savSalkind_datasetsLess.docx
Salkind_datasetsCrab Scale Results.savSalkind_datasetsLess.docxagnesdcarey33086
 

Mehr von agnesdcarey33086 (20)

Sample Summaries of Emily Raine’s Why Should I Be Nice to You.docx
Sample Summaries of Emily Raine’s Why Should I Be Nice to You.docxSample Summaries of Emily Raine’s Why Should I Be Nice to You.docx
Sample Summaries of Emily Raine’s Why Should I Be Nice to You.docx
 
SAMPLEExecutive Summary The following report is an evalua.docx
SAMPLEExecutive Summary The following report is an evalua.docxSAMPLEExecutive Summary The following report is an evalua.docx
SAMPLEExecutive Summary The following report is an evalua.docx
 
Sample Student Industry AnalysisExecutive SummaryCom.docx
Sample Student Industry AnalysisExecutive SummaryCom.docxSample Student Industry AnalysisExecutive SummaryCom.docx
Sample Student Industry AnalysisExecutive SummaryCom.docx
 
sample.sql-- START-- SETUP Create userCREATE USER .docx
sample.sql-- START-- SETUP Create userCREATE USER .docxsample.sql-- START-- SETUP Create userCREATE USER .docx
sample.sql-- START-- SETUP Create userCREATE USER .docx
 
SAMPLING MEAN DEFINITION The term sampling mean is.docx
SAMPLING MEAN  DEFINITION  The term sampling mean is.docxSAMPLING MEAN  DEFINITION  The term sampling mean is.docx
SAMPLING MEAN DEFINITION The term sampling mean is.docx
 
SAMPLING MEANDEFINITIONThe term sampling mean is a stati.docx
SAMPLING MEANDEFINITIONThe term sampling mean is a stati.docxSAMPLING MEANDEFINITIONThe term sampling mean is a stati.docx
SAMPLING MEANDEFINITIONThe term sampling mean is a stati.docx
 
sampleReportt.docxPower Electronics Contents.docx
sampleReportt.docxPower Electronics            Contents.docxsampleReportt.docxPower Electronics            Contents.docx
sampleReportt.docxPower Electronics Contents.docx
 
Sample Workflow of Answering a Telephone in an OfficeInform .docx
Sample Workflow of Answering a Telephone in an OfficeInform .docxSample Workflow of Answering a Telephone in an OfficeInform .docx
Sample Workflow of Answering a Telephone in an OfficeInform .docx
 
Sample Investment PropertyAverage InlandSan Diego HomeASSUMPTION.docx
Sample Investment PropertyAverage InlandSan Diego HomeASSUMPTION.docxSample Investment PropertyAverage InlandSan Diego HomeASSUMPTION.docx
Sample Investment PropertyAverage InlandSan Diego HomeASSUMPTION.docx
 
SAMPLE Project (Answers and explanations are in red)I opened t.docx
SAMPLE Project (Answers and explanations are in red)I opened t.docxSAMPLE Project (Answers and explanations are in red)I opened t.docx
SAMPLE Project (Answers and explanations are in red)I opened t.docx
 
Sample Questions to Ask During an Informational Interview .docx
Sample Questions to Ask During an Informational Interview  .docxSample Questions to Ask During an Informational Interview  .docx
Sample Questions to Ask During an Informational Interview .docx
 
Sample Table.pdfTopic RatingPatients Goal Able to walk .docx
Sample Table.pdfTopic RatingPatients Goal Able to walk .docxSample Table.pdfTopic RatingPatients Goal Able to walk .docx
Sample Table.pdfTopic RatingPatients Goal Able to walk .docx
 
SAMPLE QUESTIONExercise 1 Consider the functionf (x,C).docx
SAMPLE QUESTIONExercise 1 Consider the functionf (x,C).docxSAMPLE QUESTIONExercise 1 Consider the functionf (x,C).docx
SAMPLE QUESTIONExercise 1 Consider the functionf (x,C).docx
 
Sample PowerPoint Flow Week 5Select a current product with which.docx
Sample PowerPoint Flow Week 5Select a current product with which.docxSample PowerPoint Flow Week 5Select a current product with which.docx
Sample PowerPoint Flow Week 5Select a current product with which.docx
 
Sample Of assignmentIntroductionComment by Jane Summers Introd.docx
Sample Of assignmentIntroductionComment by Jane Summers Introd.docxSample Of assignmentIntroductionComment by Jane Summers Introd.docx
Sample Of assignmentIntroductionComment by Jane Summers Introd.docx
 
Sample Access Control Policy1.Purpose2.Scope3.Pol.docx
Sample Access Control Policy1.Purpose2.Scope3.Pol.docxSample Access Control Policy1.Purpose2.Scope3.Pol.docx
Sample Access Control Policy1.Purpose2.Scope3.Pol.docx
 
SAMPLE GED 501 RESEARCH PAPERTechnology Based Education How.docx
SAMPLE GED 501 RESEARCH PAPERTechnology Based Education How.docxSAMPLE GED 501 RESEARCH PAPERTechnology Based Education How.docx
SAMPLE GED 501 RESEARCH PAPERTechnology Based Education How.docx
 
Sample Action Research Report 1 Effect of Technol.docx
Sample Action Research Report 1    Effect of Technol.docxSample Action Research Report 1    Effect of Technol.docx
Sample Action Research Report 1 Effect of Technol.docx
 
Sample Case with a report Dawit Zerom, Instructor Cas.docx
Sample Case with a report Dawit Zerom, Instructor  Cas.docxSample Case with a report Dawit Zerom, Instructor  Cas.docx
Sample Case with a report Dawit Zerom, Instructor Cas.docx
 
Salkind_datasetsCrab Scale Results.savSalkind_datasetsLess.docx
Salkind_datasetsCrab Scale Results.savSalkind_datasetsLess.docxSalkind_datasetsCrab Scale Results.savSalkind_datasetsLess.docx
Salkind_datasetsCrab Scale Results.savSalkind_datasetsLess.docx
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...Amil baba
 
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptxGoogle Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptxDr. Sarita Anand
 
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024Elizabeth Walsh
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxannathomasp01
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsMebane Rash
 
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxPooja Bhuva
 
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the ClassroomFostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the ClassroomPooky Knightsmith
 
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptxExploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptxPooja Bhuva
 
Basic Intentional Injuries Health Education
Basic Intentional Injuries Health EducationBasic Intentional Injuries Health Education
Basic Intentional Injuries Health EducationNeilDeclaro1
 
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfUnit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfDr Vijay Vishwakarma
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxmarlenawright1
 
Tatlong Kwento ni Lola basyang-1.pdf arts
Tatlong Kwento ni Lola basyang-1.pdf artsTatlong Kwento ni Lola basyang-1.pdf arts
Tatlong Kwento ni Lola basyang-1.pdf artsNbelano25
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and ModificationsMJDuyan
 
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning PresentationSOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentationcamerronhm
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfNirmal Dwivedi
 
Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)
Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)
Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)Jisc
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...Poonam Aher Patil
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.MaryamAhmad92
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
 
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptxGoogle Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
Google Gemini An AI Revolution in Education.pptx
 
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
 
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan FellowsOn National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
On National Teacher Day, meet the 2024-25 Kenan Fellows
 
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptxOn_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
On_Translating_a_Tamil_Poem_by_A_K_Ramanujan.pptx
 
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the ClassroomFostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds  in the Classroom
Fostering Friendships - Enhancing Social Bonds in the Classroom
 
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptxExploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
Exploring_the_Narrative_Style_of_Amitav_Ghoshs_Gun_Island.pptx
 
Basic Intentional Injuries Health Education
Basic Intentional Injuries Health EducationBasic Intentional Injuries Health Education
Basic Intentional Injuries Health Education
 
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfUnit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
 
Tatlong Kwento ni Lola basyang-1.pdf arts
Tatlong Kwento ni Lola basyang-1.pdf artsTatlong Kwento ni Lola basyang-1.pdf arts
Tatlong Kwento ni Lola basyang-1.pdf arts
 
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding  Accommodations and ModificationsUnderstanding  Accommodations and Modifications
Understanding Accommodations and Modifications
 
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning PresentationSOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
SOC 101 Demonstration of Learning Presentation
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
 
Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)
Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)
Jamworks pilot and AI at Jisc (20/03/2024)
 
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
80 ĐỀ THI THỬ TUYỂN SINH TIẾNG ANH VÀO 10 SỞ GD – ĐT THÀNH PHỐ HỒ CHÍ MINH NĂ...
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual  Proper...
General Principles of Intellectual Property: Concepts of Intellectual Proper...
 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
 

Running  head  RESEARCH  PROPOSAL   1  .docx

  • 2. Research Proposal Background Australia’s Ageing population At present, Australia’s population is aged, as indicated by the large proportion of older adult residents, and . This would at least be partially attributable to 100 years of progressively greater longevities (Howat & Stoneham, 2011). With increasing longevity come several challenges, for individuals and the health care sector if a number individuals later life is spent in a state of morbidity. While normal ageing research is not without contention (e.g., about the underpinning processes in cognitive decline), evidence suggests that the process of normal ageing appears to involve a decline in such higher order cognitive domains as memory and information processing REF, which are crucial for living autonomously. While many cognitive functions are studied in relation to ageing, memory impairment topic of research in older adulthood, as it comprises multiple cognitive abilities that are
  • 3. each fundamental to in successful ageing. The Ageing Process A normal ageing trajectory human beings represents a latent, time-dependent biological process (Moody & Sasser, 2014), which albeit it is not pathological, is often characterised by functional degradation in one or more of physical, sensory, or cognitive abilities (Desjardins, 2012). Many adults experience some age-related cognitive decline (Deary et al., 2009), but the explicit causal underpinnings of such changes are not robustly understood. The idea of negative learning was proposed in Mahncke et al.’s (2006) framework, which argues that normal cognitive changes stem from the combined effect of physical ageing of the cortices and negative cortical plasticity (i.e., plasticity with negative consequences) (Mahncke, Bronstone, and Merzenich, 2006). Briefly outsine the scaffolding idea and negative cortical plasticity
  • 4. Normal ageing The maximum upper limit of human lifespan RESEARCH PROPOSAL 3 The maximum human lifespan is indicated to have a fixed theoretical upper limit of 120 years (Moody & Sasser, 2014). This upper bound has remained unchanged over time, while the life expectancy continues to climb. Promoting optimal development right across the lifespan is a crucial task of developmental psychology Compression of morbidity hypothesis Discuss briefly in conjunction: max lifespan, compression of morbidity theory in relation to health goals and the possibility of human beings, as prevention strategies implemented with healthy participants fosters the healthy trajectory not the one of morbidity.
  • 5. “With this concept of limit in mind, compression of morbidity is attractive because delaying dysfunction would enhance the quality of life, extend life expectancy, and reduce health care costs (Butler, 1995). A compression-of-morbidity strategy would move life “ Prospective Memory What is PM (related to RM, what do they encompass?) Qualitative study: Subjective complaints of particcipants were all PM and some RM, some really dangerous (community-dwelling older adults, focus group) Types of PM, cued. Cortical Plasticity Evidence of maintaining plasticity throughout lifespan Negative learning (NCP) Conclusions of OA misbeliefs (negative learning) Cognitive training programs Extant research Process vs. system designs Virtual week
  • 6. Original, P. R. RESEARCH PROPOSAL 4 Computerised, 2000 VW14, computerised with graded difficulty etc. Rationale. Gaps in literature study aims to address Firstly, relatively few studies include control groups when running studies with cognitive training (REF), yet it is crucial for being able to make causal inferences about its efficacy. Secondly, many cognitive training programs are conducted in laboratories, which limits ecological validity. The design of the study has participants carrying out the tasks in their own home, without the experimenter there was they play, and the tasks are relevant to functioning outside of the study. Finally, the adherence of participants to
  • 7. the prescribed training program and protocol is not a prioritised assessment in training studies. Current Study Aims and objectives. The two primary objectives of this pilot study are to examine both the usability and assess the preliminary efficacy of 6-weeks of Virtual Week Training 2014 (VWT14) program for improving prospective memory in healthy Australian older adults. In addition, this study will aim to analyse changes in subjective reports of memory functioning following 6-weeks of VWT14 program. VWT14 may represent one way of decelerating normal prospective memory decline in older adulthood, and as such, it is hypothesised that participants who have completed the training program will have significant improvement in their prospective memory compared to waitlist controls, based on Memory for Intentions Screening Test (MIST) (S. Raskin, Buckheit, &
  • 8. RESEARCH PROPOSAL 5 Sherrod, 2010) performance. It is also hypothesised that when all participants’ posttest data are pooled, there will significant changes from pre- to posttest performance on the MIST. Finally, it is hypothesised that subjective reports of memory, based on the Memory Function Questionnaire Revised (MFQ-R, Gilewski, Zelinski, & Schaie, 1990), will improve from pre- to posttest. Research Methodology Participants A B C D E F 1 Recruitment phase Prospective participants 2 Make contact (phone/email)
  • 9. Pre-screen checklist First assessment 3 Arrange in-person meeting Cog screen; BG survey; MFQ 4 Participants assigned to 1/2 conditions 5 Stratified by age, gender, ACE-III score 6 Pretesting phase Figure 1: Participant Selection Map (coordinates referred to throughout research methods section) Study design
  • 10. This pilot study will be a pre-post-test waitlist control design with both intervention and comparison groups, so as to pilot the VWT14 program as a means of improving prospective memory in healthy older adults. Inclusion/exclusion criteria Participants will be deemed eligible to partake in the study if they meet all of the criteria examined during the screening assessment (see Appendix 1). Measures RESEARCH PROPOSAL 6 Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination III ipad version (ACEmobile) (Newman, Noad, & Hodges, 2014) The ACE-III (Mathuranath, Nestor, Berrios, Rakowicz, & Hodges, 2000) is a sensitive and specific screening tool for general cognitive functioning that takes appropriately 15 minutes
  • 11. to administer. Nineteen brief subtests comprise the following five dimensions: attention/orientation, memory, verbal fluency, language, and visuo-spatial, which give five sub- scores and a total score out of 100. The ACE-III has the capacity to screen for cognitive impairment and diagnoses, severity and profile of cognitive functioning, and has three Australian alternate forms for assessing change and comparing scores between groups. ACEmobile, an electronic version of ACE-III was released for the iPad June 2014, and will be used in this study. It enhances administration through on-screen instructions, requires no training, reduces error by scoring automatically, and provides results instantly on-screen and via email to the researcher. Memory Functioning Questionnaire Revised REF DESCRIBE TEST Memory for Intentions Screening Test The Memory for Intentions Screening Test (MIST) (S. A. Raskin, 2009) is a standardised tool for measuring prospective memory that takes approximately 30 minutes to administer. The
  • 12. MIST is reported to have sound psychometric properties (Woods et al., 2008), and be sensitive to mild impairment (Karantzoulis, Troyer, & Rich, 2009). Participants are provided with an eight- task set of tests at the outset of a testing session and must remember, throughout the session, to perform both time-based tasks (‘in exactly x minutes, please alert me that it is break time’) and event-based tasks (‘when I pass you the blue pen, may you write your name on this sheet of paper’). Between these PM tasks, participants solve word puzzles, while the researcher observes RESEARCH PROPOSAL 7 whether the PM tasks are remembered and carried out. Correct and error responses are both recorded, while variables and error scores can be combined to discern different types of prospective memory fail Recruitment
  • 13. Participants will be sourced from the community (Catholic homes, advertisements in relevant public locations, word-of-mouth) (A1, 2, 3). Prospective participants will be asked to contact the researcher via either email or telephone if they are interested in partaking or to find out more information about the study (B1, B2, 3). The researcher will then screen these individuals over the phone using a checklist (C2). Procedure After eligible participants have been explained the details of the study (C2) and provided informed consent (D2), they will then be asked to fill in a background information survey, the Memory Functions Questionnaire (Revised), and be screened on global cognitive ability to ensure healthy cognitive functioning on the ACE-III (D3). Both the intervention and waitlist control groups will partake in the VWT14 program, but the immediate intervention group will commence the 6-week training within the first two weeks of screening (C2), while the waitlist control will commence their 6-week block around 8 weeks after screening (C2). Given the data
  • 14. collection and intervention implementation procedures are conducted one participant at a time rather than altogether in a group, so long as each participant has the appropriate intervals across screening, pre-test, and post-test for their assigned condition (i.e., immediate intervention or waitlist control), the waitlist control group does not necessarily have to wait until each immediate intervention participant has completed the training. Specifically, so long as the waitlist control has had a 6-week no-contact interval after screening, they can be pre-tested and RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8 commence training as soon as this 6-week period is over (see Appendix 2 for visual representation). The Memory for Intentions Test (MIST) (S. Raskin et al., 2010) will be administered at two time points for each participant: for the immediate
  • 15. intervention group, first time (pretest) will be within two weeks following the first meeting between participant and researcher (D2, 3), and two weeks before the start of the VWT14 program, and second time (posttest) will be within a week of completing VWT14. For the waitlist control group, their pretest will be 6 weeks after the first meeting between participant and researcher, and this pretest data will serve as a comparison point for intervention effects. Planned Statistical Analyses The following table outlines how data will be analysed in order to address the aims and objectives of the study. Due to the nature of a pilot study, the sample size is projected to be relatively small so until distributions are analysed, it is unknown whether non- or parametric statistical analyses will be more appropriate, so both are recorded below where necessary. Table 1 Planned Statistical Analyses Assessing By: Variables Statistics
  • 16. Efficacy (a) Of VW at improving PM performance on MIST (control comparison) Change scores on MIST from pre- to posttest: between groups difference (independent samples) IV1 group: intervention /control IV2 time point: pretest/ posttest DV change scores: MIST Hodges-Lehmann estimate of median differences (estimates the median difference between the groups’ change scores from pre-post test) Efficacy (b) Of VW at improving PM performance on MIST (pooled comparison) Scores on MIST, difference from pre-
  • 17. to posttest: within groups difference (paired sample) IV1 time point: pretest/ posttest DV scores: MIST Paired samples t-test or Wilcoxon test (changes in the same participants’ scores across two time points) Efficacy (c) Of VW at Scores on MFQ IV1 time point: Paired samples t-test or Wilcoxon RESEARCH PROPOSAL 9 improving subjective perceptions of memory (pooled comparison) difference from pre- to posttest: within groups difference pretest/ posttest
  • 18. DV scores: MFQ test (changes in the same participants’ scores across two time points) Usability Of VW training program for older adults Scores on VWUQ- 14, an adapted version of Lewis’ 1995 questionnaire DV scores: VWUQ- 14 Descriptive statistics: means, standard deviations, confidence intervals, and % ≥ or ≤ given scoring ranges (i.e., difficulty: easy, med, hard) according to author’s recommended scoring. Adherence/ compliance To training program’s prescribed protocol Rate of sessions
  • 19. completed per week (χ/4) (& total χ/24) DV rate: of completed sessions out of 4 Descriptive statistics: means, SDs, CIs, n’s and % rates of completed sessions per week, total over program, and % of participants completing all prescribed sessions. Usability of VWT14 A usability questionnaire (adapted from Lewis, 1995) will be administered via email or post within one week of each participant’s final VWT14 session (as shown by automated data). Participants rated how strongly they agreed with each item on a scale of 1(Strongly Disagree) to 7 (Strongly Agree) (see Appendix 3). Efficacy of VWT14 The primary efficacy outcome measure was performance on the MIST (S. Raskin et al., 2010) (with control comparison), which aims to objectively measure prospective memory.
  • 20. The secondary efficacy outcome measures were performance on the MIST (without control comparison), and responses on the MFQ, a self-report questionnaire that assessed subjective perceptions of memory functioning. RESEARCH PROPOSAL 10 Appendix 1: Pre-Screen and Screener Assessments The following will be asked/assessed during phone calls with prospective participants (pre- screen) Inclusion Criteria No. Question Y N 1 Are you aged 60 or older 2 Are you retired 3 Do you have a windows computer? 4 Are you comfortable using the computer 5 Are you living independently in the community (i.e. without
  • 21. assistance) 6 Are you comfortable reading and understanding written instructions in English Exclusion Criteria 1 Do you have any neuro/psychiatric disorders or medical conditions that could affect your ability to play a computer game e.g., epilepsy, blindness 2 Are you involved in another research study at this time The following will be information will be collected during the first face-to-face meeting (Screener) 1. Cognitive functioning using Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination V.3 (ACE-III) ipad version 2. Background information using a demographic questionnaire 3. Subjective memory functioning using the MFQ
  • 22. RESEARCH PROPOSAL 11 Appendix 2: Data Collection and Intervention Timelines for Two Groups The following two hypothetical timelines are displaying one of two pathways each participant can take depending on which group they are assigned. 1 2 ! ! 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
  • 25. PRETEST Immediate Intervention Waitlist Control SCREENER PRESCREEN PARTICIPANT RECRUITMENT RESEARCH PROPOSAL 12 Appendix 3: Usability Questionnaire The following questionnaire will be given at the completion of the 6-week training period; it is adapted from Lewis (1995). Virtual Week 14 Usability Questionnaire (VWUQ-14) For each of the statements below, please circle the rating of your choice. 1 Overall, I am satisfied with the ease of completing the Virtual Week program
  • 26. Strongly AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly DISAGREE 2 I am satisfied with the availability of support information (phone calls, messages, instructions/documentation) for completing this task Strongly AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly DISAGREE 3 I am satisfied with how clear/easy to understand instructions/documentation for completing this task was Strongly AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly DISAGREE 4 I enjoyed playing Virtual Week Strongly AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly DISAGREE 5 It was easy to learn to play/use Virtual Week Strongly AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
  • 27. Strongly DISAGREE 6 I felt comfortable using the Virtual Week program Strongly AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly DISAGREE 7 I would recommend Virtual Week to family and friends Strongly AGREE 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly DISAGREE RESEARCH PROPOSAL 13 Appendix 4: Memory Functioning Questionnaire-Revised
  • 28. RESEARCH PROPOSAL 14 110 Se riousness of Fo rgetting When you actually forget in these situations, how serious of a problem do you consider the memory to be? very serious somewhat serious not serious a. names 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 b. faces 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 c. where you put things (e.g., keys) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 d. directions to places 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 e. beginning to do something and forgetting what you were doing 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _____________________________________________________ _________________________ R et rospective F unctioning How is your memory compared to the way is was? much worse same much better
  • 29. a. 1 year ago? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 b. 5 years ago? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 c. 10 years ago? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 d. 20 years ago? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 e. when you were 18? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _____________________________________________________ _________________________ M nemonics Usage How often do you use these techniques to remind yourself about things? always sometimes never a. keep an appointment book 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 b. write yourself reminder notes 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 c. make lists of things to do 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 d. make grocery lists 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 e. plan your daily schedule in advance 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 f. mental repetition 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 g. associations with other things 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 h. keep things you need to do in a prominent place where you will notice them 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 _____________________________________________________ _________________________ RESEARCH
  • 30. PROPOSAL 15 References Deary, I. J., Corley, J., Gow, A. J., Harris, S. E., Houlihan, L. M., Marioni, R. E., . . . Starr, J. M. (2009). Age-associated cognitive decline. British Medical Bulletin, 92(1), 135-152. Desjardins, R. (2012). Ageing and skills: a review and analysis of skill gain and skill loss over the lifespan and over time. Gilewski, M. J., Zelinski, E. M., & Schaie, K. W. (1990). The Memory Functioning Questionnaire for assessment of memory complaints in adulthood and old age. Psychology and aging, 5(4), 482. Howat, P., & Stoneham, M. (2011). Why sustainable population growth is a key to climate change and public health equity. Health Promotion Journal of Australia, 22(4), 34-38. Karantzoulis, S., Troyer, A. K., & Rich, J. B. (2009). Prospective memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(3), 407-415. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090596 Lewis, J. R. (1995). IBM computer usability satisfaction
  • 31. questionnaires: Psychometric evaluation and instructions for use. International Journal of Human- Computer Interaction, 7(1), 57- 78. Mathuranath, P. S., Nestor, P. J., Berrios, G. E., Rakowicz, W., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). A brief cognitive test battery to differentiate Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 55(11), 1613-1620. Moody, H. R., & Sasser, J. R. (2014). Aging: Concepts and Controversies: SAGE Publications. Newman, C., Noad, R., & Hodges, J. (2014). ACEmobile (Version iPad): Neuroscience Research Australia & Plymouth University. Retrieved from http://www.acemobile.org Raskin, S., Buckheit, C., & Sherrod, C. (2010). Memory for intentions test (MIST). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Raskin, S. A. (2009). Memory for intentions screening test: Psychometric properties and clinical evidence. Brain Impairment, 10(1), 23-33. doi: 10.1375/brim.10.1.23 Woods, S. P., Moran, L. M., Dawson, M. S., Carey, C. L., Grant, I., Atkinson, J. H., . . . Vaida, F. (2008). Psychometric characteristics of the memory for intentions screening test. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22(5), 864-878. doi: 10.1080/13854040701595999
  • 32. Prospective memory Successful prospective memory depends on retrospective (episodic) memory processes (e.g., encoding, retrieval), as well as controlled attention/executive processes associated with planning, working memory, and task switching As such, having good prospective memory may be especially important for older adults, who often have health-related prospective memory demands, such as remembering to take medication. Prospective memory (PM) refers to memory for future intentions, such as remembering to take medication, to turn off appliances, pay bills and keep appointments. It involves “remembering to carry out actions” (Huppert & Beardsall, 1993), or “remembering to do things in the future” (McDaniel, Einstein, & Rendell, 2008). PM involves recognising that a task needs to be undertaken in the future, encoding the task and the cue to perform the task, storing the task and the cue, monitoring for and recognising the cue while engaged in other ongoing tasks, with retrieval of the content of the task when the cue is recognised. Thus PM is not a single cognitive process but contains elements of multiple cognitive processes including attention, working memory, retrospective memory and executive functioning (Shum & Fleming, 2009). This ability is crucial to the maintenance of functional independence, which is a fundamental concern for older adults (Chasteen, Park, & Schwarz, 2001). Problems with PM cause more deficits in activities of daily living, instrumental activities of daily living and caregiver burden than do retrospective memory (RM) failures (Smith, Della Sala, Logie, & Maylor, 2000). It is therefore of considerable concern that PM is often disrupted in the context of normal adult ageing (Henry, MacLeod, Phillips, & Crawford, 2004; Kvavilashvili, Kornbrot, Mash, Cockburn, & Milne, 2009)and to an even greater extent in dementia, even in the mild (Martins & Damasceno, 2008) and preclinical stages (Duchek, Balota, & Cortese, 2006; Jones, Livner, & Backman, 2006). As will be discussed, there is
  • 33. evidence of PM impairment in individuals with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), a condition in which cognitive difficulties are present, but diagnostic criteria for dementia are not met (Petersen, 2004). In contrast to PM, retrospective memory (RM) refers to memory of past knowledge and events. Most memory assessment and research to date has focused on this type of memory function, using tests of learning, recall and recognition such as the Wechsler Memory Scales (Wechsler, 1945, 1997b) and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; Rey, 1964). Normal adult ageing is associated with decline in some cognitive abilities (Rabbitt & Lowe, 2000), including RM (Ivnik, Malec, Smith, Tangalos, Petersen, Kokmen, & Kurland, 1992; Kral, Cahn, & Mueller, 1964). Memory training (cognitive training to enhance functioning or postpone decline) Training programs designed to enhance memory among older adults have proliferated in recent years. Memory training programs exist within the larger context of cognitive training, which is based on the notion that cognition is plastic in older age (Acevedo & Lowenstein, 2007; Rebok, Carlson, & Langbaum, 2007). Through cognitive plasticity and reserve (Stern, 2002), mentally stimulating activities like memory training help maintain and improve cognitive and functional abilities in daily life (Hertzog et al., 2008). Memory training interventions facilitate plasticity by teaching various strategies that help encode and retrieve information (Gross & Rebok, 2011; McDaniel, Einstein, & Jacoby, 2008; Rebok et al., 2007; Verhaeghen, Marcoen, & Goossens, 1992). In addition to improving objective memory performance, research shows interventions can improve self-rated subjective memory (Floyd & Scogin, 1997). Framework for prospective memory and ageing There is some evidence that prospective memory deteriorates with age (Huppert et al., 2000). This age difference is more apparent when using laboratory assessments as opposed to more
  • 34. naturalistic assessment paradigms (Henry et al., 2004). It is difficult to ascertain if prospective memory or retrospective memory is more affected by age, because of heterogeneous methodologies (Maylor et al., 2002). Prospective memory errors, however, may be more concerning and salient than retrospective memory errors (Maylor et al., 2002). Hence why the individual components of memory (PM, RM, WM) will be measured. Prospective memory has been studied most extensively with regard to its functioning in older adults (typically those over 60 years of age). Craik (1986) stimulated an early and abiding interest in examining prospective memory in older adults with his seminal framework on age-related memory deficits. Craik suggested that memory tasks could be ordered in terms of the amount of self-initiated processes required to retrieve the target information. Generally, the fewer the cues provided by the memory task, the more retrieval is dependent on processes initiated by the individual, such as generating possible cues, generating potential targets, and implementing any other strategies that will help bring the desired information to mind. E.g., recognition is considered to have low self-initiated retrieval demands, because the recognition task provides the target item. In this scheme, prospective memory is thought to have the highest self-initiated retrieval demands because not only is there an absence of cues, but also one has to remember to remember. According to Craik's framework, because self-initiated retrieval presumably requires extensive processing resources and because processing resources decline with age, age-related deficits in memory should be a function of the amount of self-initiated retrieval required by the memory task. Because prospective memory is assumed to require the most self-initiated retrieval, this compelling theory makes the strong prediction that prospective memory tasks would be especially difficult for older adults.
  • 35. Virtual Week Memory Training Program (computerised Australian version, 2014) A test that has shown considerable promise in research studies of PM is Virtual Week (Rendell & Craik, 2000; Rendell & Henry, 2009). Virtual Week takes the structure of a board game, in which participants move through each day as they circuit the board, with a series of events occurring and tasks to be remembered each day. The test can be computerised to facilitate data capture. Virtual Week has evidence of good reliability and validity, with reliability estimates from .84 to .94 (Rose, Rendell, & McDaniel, 2007) and validity established through controlled studies showing group differences with poorer performance in the aged (Rendell & Craik, 2000; Rose et al., 2007) and in clinical populations including schizophrenia (Henry et al., 2007), multiple sclerosis (Rendell et al., 2007b) and substance abuse (Rendell et al., 2007a). As with Virtual Street, Virtual Week has been argued to be more ecologically valid than more conventional laboratory assessments, in that it mirrors the types of PM tasks that occur in daily life (Rendell & Henry, 2009). One of the most important advantages of Virtual Week as a measure of PM is the inclusion of time and event-based PM cues and regular and irregular tasks. A unique and important aspect of this test is that a differentiated profile of impairment on Virtual Week may therefore be informative of the degree of PM impairment per se, and also of the particular circumstances in which PM impairment is more likely to arise (and consequently the manner in which rehabilitation efforts should be targeted). While a number of promising measures of PM have now been developed, of these, Virtual Week is notable for its psychometric properties, for including tasks that vary in their relative demands, and having documented sensitivity to both the effects of normal adult ageing, as well as psychiatric and neurological illness. Thus Virtual week was the test chosen for a group comparison of PM performance in Study 1.
  • 36. Training Protocol In the training protocol, we used the original Virtual Week paradigm and extended it to a process-based training version to examine whether older adults’ prospective memory could be improved and if training gains could transfer to other cognitive and everyday life tasks (Rose et al., 2012). The study consisted of three experimental groups, one group performed the Virtual Week training; the second group, an active control group, performed a music training; the third group was a waitlist, no- contact control group. In total, 50 older adults between 60 and 80 years participated in the study. It is anticipated the first training session will take 40-45 mins. At the training session the patient participant will be provided with a USB version of Virtual Week to use on their home computer over six weeks. They will be advised to use the training program four times per week over six weeks. Each training session will take approximately 20 to 30 minutes to complete each day. On insertion of the USB into the participant’s computer, it will bring up prompts to begin the training at the appropriate level and limit the number of sessions that can be completed on each day. The training version of Virtual Week has the critical factor of adaptive difficulty where the task starts at a relatively easy level and then is progressively more difficult, adapting to the performance of the participant. Another key feature of this program is the distributed practice of the cognitive training, in this case over six weeks, rather than massed practice in one or two long sessions. A final key training feature, is that Virtual Week has provision to provide feedback at the conclusion of each 'virtual day' (which will occur twice per session) so that participants are aware of their performance on each day and of their progression through the difficulty levels. The program also has provision to record performance. At the first training session the RA will provide face-to-face support to the patient participant in using the computer program.
  • 37. An automatic emailing system will also be set up by the RA to send participant's training data automatically at the completion of each session so that the participant’s involvement in the restorative memory training program can be monitored. In addition, the RA will make contact with participants by telephone twice a week to offer support and monitor progress. The RA will also be accessible to participants by telephone four days a week, during business hours to answer queries or resolve issues relating to the program. Face-to-face support will be arranged where needed. After six weeks of using the memory training program the RA will arrange for the USB to be returned. 1 23 Psychological Research An International Journal of Perception, Attention, Memory, and Action ISSN 0340-0727 Psychological Research DOI 10.1007/s00426-014-0566-4 Prospective memory training in older adults and its relevance for successful aging Alexandra Hering, Peter G. Rendell, Nathan S. Rose, Katharina M. Schnitzspahn & Matthias Kliegel
  • 38. 1 23 Your article is protected by copyright and all rights are held exclusively by Springer- Verlag Berlin Heidelberg. This e-offprint is for personal use only and shall not be self- archived in electronic repositories. If you wish to self-archive your article, please use the accepted manuscript version for posting on your own website. You may further deposit the accepted manuscript version in any repository, provided it is only made publicly available 12 months after official publication or later and provided acknowledgement is given to the original source of publication and a link is inserted to the published article on Springer's website. The link must be accompanied by the following text: "The final publication is available at link.springer.com”.
  • 39. R E V I E W Prospective memory training in older adults and its relevance for successful aging Alexandra Hering • Peter G. Rendell • Nathan S. Rose • Katharina M. Schnitzspahn • Matthias Kliegel Received: 31 May 2013 / Accepted: 22 March 2014 � Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014 Abstract In research on cognitive plasticity, two training approaches have been established: (1) training of strategies to improve performance in a given task (e.g., encoding strategies to improve episodic memory perfor- mance) and (2) training of basic cognitive processes (e.g., working memory, inhibition) that underlie a range of more complex cognitive tasks (e.g., planning) to improve both the training target and the complex transfer tasks. Strategy training aims to compensate or circum-
  • 40. vent limitations in underlying processes, while process training attempts to augment or to restore these pro- cesses. Although research on both approaches has pro- duced some promising findings, results are still heterogeneous and the impact of most training regimes for everyday life is unknown. We, therefore, discuss recent proposals of training regimes aiming to improve prospective memory (i.e., forming and realizing delayed intentions) as this type of complex cognition is highly relevant for independent living. Furthermore, prospective memory is associated with working memory and exec- utive functions and age-related decline is widely repor- ted. We review initial evidence suggesting that both training regimes (i.e., strategy and/or process training) can successfully be applied to improve prospective memory. Conceptual and methodological implications of the findings for research on age-related prospective memory and for training research in general are
  • 41. discussed. Introduction Aging is associated with cognitive decline causing indi- vidual and social burdens. Lawton et al. (1999), for example, showed how threatening cognitive decline can be for older adults. They asked 600 older adults how long they would desire to live under the condition of functional limitations (e.g., being in a bedridden state), cognitive impairment (e.g., being confused) or pain. Above 60 % of the participants aged 70 years and older did not wish to live any longer under any condition of cognitive impairment. Indeed, cognitive decline was deemed more threatening for older adults’ quality of life than functional impairment or pain. These results illus- trate the importance of cognitive wellbeing and inde- pendence in old age and reveal the need for interventions aimed at maintaining a high level of cognitive func- tioning as long as possible. Research following this need
  • 42. faces two challenges: (1) developing effective training interventions that protect or enhance cognitive functions; (2) ensuring that these interventions actually transfer to everyday life and impact quality of life in older adults. The present review aims to discuss and connect existing literature on cognitive training to everyday functioning in older adults. To do so, we focus on a key everyday memory skill, prospective memory, and its application to cognitive training. A. Hering (&) � K. M. Schnitzspahn � M. Kliegel Faculté de Psychologie et Sciences de l’Education, Université de Genève, 40, Boulevard du Pont-d’-Arve, 1211 Geneva, Switzerland e-mail: [email protected] P. G. Rendell Australian Catholic University, Melbourne, Australia N. S. Rose Rotman Research Institute of Baycrest, Toronto, Canada 123
  • 43. Psychological Research DOI 10.1007/s00426-014-0566-4 Author's personal copy Prospective memory and aging Prospective memory describes the ability to plan and successfully execute delayed intentions in the future (e.g., Einstein & McDaniel, 1990; for an overview see Kliegel, McDaniel, & Einstein 2008). Typical prospective memory tasks in everyday life are to take one’s medication at the right time, to call friends on their birthday or to pay bills on time. These examples show how important intact prospective remembering is for an individual’s everyday and social life. Furthermore, studies show that forgetting planned intentions (e.g., forgetting to make a phone call) belongs to the most frequent memory errors in everyday life (Crovitz & Daniel, 1984; Kliegel & Martin, 2003). Moreover, prospective memory is predictive of a wide
  • 44. range of everyday activities in older adults (Woods, Weinborn, Velnoweth, Rooney, & Bucks, 2012). Specif- ically, Wood and colleagues found that prospective memory deficits in older adults were associated with problems and greater self-reported dependence in instru- mental activities of daily living (e.g., housekeeping, shopping). Hence, an intact prospective memory is a crucial factor for maintaining independence and auton- omy. It may therefore be the ideal target for cognitive interventions in old age. This seems to hold especially, because developmental studies have shown that it may be a particular challenge to keep an intact prospective memory with increasing age. Research on aging and prospective memory reveal a gen- eral decline with increasing age in laboratory-based studies compared to young adults (e.g., Altgassen, Kliegel, Brandimonte, & Filippello, 2010; Bisiacchi, Tarantino, & Ciccola, 2008; Einstein, McDaniel, Manzi, Cochran, &
  • 45. Baker, 2000; Park, Hertzog, Kidder, Morrell, & Mayhorn, 1997). Including 26 studies, Henry, MacLeod, Phillips, and Crawford (2004) reported in their meta-analysis on pro- spective memory a substantial age-related deficit (rs = - 0.39 to -0.34) indicating that young adults outperform old adults in laboratory-based prospective memory tasks (see Ihle, Hering, Mahy, Bisiacchi, & Kliegel, 2013; Kliegel, Phillips, & Jäger, 2008 for further meta-analytic evidence confirming those findings). Obviously, the robust age- related decline in older adults raised the question which underlying mechanisms could explain those findings. While there is an ongoing debate over the precise cognitive processes that are associated with age-related decline in prospective memory, there is some consensus that it can partly be linked to the age-related decline of attentional resources and cognitive control (Craik & Byrd, 1982; Kliegel, Ramuschkat, & Martin, 2003; Martin, Kliegel, & McDaniel, 2003; Maylor, Smith, Della Sala, & Logie,
  • 46. 2002; Rose, Rendell, McDaniel, Aberle, & Kliegel, 2010). In particular, age-related decline in working memory, inhibition and shifting has been associated with decline in prospective memory (Gonneaud et al., 2011; Rose et al., 2010; Schnitzspahn, Stahl, Zeintl, Kaller, & Kliegel, 2013). For example, Schnitzspahn et al. (2013) tested 175 young and 110 older adults on a battery of different prospective memory tasks and cognitive tests assessing shifting, updating, inhibition, working memory, and speed. Aiming to disentangle the role of different executive functions on age-related prospective memory performance, the authors found age-related declines in performance on both the prospective memory and executive function measures. Furthermore, they showed that inhibition and shifting play an especially important role in predicting prospective memory performance. Taken together, prospective memory is of high rele- vance for older adults, yet there is an age-related decline in
  • 47. prospective memory performance. As a result, it is not surprising that, recently, there has been increasing interest in training interventions that may have the potential to enhance prospective memory performance in older adults and maybe even prevent age-related declines (McDaniel & Bugg, 2012). However, so far, very little has been pub- lished in this area. The present review, therefore, aims to offer some insights on a possible rationale for future research in the area of prospective memory training. In the following, we briefly outline a distinction made in the available literature on cognitive training in general that will guide our rationale. Cognitive training: general remarks and two ways to go The approach of training cognitive resources is generally motivated by the concept of cognitive plasticity, also described as the malleability of behavior (Greenwood & Parasuraman, 2010; Lövdén, Bäckman, Lindenberger, Schaefer, & Schmiedek, 2010; Willis & Schaie, 2009). In
  • 48. more detail, cognitive plasticity refers to the general potential within a person to change as a function of expe- rience in the sense of improvements (Baltes, 1987). Empirical evidence suggests that cognitive plasticity is still possible in late adulthood (e.g., Carretti, Borella, Zavagnin, & de Beni, 2013; Karbach & Kray, 2009; Singer, Lin- denberger, & Baltes, 2003; Verhaeghen, Marcoen, & Goossens, 1992) and also in very late adulthood in indi- viduals aged over 80 years (e.g., Borella, Carretti, Zanoni, Zavagnin, & De Beni, 2013; Buschkuehl et al., 2008; Fernandez-Ballesteros et al., 2012; Zinke, Zeintl, Eschen, Herzog, & Kliegel, 2012; Zinke et al., 2013). Furthermore, the capability to learn principally remains intact even in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease (e.g., Carretti, Borella, Fostinelli, & Zavagnin, 2013; Fernandez-Ballesteros et al., 2012). Psychological Research 123
  • 49. Author's personal copy Considering the available literature, two main approa- ches exist in the area of cognitive interventions: strategy- oriented training and process-based training. Strategy training aims to compensate or circumvent limitations in underlying processes, while process training aims to aug- ment or to restore the underlying processes (Reichman, Fiocco, & Rose, 2010). Strategy-oriented training interventions typically (but not exclusively) train the use of a mnemonic strategy (e.g., rehearsal, imagery, method of loci) to enhance particular functions as encoding or retrieval from memory in a compensatory way (Morrison & Chein, 2011). Meta-anal- yses by Verhaeghen et al. (1992) and Gross et al. (2012) show substantial effect size for pre-post change scores, indicating large benefits for the training groups compared to control groups. However, these training gains are mostly
  • 50. limited to the training task itself. Although it remains questionable if transfer of training improvements to other new and untrained tasks is possible (McDaniel & Bugg, 2012; Morrison & Chein, 2011), there is some promising positive evidence (for benefits to working memory see, e.g., Carretti, Borella, & De Beni, 2007; for benefits to everyday activities see, e.g., Cavallini, Pagnin, & Vecchi, 2003). In any case, memory strategies have important practical value. They are comparably easy to teach, train and to use in everyday life. However, the scientific chal- lenge is to find the appropriate strategy targeting the rele- vant cognitive difficulties in older adults’ everyday life. For example, as discussed by McDaniel and Bugg (2012), instructing older adults to use a mnemonic strategy to learn a list of items (e.g., grocery list) is fairly easy to do, but might not be used in everyday life because of other com- pensational strategies or technical devices (e.g., using mobile phone to record a list). Furthermore, other critical
  • 51. aspects that influence possible transfer need to be taken into account, such as the role of homework, the training material, and the transfer tasks (McDaniel & Bugg, 2012). A possibility to extend the benefits of strategy-oriented training could be to combine strategy use with additional metacognitive interventions. For instance, Cavallini, Dun- losky, Bottiroli, Hertzog, and Vecchi (2010) found greater transfer effects in older adults when teaching them mne- monic strategies combined with an instruction phase on how to use these strategies for other tasks. Participants learned two strategies: imagery and sentence generation to learn paired associates and lists of words. One half of the group received additional instructions on how they could use the strategies for other tasks besides the training including two of the transfer tasks; the other half performed only the training. Transfer was measured for four different tasks, two of them were mentioned as examples on how the trained strategies could be used for them, and the other two
  • 52. transfer tasks were not mentioned. One major result showed transfer effects to a non-mentioned transfer task (text learning), which were greater for the instruction phase group than the training only group (see also Craik & Rose, 2012). Process-based training regimes (sometimes called restorative training) aim to improve a particular cognitive ability (e.g., working memory) by repetitively exercising the underlying core mechanisms (e.g., updating) in a cog- nitively intensive way, typically by adaptively increasing the difficulty based on the participant’s individual perfor- mance level (Morrison & Chein, 2011). The rationale behind process-based training is the assumption that the improvement in central or core neuro-cognitive resources should also stimulate benefits in associated cognitive functions (e.g., planning, intelligence). Therefore, the out- come of process-based training should result in broader effects such as near and far transfer.
  • 53. Importantly for present purposes, what is often criticized in the training literature is the lack of everyday transfer of such process-based training regimes (Craik & Rose, 2012; McDaniel & Bugg, 2012). There exist a few studies tar- geting this issue so far; however, some promising results were found in self-reported, but not objective measures of everyday functioning (Brehmer, Westerberg, & Bäckman, 2012; Willis et al., 2006) or in reading comprehension (e.g., Chein & Morrison, 2010; Loosli, Buschkuehl, Perrig, & Jaeggi, 2012). For example, Brehmer et al. (2012) investigated the effectiveness of a computerized adaptive working memory training compared to a control training. The control group worked with the same task material but practiced on a low level of difficulty throughout the intervention. The training program was applied to younger and older adults for 5 weeks. Besides near and far cogni- tive transfer measures, they also included a self-rating scale for subjective cognitive functioning in everyday life. The
  • 54. training led to near and far transfer effects for both age groups. More interestingly, the results showed that both training groups reduced their subjective memory com- plaints more than the active control groups and that this benefit was even maintained across a follow-up interval of 3 months. A limitation of this study is that this everyday transfer is restricted to only self-report improvements. Thus, the question remains if this subjective impression holds true on an objective performance level. Trainability of prospective memory Taken together, to this date, evidence of far transfer effects as well as everyday transfer is limited, for both strategy- and process-based training approaches. In this context, McDaniel and Bugg (2012) suggested that this may partly be due to a lack of theory-driven hypotheses for transfer. Psychological Research 123 Author's personal copy
  • 55. For example, the authors criticized memory training interventions that train older adults to perform a strategy or task in the lab, but fail to generalize the training to everyday memory. In their opinion, training should be training for transfer that rests on clear conceptual predic- tions on the possible overlap between training and transfer tasks, particularly with respect to transfer to everyday functioning. Specifically, they suggested that training pro- grams should aim for a better fit between the trained strategies or processes in the lab and their application in everyday life. Authors argued that it is important to iden- tify everyday problems and develop and train strategies and processes that are realistically applicable and useful for those problems in everyday life. As stated in the beginning of this article, prospective memory can be seen as such a target of cognitive interventions (c.f., Kliegel & Bürki, 2012). Also, from a more fundamental cognitive training
  • 56. perspective, prospective memory may be considered an interesting target, as it is associated to executive functions such as task switching and working memory on the one hand and to episodic memory demands on the other hand. As stated earlier, available evidence shows that these associated processes seem to be trainable. In conclusion, this should also be the case for prospective memory. To guide such interventions, Kliegel, Altgassen, Hering, and Rose (2011) recently proposed a framework for a theory-driven training approach in the area of prospective memory. Specifically, Kliegel et al. (2011) recommended disentangling the process of prospective remembering by its different phases to identify the critical components of prospective memory in need of training in a given popu- lation. Remembering delayed intentions at the appropriate moment in the future is conceptualized as a four-phase process (Ellis, 1996; Kliegel, Martin, McDaniel, & Ein- stein, 2002; Kliegel, McDaniel, & Einstein, 2000). In the
  • 57. first phase—intention formation—the individual has to formulate and plan the intention. This first phase relies mainly on encoding and planning processes (Kliegel et al., 2002; Kliegel et al., 2000). After successfully forming an intention, there is the intention retention phase. Typically, the individual performs other ‘‘ongoing’’ tasks during a delay. This temporal delay until the initiation of the intention can vary in its length from a few minutes (e.g., to remember to take the pizza out of the oven) up to several days or months (e.g., to remember to pay a monthly bill). During this phase, the intention is maintained in long-term memory. Important for this phase is a more or less elabo- rate representation of the intended action (e.g., a strong association between intention and content; Marsh, Hicks, Cook, Hansen, & Pallos, 2003)—to facilitate later memory retrieval; and working memory capacity (Martin & Schu- mann-Hengsteler, 2001) since the intention may need to be refreshed or updated in memory while being engaged in
  • 58. other activities. The last two phases refer to the intention initiation and the intention execution. The individual has to detect the appropriate moment indicated by the prospective memory cue (event-based prospective memory) or after an elapsed time (time-based prospective memory) and retrieve the intention from memory to successfully execute it. Although these two phases are sometimes considered together as they occur close in real time, Kliegel et al. (2002) describe fine conceptual differences. The intention initiation phase involves the initiation of the planned intention at the appropriate moment. The involved under- lying executive processes include monitoring (to detect the appropriate situation by noticing the cue or monitoring the time), cognitive flexibility (to switch between ongoing and intended activity), and inhibition (to inhibit the ongoing activity). The intention execution phase refers to the completion of intentions and depends also on cognitive flexibility. There are two possible outcomes: the correctly
  • 59. retrieved intention could be executed correctly or an incorrect action could be executed. Performance is influ- enced by the previously formed plan and situational factors that occur while performing the intended tasks. Besides the relevance of these cognitive functions for each phase of prospective remembering, non-cognitive factors such as metacognitive abilities play a role in prospective memory performance as well (e.g., Kliegel, Altgassen, Hering, & Rose, 2011; Meeks, Hicks, & Marsh, 2007; Schnitzspahn, Zeintl, Jaeger, & Kliegel, 2011). To which extent someone rates his or her ability to remember intentions as high or low might also influence which strategies and how many cognitive resources were allocated to the execution of the intentions. A recent study by Rummel and Meiser (2013) in young adults showed that the allocation of attention and monitoring in a prospective memory task was influenced by the metacognitive beliefs about the task demands. To conclude, successful prospective memory depends
  • 60. on retrospective (episodic) memory processes (e.g., encoding, retrieval), as well as controlled attention/execu- tive processes associated with planning, working memory, and task switching. Given this theoretical process-based framework, a more tailor-made approach could be applied to the development of training programs where different training aspects fit to the different cognitive requirements that underlie prospective remembering (Kliegel & Bürki, 2012). But what is actually known about the malleability of prospective memory? So far, there is only a small literature on prospective memory training (Fleming, Shum, Strong, & Lightbody, 2005; Kinsella et al., 2009; Radford, Lah, Thayer, Say, & Miller, 2012; Raskin & Sohlberg, 1996; Shum, Fleming, Gill, Gullo, & Strong, 2011), which focuses on clinical samples only [e.g., traumatic brain injury (TBI), mild cognitive impairment] and implements interventions more Psychological Research
  • 61. 123 Author's personal copy in a neuropsychological rehabilitation setting (for a reviews see Fish, Wilson, & Manly, 2010; Piras, Borella, Incoccia, & Carlesimo, 2011). While most of the studies investigate small sample groups or single cases, a study by Shum et al. (2011) used a more systematic approach. The authors compared four different training regimes. TBI patients (aged between 18 and 60 years) participated in each of the four training groups. All training regimes consisted of two parts. The first part of the training comprised either 2 weeks of self-awareness training for prospective memory or 2 weeks of a control condition (for details see below). The second part of the training consisted of either 6 weeks of a so-called compensatory prospective memory training or 6 weeks of a so-called remedial prospective memory training. Combining the two versions of the first part with
  • 62. the two versions of the second part formed the four training groups (i.e., self-awareness ? compensatory training; control ? compensatory training; self-awareness ? reme- dial training; control ? remedial training). The self- awareness training introduced the concept of prospective memory and exercised self-prediction and monitoring of performance on various prospective memory tasks. The corresponding control training contained the discussion of autobiographical memories and life events as well as education about attention and concentration. The com- pensatory prospective memory training aimed to train strategies to compensate for prospective memory problems in daily life such as training of note-taking and maximizing the use of diaries and organizational devices. The remedial training aimed to restore lost functions. In the case of prospective memory, training involved increasing the time delay for remembering to perform the prospective memory tasks continuously. First, participants’ initial prospective
  • 63. memory capacity (i.e., the longest time period for which they could successfully remember the given intentions) was assessed. During the training, the time delay between instruction of the prospective memory task and the appropriate moment to execute the task was increased always by 1 min for the next task if they performed the task successfully. Outcome measures for all training groups were performance on a standardized prospective memory test, a questionnaire to assess the frequency of prospective memory lapses in everyday life, and a rating of everyday functioning by a relative of the training participant. These measures were assessed before and after the training per- iod. Results showed that only both compensatory training groups profited from the training showing larger change scores for the standardized prospective memory test. In contrast, there were found no training effects for the remedial training. Furthermore, the groups with the self- awareness training did not differ concerning their training
  • 64. gains from those without self-awareness training. There were neither effects on the questionnaire nor the everyday functioning ratings. Taken together, results are promising, as they show that patients can benefit from compensatory prospective memory training. However, the studies repor- ted so far did not include far transfer measures or examine healthy individuals. Further research is clearly needed to examine training effects in healthy older adults using big- ger samples and to investigate prospective memory inter- ventions from a more theory-driven perspective. In the following sections, we present some evidence from our own work examining the possibilities for prospective memory training following the distinction of strategy and process-based training outlined above. Strategy-oriented prospective memory training In the following section, we will discuss external and internal strategy trainings on prospective memory. External strategy trainings comprise the use of external memory
  • 65. aids such as clocks and phones. Several studies used technological devices as external reminders to overcome problems with self-initiated retrieval (e.g., setting an alarm for an appointment). As already indicated in the study by Shum et al. (2011) and also investigated in other studies in rehabilitation settings, electronic devices indeed supported successfully remembering delayed intentions (for mobile phones and smartphones e.g., DePompei et al., 2008; Thöne-Otto & Walther, 2003; Wade & Troy, 2001; for pager e.g., Wilson, Emslie, Quirk, & Evans, 2001; Wilson, Evans, Emslie, & Malinek, 1997). For example, Thöne- Otto and Walther (2003) compared the efficacy of two electronic devices, mobile phone and palm organizer in 12 brain injured patients. First, participants received some naturalistic, experimenter-given tasks to perform without an electronic device and they should send back a list with all everyday intentions they forgot to the experimenter. In this first phase, participants learned also, how they could
  • 66. use one of the electronic devices. In the second phase, the experimenter gave some naturalistic prospective memory tasks that were already entered in the device and partici- pants could enter their everyday intentions themselves. Again, they had to send back the list of forgotten inten- tions. Phase 3 and 4 repeated this procedure but for the second device. Results revealed that participants forgot fewer intentions when using the electronic devices and this was especially pronounced for their self-entered everyday tasks. Although electronic devices do not improve pro- spective memory capacity per se, they help to keep or regain a sufficient level of prospective memory perfor- mance from an outcome perspective. Considering the process-model of prospective memory, internal strategies should play a beneficial role, especially for the first phases (intention formation, intention Psychological Research 123
  • 67. Author's personal copy retention), which rely on planning abilities, encoding and memory storage. There is some empirical evidence from cross-sectional designs comparing the effectiveness of different planning and encoding strategies on later pro- spective memory performance in young and older adults (Brom et al., 2013; Henry, Rendell, Phillips, Dunlop, & Kliegel, 2012; Kliegel, Martin, McDaniel, Einstein, & Moor, 2007; Liu & Park, 2004; Park, Gutchess, Meade, & Stine-Morrow, 2007). Kliegel et al. (2007) found that older adults’ prospective memory performance benefited from different planning aids (e.g., structuring of plans), which facilitated intention initiation. The authors used the modified six-elements task as a complex prospective memory task. The task is a delayed multi-tasking paradigm where participants have to plan the order how they intend to initiate and execute the
  • 68. six delayed tasks while respecting the time limit and other constraining rules concerning the order of the subtasks (Kliegel et al., 2002; Kliegel et al., 2000). To be successful in the task, it is necessary to plan how to proceed, to initiate the tasks appropriately, and to switch between the tasks according to the rules. First, participants received the instruction and had to plan how to perform the six subtasks, which was followed by a delay period with filler tasks. Finally, participants had to self-initiate the execution of the modified six-elements task. Studies using this paradigm show that older adults tend to make less detailed plans compared to young adults and that a less detailed plan is associated with impairment of later realization of delayed intentions (Kliegel et al., 2002; Kliegel et al., 2000). To overcome this performance deficit in older adults, Kliegel et al. (2007) provided different planning aids to foster different aspects of the planning process of the task in an attempt to improve later prospective memory per-
  • 69. formance. The authors investigated young and old adults and compared the effectiveness of planning aids with a no- aid condition in both age groups. The aids helped partici- pants to plan in three ways. First, participants were asked to include information about the appropriate moment to ini- tially start the task (self-initiation of intention). Second, participants were asked to include specific cues in their plans that described when and how they wanted to switch between tasks (task switching). Third, a general planning aid helped participants structure and schedule the tasks appropriately (planning). To do so, participants received a flow chart to help plan the order of the six subtasks in advance. In the first two experiments of the study, partic- ipants received either a combination of the first and second planning aid or no planning aid. The results showed that participants’ prospective memory performance benefited from planning aids. Both age groups in the planning aid condition were more likely to initiate the six-elements task
  • 70. at the appropriate moment compared to participants in the no-aid condition. Furthermore, older adults also profited from the planning aid that benefited their task switching between the subtasks. In the third experiment, the authors compared the effect of three conditions in young and old adults: no planning aid versus the general planning aid versus a combined version of the general and the switching aid. Results showed no influence on self-initiation of intentions (1) but the combined aid of structuring the plans (3) and encouraging task switching (2) improved realiza- tion of delayed intentions with a greater effect for old adults than young adults. A recent study on the influence of reminders on pro- spective memory performance also shows the benefits of compensatory approaches for older and younger adults (Henry et al., 2012). Henry et al. (2012) found that pro- spective memory performance of both age groups could be enhanced using reminders either self-initiated or presented
  • 71. by the experimenter. In sum, the findings show that already simpler internal strategies, such as basic planning strategies or provision of reminders, could be a useful approach to foster prospective memory. A more complex strategy successfully used in prospec- tive memory research is implementation intentions (e.g., Chasteen, Park, & Schwarz, 2001; Liu & Park, 2004; McFarland & Glisky, 2011; Park et al., 2007; Rummel, Einstein, & Rampey, 2012; Schnitzspahn & Kliegel, 2009; Zimmermann & Meier, 2010). The implementation inten- tions strategy is a goal-directed verbalization of intentions in a ‘‘If x arises, then I will perform y’’ manner (Gollwitzer, 1993, 1999). It is assumed that formulating implementation intentions creates a strong connection between the situation in the ‘‘if’’-part and the intended action formulated in the ‘‘then’’-part. When the specific situation is encountered, the planned action is automatically activated and, therefore, more easily retrievable. When applied to prospective
  • 72. memory, there is promising evidence showing that older adults’ prospective memory benefits from formulating implementation intentions (e.g., Chasteen et al., 2001; Schnitzspahn & Kliegel, 2009; Zimmermann & Meier, 2010). Interestingly, Zimmermann and Meier (2010) found the beneficial effect of implementation intentions only for the older adults’ group but not in younger adults or ado- lescents. Thus, implementation intentions seem to be especially useful in older adults but the explaining mech- anisms are still under debate. Two hypotheses are currently discussed. One assumption is that implementation inten- tions should improve the encoding of prospective inten- tions, thereby reducing the need for strategic monitoring. As a result, more automatic detection of the prospective memory cue as well as a more spontaneous retrieval of the intention should occur (Gollwitzer, 1999; McDaniel & Scullin, 2010; Rummel et al., 2012). Alternatively, it has also been argued that implementation intentions strengthen
  • 73. Psychological Research 123 Author's personal copy the perceived importance of the intention and foster more monitoring processes at the costs of the ongoing task (Meeks & Marsh, 2010). We recently conducted a study investigating the effect of implementation intentions in performing health behav- iors in older adults (Brom et al., 2013). The aims of the study were to investigate the effectiveness of implemen- tation intentions in a real-life prospective memory task and to examine possible moderators (e.g., fluid intelligence, conscientiousness) of the effectiveness of the strategy. The study follows up on results by Liu and Park (2004) who also examined the beneficial influence of implementation intentions on prospective memory in health behavior. Liu and Park used implementation intentions to enhance glu-
  • 74. cose monitoring but in a small sample of older participants (n = 10). We decided to use an alternative everyday health task that could be of importance for older adults: remem- bering to measure one’s blood pressure at three times per day for five consecutive days of a week. The treatment consisted of one session where participants were first instructed on how to use a blood pressure monitor and then they were trained to formulate the implementation inten- tion about how they were going to control their blood pressure. Furthermore, standardized tests and question- naires were used to assess cognitive resources (e.g., fluid intelligence) as well as other possible environmental (e.g., everyday stress) and motivational moderators (e.g., self- efficacy). In total, 39 healthy older adults with a mean age of 68.8 years (SD = 4.99) who had no experience with monitoring blood pressure were included in the study. Participants were randomly assigned to two treatment
  • 75. groups: implementation intention instruction (n = 19) and a control instruction (n = 20). The groups did not differ in age or intelligence. Participants in the implementation intention condition were asked to think of specific times when they could perform the blood pressure monitoring, where it will take place, and how they will do it. They had to write down all this information on how to perform the blood pressure monitoring during the next 5 days. In a second step, participants transformed this information through the use of the implementation intentions strategy using prepared worksheets (e.g., ‘‘If I am in the living room next week from Monday to Friday at 8 a.m., 12 p.m. and 6 p.m., then I will check my blood pressure with the blood pressure monitor’’). In a last step, the participants were asked to mentally visualize how they would enact their intentions. Participants in the control group were asked to write down the times when they wanted to check their blood pressure and read an article about blood pressure to
  • 76. ensure equal treatment durations. The dependent variable was the rate of forgotten blood pressure tests. Comparing the implementation intention group with the control group, a significant main effect of treatment of large effect size emerged. Participants using implementation intentions forgot to check their blood pressure at the intended times considerably less often than participants of the control group. In fact, participants in the implementation intentions training group performed almost perfectly. Furthermore, and extending previous work by Liu and Park (2004), there was a significant interaction between the treatment group and level of fluid intelligence (based on a median split in high and low fluid intelligence). Control participants with a lower level of fluid intelligence forgot more often to check their blood pressure than par- ticipants with higher fluid intelligence; however, for the implementation intention group, there was no difference between those with low or high fluid intelligence.
  • 77. These results provide an example that older adults’ prospective memory can benefit from a strategy interven- tion. More precisely, the moderation effect indicates that implementation intentions benefited prospective remem- bering by compensating for low fluid intelligence. Fur- thermore, the study shows the effectiveness of a strategy intervention for an everyday health behavior that is highly relevant for many older adults. However, one limitation of the study is that maintenance effects were not assessed. It would be of interest to know, if participants applied the learned strategy also on other everyday behavior. In terms of future directions, it should be acknowledged that, although the implementation intention effect in the study by Brom et al. (2013) was strong, the strategy was very specific to the task that should benefit from it. It would be of conceptual and applied interest to promote prospective memory also in a more general way and to test transfer to other prospective memory tasks as well as related cognitive
  • 78. abilities. In the next section, we give an example for a process-based approach to train prospective memory that investigated the possibility of transfer from prospective memory training. Process-based prospective memory training The training described below (Rose et al., 2012) was based on the Virtual Week paradigm (Rendell & Craik, 2000). Virtual Week is a computerized laboratory prospective memory task in the style of a board game but with everyday life content to increase ecological validity. Par- ticipants roll a dice and move a token around a board that symbolizes a virtual day from 7 a.m. to 10 p.m. During this fictitious day, participants pass several event squares that represent different everyday events (e.g., breakfast, shop- ping, going to the library, dinner). For each of these events, participants have to make choices about how the event would end and what they would do in real life (e.g., Psychological Research
  • 79. 123 Author's personal copy deciding what to eat for breakfast or deciding what to buy in the shopping mall). Two clocks are displayed in the middle of the board, one indicating the virtual day time (calibrated to the token position on the board), the other displaying a stop clock that measures the actual play time in minutes and seconds. The prospective memory tasks are embedded in the story of the game. At the beginning, participants learn different health tasks, which they have to remember to simulate performing over the course of the whole game. For example, participants have to remember to take anti- biotics each virtual day at breakfast and dinner events or they have to remember to use an asthma inhaler at 11 a.m. and 9 p.m. These health-related tasks have to be performed regularly—that is, on every virtual day (i.e., one circuit of
  • 80. the board). There are also irregular prospective memory tasks that are instructed for the current virtual day (e.g., pick up dry cleaning at the shopping mall, return book at the library, call the restaurant for a dinner reservation at 5 p.m.). These tasks have to be encoded at the beginning of each new virtual day and during the day. The third type of tasks are stop-clock tasks, in which participants have to perform a certain task after a specific time has elapsed, for example, testing blood sugar when the stop clock reaches 2 and 4 min. Participants are not obliged to perform the regular, irregular and stop-clock prospective memory tasks in real life. They are to simulate performing the task at the appropriate moments, either time-cued or event-cued, by clicking a ‘‘perform task’’ button and selecting the right task from a list. In the original version by Rendell and Craik (2000), participants had to perform 10 tasks per virtual day for 7 virtual days indicating Monday to Sunday. Previous research has demonstrated that Virtual Week is
  • 81. a useful and reliable tool for prospective memory research with good psychometric properties (Rendell & Henry, 2009). The different task types vary in their demands requiring automatic or strategic processing and working memory abilities (McDaniel & Einstein, 2000; Rose et al., 2010). The distinction between regular and irregular tasks reflects low versus high retrospective memory demands (Foster, Rose, McDaniel, & Rendell, 2013; Mioni, Rendell, Henry, Cantagallo, & Stablum, 2013). Differences based on the cue of the prospective memory task (event-based, time-based and stop clock) result in varying demands on monitoring processes, with time-based and stop-clock tasks being more demanding as they require checking the clocks, whereas event-based tasks require attending to the events which pop up when the participant passes an event-square (Rendell & Henry, 2009; Rose et al., 2010). Furthermore, Rose et al. (2010) showed that individual differences in working memory were predictive of prospective memory
  • 82. performance in the Virtual Week game, especially for high-demanding tasks. Moreover, Virtual Week is an age- sensitive task: Research on aging shows that older adults perform worse compared to young adults for the irregular tasks compared to regular tasks (Aberle, Rendell, Rose, McDaniel, & Kliegel, 2010; Rendell & Craik, 2000; Rose et al., 2010) and also for time-based tasks compared to event-based tasks (e.g., Henry et al., 2012; Rendell et al., 2011). In the training protocol, we used the original Virtual Week paradigm and extended it to a process-based training version to examine whether older adults’ prospective memory could be improved and if training gains could transfer to other cognitive and everyday life tasks (Rose et al., 2012). The study consisted of three experimental groups, one group performed the Virtual Week training; the second group, an active control group, performed a music training; the third group was a waitlist, no-contact control
  • 83. group. In total, 50 older adults between 60 and 80 years participated in the study. All participants performed a pre- and post-testing ses- sion using a battery of established prospective memory measures, neuropsychological tasks and everyday compe- tence tests. We also developed and assessed performance on a novel real-life prospective memory task—the ‘‘call- back task’’ (Rose et al., 2012). While participants were at home engaged in their daily activities, they received a phone call from an experimenter and they were to call the experimenter back in exactly 10, 25, 35 and 40 min. For everyday competence, we administered the timed version of instrumental activities of daily living (Owsley, Sloane, McGwin, & Ball, 2002). In this standard measure of instrumental activities of daily living, participants had to perform short everyday tasks as fast as possible (e.g., counting money, reading out loud ingredients, finding telephone numbers in a phone book). Performance is typ-
  • 84. ically used to assess one‘s fitness for independent living. Between the pre- and post-test there was a delay of 4 weeks. Participants in the no-contact control group per- formed only the pre- and the post-testing. Participants in the active control group received fifteen 1-h music lessons over 4 weeks. Subjects in the Virtual Week training group participated in twelve 1-h training sessions, three times per week for 4 weeks on the prospective memory training. The Virtual Week training consisted of 24 different levels. One level corresponded to one virtual day that was one circuit of the board. To implement a process-based training design, the levels increased in difficulty in an adaptive manner. We varied the number of tasks to train the retro- spective component. The prospective component was also trained by changing the cues across the training and dis- playing visible but also hidden stop clocks for the stop- clock tasks to put increasing demands on cue detection, monitoring, and cognitive flexibility. Participants had to
  • 85. play two virtual days per training session. After each level, Psychological Research 123 Author's personal copy they received feedback about their performance. Partici- pants saw whether they performed each task correctly or not. They had to achieve at least 75 % correct to pass the level or else they had to repeat and practice the day with the same events. However, the game routine differed depending on the rolling of the dice. After each week of training participants filled in a questionnaire on how they performed the different types of tasks during the game. Over the course of the training, participants increased their performance while the numbers of day repetitions decreased. Pre-post comparisons indicated a training ben- efit for the prospective memory training group in the ori- ginal Virtual Week task compared to the control groups.
  • 86. Likewise, participants in the Virtual Week training group tended to show greater improvement in the real-life pro- spective memory task, the call-back task, after the training compared to both control groups, hinting at some near transfer to performing prospective memory tasks in the real world. Furthermore, participants in the Virtual Week training group showed a greater pre- to post-test improvement in the timed instrumental activities of daily living after the training compared to both control groups, indicating far transfer. Qualitative analyses of the post- training questionnaires further indicated that participants used either no or poor strategies (e.g., ‘‘concentrate’’) at the beginning of the training, but some improved to using better strategies (e.g., ‘‘visualize’’) over the course of the training. Taken together, Rose et al. (2012) showed that older adults could improve their prospective memory per- formance on the Virtual Week paradigm relative to con- trols. Furthermore, there were some trends for near and far
  • 87. transfer to everyday naturalistic tasks relative to controls. Prospective memory training among cognitive training interventions The examples discussed above show that there is evidence that prospective memory is a suitable candidate for both strategy-oriented and process-based training approaches. However, prospective memory is also a construct that breaks ranks among the other target cognitive abilities in previous training research. The focus of process-based training in the literature lays especially on working mem- ory. The typical training of working memory targets basic cognitive processes constituting working memory by very specific tasks. For example, participants have to perform visuospatial span tasks or n-back tasks with increasing difficulty in a repetitive way (e.g., Jaeggi, Buschkuehl, Jonides, & Perrig, 2008; Klingberg, Forssberg, & Wester- berg, 2002). Also, verbal working memory trainings showed promising effects (Borella, Carretti, Riboldi, & De
  • 88. Beni, 2010; Borella et al., 2013; Carretti, Borella, Fostinelli, & Zavagnin, 2013). The question resulting from this approach is if it is really possible to bridge the gap between the highly specific tasks used in training inter- ventions to improvements in everyday life for older adults (e.g., making financial decisions, health behavior, house- keeping). If cognitive training is intended to improve older adults’ cognition and increase their independence and wellbeing, then this should be the primary aim. In our opinion, training of prospective memory could actually connect the applied perspective of an attempt to improve everyday life functioning in older adults with the basic research on plasticity and its process-based approach. Of course, this is an empirical question and future research will have to systematically test this proposition. However, so far, only a few studies have examined the trainability of prospective memory. We discussed exam- ples for both training approaches that showed encouraging
  • 89. results for this line of research. Taken together, we dem- onstrated the effectiveness of a strategy-oriented approach to improve prospective memory in everyday life (Brom et al., 2013). The second example showed the possibility to train prospective memory with a process-based training regime (Rose et al., 2012). This initial evidence leads to further important research questions: how can we maxi- mize benefits of prospective memory training and how can we ensure robust transfer to the daily requirements of older adults? As discussed previously, each of the two training approaches has its strengths and weaknesses. For training prospective memory, one possibility might be to combine both approaches and perhaps profit from the advantages of each approach and to overcome some of their limitations. For example, in future studies both trainings could be combined. The strategy of implementation intentions could be implemented in the Virtual Week training as well. The training of implementation intentions on the realistic con-
  • 90. tent of Virtual Week could stimulate transfer to everyday tasks of the participants. On the other hand, the adaptive training of prospective memory abilities could be boostered by the strategy. Another route for future research may be to consider the benefits of strategies that help ‘‘train for transfer’’. For example, if we aim to improve older adults’ everyday functioning and wellbeing (Craik & Rose, 2012), training the use of strategies for varying stimuli and situ- ations over the course of the intervention could help achieve transfer effects of the training regimes applied. Another crucial point within this discussion is the aim for long-term effects. When improving cognitive functioning by training interventions, the second aim, besides reliable transfer, should be the maintenance of these effects (e.g., Borella et al., 2013; Brehmer et al., 2012; Li et al., 2008; Zinke et al., 2013). For instance, Zinke et al. (2013) found that their working memory training for older adults pro- duced stable training effects as well as near transfer effects
  • 91. Psychological Research 123 Author's personal copy at a 9-month follow-up assessment. However, the results of Melby-Lervag and Hulme (2013) were more sobering. In their meta-analysis, they found long-term effects for working memory training on visual working memory tasks, but not on verbal tasks or far transfer measures (e.g., word reading, attention, reasoning). One main problem of eval- uating long-term effects rests on the fact that the majority of training studies did not include follow-up assessments. Further training research is clearly needed to determine factors responsible for maintenance effects. Moreover, a limitation of the meta-analysis for the present purpose is, that only a few training studies on older adults were included. The characterization of prospective memory as a multi-
  • 92. phase process (Kliegel et al., 2002; Kliegel et al., 2000) provides several different targets for combined cognitive interventions. Compensational strategies like implementa- tion intentions are especially helpful for encoding inten- tions and should, therefore, be especially helpful when deficits are likely to be due to the intention formation phase (e.g., such as in Parkinson patients, see Kliegel et al., 2011). In terms of transfer, implementation inten- tions seem to be fruitful as they are a rather general strategy with a broad range of possible applications com- pared to the typical mnemonic strategies that tend to be more specific to retrieval processes (e.g., method of loci). Complementary, traditional process-based training regimes may be especially indicated when the improvement of cognitive components underlying prospective memory is the key target of the intervention. For example, training of working memory or inhibition should reduce deficit levels in these resources (training improvement) and subse-
  • 93. quently drive improvements in those prospective memory phases where they are most involved (i.e., intention initi- ation and execution; far transfer). Finally, training all phases of the prospective memory process at once with a tool such as the Virtual Week game actually provides a learning environment to apply the principles of training, stabilize training gains, and possibly stimulate transfer to other cognitive and everyday life functions. In addition, prospective memory could also be a suitable candidate for a multi-domain training approach. Multi-domain training targets several cognitive domains as an ensemble, for example, simultaneously training memory, reasoning, and attention in one training regime (e.g., Cheng et al., 2012; Oswald, Gunzelmann, Rupprecht, & Hagen, 2006). Whereas single-domain training targets one cognitive function, such as working memory or set shifting. In fact, prospective memory is itself a multi-phase, multi-domain process that is closely related to executive functions and
  • 94. memory. For instance, prospective memory tasks by defi- nition target a full set of functions, such as retrospective memory (for the intention content), monitoring (for the cues) or inhibition (of the ongoing task before initiating the prospective memory task). For example, in a possible training, the episodic memory component could be adap- tively increased in difficulty by systemically increasing number, length and/or complexity of the to-be-executed intention and at the same time the difficulty to find the cue and the inhibitory demands of interrupting the ongoing task may be adapted by more and more hiding the cue in the ongoing task and increasing its task demands, respec- tively. Therefore, using prospective memory tasks as training targets could prove to be a fruitful variant of recent suggestions to use multi-domain training interven- tions in contrast to single-domain regimes (see also Insel, Einstein, Morrow, & Hepworth, 2013, for a similar pro- posal; for related conceptualizations in skill acquisition see
  • 95. the review by Schmidt & Bjork, 1992). Cheng et al. (2012) found that both training approaches, single- and multi- domain training, showed immediate training gains. Fur- thermore, they demonstrated that multi-domain training especially has advantages for maintenance effects of training. Although they included booster sessions after 6 months for both trainings, Cheng et al. showed better long-term effects up to 12 months for multi-domain training in healthy older adults. To conclude on the presented literature, a tailor-made approach of a holistic prospective memory training com- bining specific strategy- and process-based components could be of interest for further research. The combination of these two general training traditions in prospective memory training could provide the possibility to not only target one but most of the corresponding processes of prospective memory as an ensemble. Therefore, future studies running this combined approach could be promising
  • 96. attempts to ensure improvements of functioning and well- being in older adults’ everyday life through prospective memory training. Acknowledgments The preparation of this article was supported by a grant by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG). References Aberle, I., Rendell, P. G., Rose, N. S., McDaniel, M. A., & Kliegel, M. (2010). The age prospective memory paradox: young adults may not give their best outside of the lab. Developmental Psychology, 46(6), 1444–1453. doi:10.1037/A0020718. Altgassen, M., Kliegel, M., Brandimonte, M., & Filippello, P. (2010). Are older adults more social than younger adults? social importance increases older adults’ prospective memory perfor- mance. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 17(3), 312–328. doi:10.1080/13825580903281308. Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span develop-
  • 97. mental-psychology—on the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 611–626. doi:10. 1037/0012-1649.23.5.611. Psychological Research 123 Author's personal copy http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0020718 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13825580903281308 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.611 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.5.611 Bisiacchi, P. S., Tarantino, V., & Ciccola, A. (2008). Aging and prospective memory: the role of working memory and monitor- ing processes. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 20(6), 569–577. Borella, E., Carretti, B., Riboldi, F., & De Beni, R. (2010). Working memory training in older adults evidence of transfer and maintenance effects. Psychology and Aging, 25(4), 767–778. doi:10.1037/A0020683.
  • 98. Borella, E., Carretti, B., Zanoni, G., Zavagnin, M., & De Beni, R. (2013). Working memory training in old age: an examination of transfer and maintenance effects. Archives of Clinical Neuro- psychology, 28(4), 331–347. doi:10.1093/Arclin/Act020. Brehmer, Y., Westerberg, H., & Bäckman, L. (2012). Working- memory training in younger and older adults: training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6:63. doi:10.3389/Fnhum.2012.00063. Brom, S. S., Schnitzspahn, K. M., Melzer, M., Hagner, F., Bernhard, A., & Kliegel, M. (2013). Fluid mechanics moderate the effect of implementation intentions on a health prospective memory task in older adults. European Journal of Ageing, 19, 1–10. doi:10. 1007/s10433-013-0288-2. Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., Hutchison, S., Perrig-Chiello, P., Däpp, C., Müller, M., et al. (2008). Impact of working memory training on memory performance in old–old adults. Psychology
  • 99. and Aging, 23(4), 743–753. doi:10.1037/A0014342. Carretti, B., Borella, E., & De Beni, R. (2007). Does strategic memory training improve the working memory performance of younger and older adults? Experimental Psychology, 54(4), 311–320. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.311. Carretti, B., Borella, E., Fostinelli, S., & Zavagnin, M. (2013a). Benefits of training working memory in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: specific and transfer effects. International Psych- ogeriatrics, 25(4), 617–626. doi:10.1017/S1041610212002177. Carretti, B., Borella, E., Zavagnin, M., & de Beni, R. (2013b). Gains in language comprehension relating to working memory training in healthy older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 28(5), 539–546. doi:10.1002/gps.3859. Cavallini, E., Dunlosky, J., Bottiroli, S., Hertzog, C., & Vecchi, T. (2010). Promoting transfer in memory training for older adults. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research, 22(4), 314–323. doi:10.3275/6704.
  • 100. Cavallini, E., Pagnin, A., & Vecchi, T. (2003). Aging and everyday memory: the beneficial effect of memory training. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 37(3), 241–257. doi:10.1016/ S0167-4943(03)00063-3. Chasteen, A. L., Park, D. C., & Schwarz, N. (2001). Implementation intentions and facilitation of prospective memory. Psychological Science, 12(6), 457–461. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00385. Chein, J. M., & Morrison, A. B. (2010). Expanding the mind’s workspace: training and transfer effects with a complex working memory span task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(2), 193–199. doi:10.3758/Pbr.17.2.193. Cheng, Y., Wu, W. Y., Feng, W., Wang, J. Q., Chen, Y., Shen, Y., et al. (2012). The effects of multi-domain versus single-domain cognitive training in non-demented older people: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Medicine, 10:30. doi:10.1186/1741-7015- 10-30.
  • 101. Craik, F. I. M., & Byrd, M. (1982). Aging and cognitive deficits: the role of attentional resources. In F. I. M. Craik & S. E. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 191–211). New York: Plenum. Craik, F. I. M., & Rose, N. S. (2012). Training cognition: parallels with physical fitness? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1(1), 51–52. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2011.12.001. Crovitz, H. F., & Daniel, W. F. (1984). Measurements of everyday memory—toward the prevention of forgetting. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22(5), 413–414. DePompei, R., Gillette, Y., Goetz, E., Xenopoulos-Oddsson, A., Bryen, D., & Dowds, M. (2008). Practical applications for use of PDAs and smartphones with children and adolescents who have traumatic brain injury. Neurorehabilitation, 23(6), 487–499. Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (1990). Normal aging and prospective memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology-
  • 102. Learning Memory and Cognition, 16(4), 717–726. doi:10.1037/ 0278-7393.16.4.717. Einstein, G. O., McDaniel, M. A., Manzi, M., Cochran, B., & Baker, M. (2000). Prospective memory and aging: forgetting intentions over short delays. Psychology and Aging, 15(4), 671–683. doi:10.1037//0882-7974.15.4.671. Ellis, J. (1996). Prospective memory or the realization of delayed intentions: A conceptual framework for research. In M. A. Brandimonte, G. O. Einstein, & M. A. McDaniel (Eds.), Prospective memory: theory and applications (pp. 1–22). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum. Fernandez-Ballesteros, R., Botella, J., Zamarron, M. D., Molina, M. A., Cabras, E., Schettini, R., et al. (2012). Cognitive plasticity in normal and pathological aging. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 7, 15–25. doi:10.2147/Cia.S27008. Fish, J., Wilson, B. A., & Manly, T. (2010). The assessment and
  • 103. rehabilitation of prospective memory problems in people with neurological disorders: a review. Neuropsychological Rehabili- tation, 20(2), 161–179. doi:10.1080/09602010903126029. Fleming, J. M., Shum, D., Strong, J., & Lightbody, S. (2005). Prospective memory rehabilitation for adults with traumatic brain injury: a compensatory training programme. Brain Injury, 19(1), 1–10. doi:10.1080/02699050410001720059. Foster, E. R., Rose, N. S., McDaniel, M. A., & Rendell, P. G. (2013). Prospective memory in Parkinson disease during a virtual week: effects of both prospective and retrospective demands. Neuro- psychology, 27(2), 170–181. doi:10.1037/A0031946. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: the role of intentions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4(1), 141–185. doi:10. 1080/14792779343000059. Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions—strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493–503. doi:10. 1037//0003-066x.54.7.493.
  • 104. Gonneaud, J., Kalpouzos, G., Bon, L., Viader, F., Eustache, F., & Desgranges, B. (2011). Distinct and shared cognitive functions mediate event- and time-based prospective memory impairment in normal ageing. Memory, 19(4), 360–377. doi:10.1080/ 09658211.2011.570765. Greenwood, P. M., & Parasuraman, R. (2010). Neuronal and cognitive plasticity: a neurocognitive framework for ameliorat- ing cognitive aging. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 2:150. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2010.00150. Gross, A. L., Parisi, J. M., Spira, A. P., Kueider, A. M., Ko, J. Y., Saczynski, J. S., et al. (2012). Memory training interventions for older adults: a meta-analysis. Aging & Mental Health, 16(6), 722–734. doi:10.1080/13607863.2012.667783. Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H., & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, 19(1), 27–39. doi:10.1037/0882-
  • 105. 7974.19.1.27. Henry, J. D., Rendell, P. G., Phillips, L. H., Dunlop, L., & Kliegel, M. (2012). Prospective memory reminders: a laboratory investiga- tion of initiation source and age effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(7), 1274–1287. doi:10.1080/ 17470218.2011.651091. Ihle, A., Hering, A., Mahy, C. E. V., Bisiacchi, P. S., & Kliegel, M. (2013). Adult age differences, response management, and cue focality in event-based prospective memory: a meta-analysis on the role of task order specificity. Psychology and Aging, 28(3), 714–720. doi:10.1037/A0033653. Psychological Research 123 Author's personal copy http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0020683 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/Arclin/Act020 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/Fnhum.2012.00063 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-013-0288-2 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10433-013-0288-2 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0014342
  • 106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.311 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1041610212002177 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/gps.3859 http://dx.doi.org/10.3275/6704 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4943(03)00063-3 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4943(03)00063-3 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00385 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/Pbr.17.2.193 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-10-30 http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1741-7015-10-30 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2011.12.001 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.717 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.15.4.671 http://dx.doi.org/10.2147/Cia.S27008 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09602010903126029 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699050410001720059 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0031946 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14792779343000059 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14792779343000059 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.54.7.493 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.54.7.493 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.570765 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.570765 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/Fnagi.2010.00150 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2012.667783 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.27 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.1.27 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.651091 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2011.651091 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/A0033653 Insel, K. C., Einstein, G. O., Morrow, D. G., & Hepworth, J. T. (2013). A multifaceted prospective memory intervention to