SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 1
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
Undergraduate/Graduate	
  
Category:	
  
Degree	
  Level:	
  
Abstract	
  ID#	
  206	
  
Results	
  
50%	
  
55%	
  
60%	
  
65%	
  
70%	
  
75%	
  
80%	
  
85%	
  
90%	
  
95%	
  
100%	
  
1	
   2	
   3	
   4	
   5	
   6	
  
Figure	
  1.	
  	
  Experiment	
  1:	
  	
  Six	
  original	
  jury	
  instruc3ons	
  
	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Subject	
  Comprehension	
  
0%	
  
1%	
  
2%	
  
3%	
  
4%	
  
5%	
  
6%	
  
7%	
  
1	
   2	
   3	
   4	
   5	
   6	
  
Figure	
  2a.	
  Experiment	
  1:	
  	
  Six	
  original	
  jury	
  instruc3ons	
  
	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Passive	
  verbs/total	
  words	
  
0%	
  
1%	
  
2%	
  
3%	
  
4%	
  
5%	
  
6%	
  
7%	
  
1	
   2	
   3	
   4	
   5	
   6	
  
Figure	
  2b.	
  Experiment	
  1:	
  	
  Six	
  original	
  jury	
  instruc3ons	
  	
  
	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Presupposed	
  terms/total	
  words	
  
0%	
  
10%	
  
20%	
  
30%	
  
40%	
  
50%	
  
60%	
  
70%	
  
80%	
  
90%	
  
100%	
  
Original	
   Plain	
  English	
  
p	
  -­‐value	
  <	
  0.01	
  
	
  
Figure	
  3a.	
  	
  Experiments	
  1	
  &	
  2	
  
	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Instruc3on	
  6:	
  	
  	
  
	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Comprehension	
  
	
  
0%	
  
1%	
  
2%	
  
3%	
  
4%	
  
5%	
  
Original	
   Plain	
  English	
  
Figure	
  3b.	
  Experiments	
  1	
  &	
  2	
  
	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Instruc3on	
  6:	
  	
  
	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Passive	
  verbs/total	
  words	
  
0%	
  
1%	
  
2%	
  
3%	
  
4%	
  
5%	
  
Original	
   Plain	
  English	
  
Figure	
  3c.	
  Experiments	
  1	
  &	
  2	
  
	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Instruc3on	
  6:	
  	
  
	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Presupposed	
  terms/total	
  words	
  
You’re	
  a	
  juror	
  in	
  MassachuseOs.	
  	
  Before	
  deliberaSng,	
  the	
  judge	
  reads	
  you	
  the	
  jury	
  
instrucSons	
  to	
  explain	
  the	
  laws	
  that	
  apply	
  to	
  the	
  case.	
  	
  One	
  reads:	
  
	
  "Failure	
  of	
  recollec-on	
  is	
  common.	
  	
  Innocent	
  mis-­‐recollec-on	
  is	
  not	
  uncommon."	
  	
  	
  
But	
  in	
  California,	
  the	
  same	
  instrucSon	
  reads,	
  
	
  "People	
  o7en	
  forget	
  things	
  or	
  make	
  mistakes	
  in	
  what	
  they	
  remember."	
  1	
  
	
  	
  
The	
  California	
  jury	
  instrucSons	
  were	
  revised	
  in	
  2003	
  because	
  jurors	
  found	
  many	
  of	
  them	
  
incomprehensible	
  and	
  someSmes	
  returned	
  misinformed	
  verdicts	
  (Marder,	
  2006).	
  	
  Now,	
  
in	
  MassachuseOs,	
  a	
  task	
  force	
  of	
  judges,	
  lawyers,	
  and	
  linguists	
  believes	
  that	
  our	
  
instrucSons	
  also	
  need	
  rewriSng.	
  	
  But	
  for	
  a	
  rewriSng	
  project	
  to	
  win	
  widespread	
  support,	
  
we	
  must	
  show	
  that:	
  (1)	
  our	
  MassachuseOs	
  instrucSons	
  are	
  too	
  difficult;	
  (2)	
  we	
  can	
  
pinpoint	
  why;	
  and	
  (3)	
  we	
  can	
  significantly	
  improve	
  them.	
  	
  	
  
	
  	
  
To	
  demonstrate	
  (1),	
  we	
  ran	
  Experiment	
  1:	
  	
  subjects	
  heard	
  six	
  MassachuseOs	
  jury	
  
instrucSons	
  and	
  answered	
  true/false	
  quesSons	
  about	
  them.	
  	
  The	
  results	
  fell	
  into	
  two	
  
groups.	
  	
  Four	
  of	
  the	
  six	
  instrucSons	
  confirmed	
  (1),	
  with	
  low	
  comprehension	
  rates,	
  
61-­‐77%,	
  but	
  two	
  instrucSons	
  were	
  understood	
  by	
  88-­‐89%	
  of	
  subjects.	
  	
  A	
  linguisSc	
  
analysis	
  suggested	
  why,	
  addressing	
  (2):	
  	
  the	
  “difficult”	
  instrucSons	
  contained	
  more	
  
linguisSc	
  features	
  known	
  to	
  increase	
  processing	
  load	
  than	
  the	
  “easy”	
  instrucSons:	
  
stacked	
  negaSves	
  (Wason,	
  1956;	
  Just	
  &	
  Clark,	
  1973;	
  Just	
  &	
  Carpenter,	
  1976),	
  passive	
  
verbs	
  (Ferreira,	
  2003),	
  mulSply-­‐embedded	
  clauses	
  (Klare,	
  1976),	
  low-­‐frequency	
  words	
  
(Diana	
  &	
  Reder,	
  2006),	
  "legalese	
  (Benson,	
  1984),"	
  and	
  presupposed	
  terms.	
  	
  
	
  	
  
Experiment	
  2	
  improved	
  subjects’	
  comprehension	
  rates	
  	
  significantly	
  (p	
  <	
  0.001)	
  using	
  
rewriOen	
  “Plain	
  English”	
  versions	
  of	
  the	
  instrucSons.	
  	
  
	
  	
  
Though	
  legal	
  language	
  is	
  entrenched	
  and	
  reform	
  is	
  difficult,	
  psycholinguisSc	
  research	
  is	
  
providing	
  the	
  jusSficaSon	
  for	
  a	
  full-­‐scale	
  rewriSng	
  project	
  with	
  promising	
  outcomes:	
  	
  to	
  
make	
  courtroom	
  verdicts	
  more	
  reliable	
  and	
  the	
  judicial	
  system	
  more	
  accessible	
  and,	
  
ulSmately,	
  more	
  fair.	
  
Abstract	
  
Background	
  
Goals	
  
Method	
  
	
  
Jury	
  Instruc3on	
  6	
  	
  
Direct	
  and	
  Circumstan;al	
  Evidence	
  
	
  Original 	
   	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  Plain	
  English	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
	
  
Conclusions	
  
References	
  
Acknowledgements	
  
The	
  California	
  jury	
  instrucSons	
  were	
  revised	
  in	
  2003,	
  because	
  jurors	
  found	
  many	
  of	
  them	
  
incomprehensible	
  and	
  someSmes	
  returned	
  misinformed	
  verdicts	
  (Marder,	
  2006).	
  Now,	
  in	
  
MassachuseOs,	
  a	
  task	
  force	
  of	
  judges,	
  lawyers,	
  and	
  linguists	
  believes	
  that	
  our	
  instrucSons	
  
also	
  need	
  rewriSng.	
  For	
  a	
  rewriSng	
  project	
  to	
  win	
  widespread	
  support,	
  we	
  must	
  show	
  
that:	
  (1)	
  our	
  MassachuseOs	
  instrucSons	
  are	
  too	
  difficult	
  for	
  jurors	
  to	
  understand;	
  (2)	
  we	
  
can	
  pinpoint	
  why;	
  and	
  (3)	
  we	
  can	
  significantly	
  improve	
  them.	
  	
  
To	
  test	
  (1)	
  -­‐	
  (3),	
  we	
  ran	
  two	
  experiments.	
  	
  Experiment	
  1	
  hypothesized	
  (1)	
  that	
  our	
  current	
  	
  
“original”	
  MassachuseOs	
  jury	
  instrucSons	
  are	
  difficult	
  to	
  understand	
  and	
  (2)	
  the	
  reason	
  
comes	
  from	
  their	
  linguisSc	
  features:	
  	
  certain	
  	
  features	
  are	
  known	
  to	
  increase	
  linguisSc	
  
“processing	
  load.”	
  	
  Experiment	
  	
  2	
  	
  was	
  designed	
  to	
  show	
  (3)	
  that	
  we	
  could	
  make	
  the	
  
instrucSons	
  easier	
  by	
  using	
  rewriOen	
  “Plain	
  English”	
  versions	
  containing	
  fewer	
  of	
  these	
  
features.	
  	
  We	
  focused	
  on	
  two	
  features:	
  passive	
  verbs	
  and	
  presupposed	
  terms.	
  
There	
  are	
  two	
  types	
  of	
  evidence	
  that	
  you	
  may	
  use	
  to	
  determine	
  the	
  
facts	
  of	
  a	
  case:	
  direct	
  evidence	
  and	
  circumstan-al	
  evidence.	
  	
  
	
  	
  
You	
  have	
  direct	
  evidence	
  where	
  a	
  witness	
  tes-fies	
  directly	
  about	
  the	
  
fact	
  that	
  is	
  to	
  be	
  proved,	
  based	
  on	
  what	
  (he/she)	
  claims	
  to	
  have	
  seen	
  
or	
  heard	
  or	
  felt	
  with	
  (his/her)	
  own	
  senses,	
  and	
  the	
  only	
  ques-on	
  is	
  
whether	
  you	
  believe	
  the	
  witness.	
  	
  
	
  	
  
You	
  have	
  circumstan-al	
  evidence	
  where	
  no	
  witness	
  can	
  tes-fy	
  directly	
  
about	
  the	
  fact	
  that	
  is	
  to	
  be	
  proved,	
  but	
  you	
  are	
  presented	
  with	
  
evidence	
  of	
  other	
  facts	
  and	
  then	
  asked	
  to	
  draw	
  reasonable	
  inferences	
  
from	
  them	
  about	
  the	
  fact	
  that	
  is	
  to	
  be	
  proved.	
  	
  
You	
  have	
  heard	
  evidence	
  that	
  you	
  must	
  use	
  to	
  decide	
  what	
  the	
  facts	
  are	
  
in	
  this	
  case.	
  	
  There	
  are	
  two	
  types	
  of	
  evidence.	
  	
  One	
  type	
  is	
  called	
  direct	
  
evidence,	
  which	
  is	
  what	
  a	
  witness	
  claims	
  to	
  have	
  seen	
  or	
  heard	
  or	
  
smelled.	
  	
  So,	
  a	
  witness	
  saying	
  that	
  she	
  saw	
  a	
  mailman	
  put	
  mail	
  into	
  her	
  
mailbox	
  is	
  direct	
  evidence	
  that	
  the	
  mailman	
  delivered	
  her	
  mail.	
  	
  	
  
	
  
The	
  other	
  type	
  of	
  evidence	
  is	
  indirect	
  or	
  “circumstan-al”	
  evidence.	
  	
  A	
  
witness	
  saying	
  that	
  she	
  saw	
  that	
  her	
  mailbox	
  was	
  empty	
  when	
  she	
  le7	
  
the	
  house,	
  and	
  full	
  when	
  she	
  came	
  home	
  is	
  indirect	
  evidence	
  that	
  the	
  
mailman	
  delivered	
  her	
  mail.	
  	
  	
  	
  
	
  
	
  Indirect	
  evidence	
  allows	
  you	
  to	
  reach	
  the	
  same	
  conclusion	
  as	
  direct	
  
evidence,	
  but	
  you	
  have	
  to	
  make	
  an	
  inference	
  -­‐-­‐	
  a	
  logical	
  connec-on	
  -­‐-­‐	
  to	
  
get	
  there.	
  	
  	
  It	
  makes	
  no	
  difference	
  whether	
  evidence	
  is	
  direct	
  or	
  indirect.	
  	
  
One	
  is	
  not	
  bePer	
  than	
  the	
  other.	
  	
  	
  
Subjects.	
  	
  	
  
•  58	
  total	
  subjects	
  (29	
  in	
  Experiment	
  1,	
  29	
  in	
  Experiment	
  2)	
  
Materials.	
  	
  	
  
•  Experiment	
  1:	
  	
  6	
  current	
  MassachuseOs	
  jury	
  instrucSons	
  (“Original”	
  instrucSons)	
  +	
  1	
  
pracSce	
  instrucSon	
  
•  Experiment	
  2:	
  	
  6	
  re-­‐wriOen	
  jury	
  instrucSons	
  (“Plain	
  English”	
  instrucSons)	
  +	
  1	
  pracSce	
  
instrucSon	
  
•  Digital	
  recordings	
  of	
  all	
  instrucSons	
  read	
  by	
  a	
  judge	
  
	
  
Procedure.	
  	
  
•  Subjects	
  listened	
  to	
  the	
  instrucSons	
  one	
  at	
  a	
  Sme,	
  beginning	
  with	
  the	
  pracSce	
  
instrucSon.	
  	
  	
  
•  Aper	
  hearing	
  each	
  instrucSon,	
  subjects	
  answered	
  a	
  set	
  of	
  true/false	
  quesSons	
  about	
  
it,	
  the	
  number	
  of	
  quesSons	
  corresponding	
  to	
  the	
  length	
  of	
  the	
  instrucSon.	
  	
  The	
  
statements	
  were	
  the	
  same	
  in	
  Experiments	
  1	
  and	
  2.	
  
•  Each	
  session	
  ran	
  for	
  approximately	
  25-­‐30	
  minutes.	
  
Psycholinguis3cs	
  meets	
  “Legalese”:	
  Improving	
  courtroom	
  verdicts	
  in	
  MassachuseWs	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  
Janet	
  Randall,	
  Lucas	
  Graf,	
  Brian	
  Kim,	
  Aaron	
  McPherson,	
  Lindi	
  Nguyen,	
  Elizabeth	
  Steyngrob	
  and	
  Tong	
  Zhang	
  	
  	
  
Discussion	
  
	
  The	
  comprehension	
  results	
  of	
  Experiment	
  1	
  fell	
  into	
  two	
  groups.	
  	
  Four	
  of	
  the	
  six	
  
instrucSons	
  (instrucSons	
  3-­‐6)	
  confirmed	
  (1):	
  	
  MassachuseOs	
  jury	
  instrucSons	
  are	
  
difficult	
  to	
  understand,	
  with	
  low	
  comprehension	
  rates	
  	
  (Fig.	
  1),	
  ranging	
  from	
  
61-­‐77%.	
  	
  However,	
  instrucSons	
  1	
  and	
  2	
  were	
  understood	
  by	
  88	
  -­‐	
  89%	
  of	
  subjects.	
  
The	
  difference	
  between	
  the	
  two	
  sets	
  was	
  significant	
  	
  	
  	
  	
  (p	
  <	
  .05).	
  	
  In	
  answer	
  to	
  (2),	
  
why?,	
  we	
  analyzed	
  the	
  instrucSons	
  for	
  two	
  features	
  known	
  to	
  increase	
  processing	
  
load:	
  	
  passive	
  verbs	
  (Fig.	
  2a)	
  and	
  presupposed	
  terms	
  (Fig.	
  2b).	
  	
  As	
  hypothesized,	
  the	
  
“difficult”	
  instrucSons	
  3-­‐6	
  contained	
  relaSvely	
  more	
  passive	
  verbs	
  and	
  presupposed	
  
terms	
  than	
  the	
  “easy”	
  instrucSons	
  1-­‐2.	
  For	
  example,	
  InstrucSon	
  6,	
  Direct	
  and	
  
Circumstan;al	
  Evidence	
  (shown	
  in	
  brown),	
  contained	
  5	
  passives	
  and	
  2	
  presupposed	
  
terms	
  in	
  the	
  Original	
  jury	
  instrucSon,	
  its	
  comprehension	
  rate	
  was	
  the	
  lowest,	
  61%.	
  	
  
In	
  Experiment	
  2,	
  we	
  rewrote	
  all	
  of	
  the	
  instrucSons	
  in	
  Plain	
  English,	
  decreasing	
  the	
  
number	
  of	
  passive	
  verbs	
  and	
  presupposed	
  terms	
  (Fig	
  3b)	
  and,	
  as	
  predicted	
  by	
  (3),	
  
comprehension	
  rates	
  rose	
  significantly.	
  	
  For	
  instrucSon	
  6,	
  for	
  example,	
  
comprehension	
  rose	
  significantly	
  	
  (p	
  <	
  .01)	
  for	
  the	
  Plain	
  English	
  version	
  from	
  61%	
  to	
  	
  
82%	
  (Fig.	
  3a),	
  reflecSng	
  significant	
  decreases	
  in	
  passive	
  verbs	
  from	
  5	
  to	
  1	
  (Fig.	
  3b)	
  
and	
  in	
  presupposed	
  terms	
  from	
  2	
  to	
  0	
  (Fig.	
  3c).	
  	
  	
  	
  
As	
  hypothesized,	
  our	
  two	
  experiments	
  have	
  shown	
  that:	
  (1)	
  our	
  MassachuseOs	
  jury	
  
instrucSons	
  are	
  too	
  difficult	
  to	
  understand;	
  (2)	
  the	
  cause	
  of	
  difficulty	
  is	
  linguisSc;	
  
and	
  (3)	
  we	
  can	
  significantly	
  improve	
  comprehension.	
  	
  	
  
	
  
Experiment	
  1	
  demonstrated	
  that	
  comprehension	
  correlated	
  with	
  two	
  linguisSc	
  
factors	
  known	
  to	
  increase	
  processing	
  load:	
  (a)	
  passive	
  -­‐-­‐	
  not	
  acSve	
  -­‐-­‐	
  verbs	
  and	
  (b)	
  
presupposed	
  terms.	
  	
  Experiment	
  2	
  demonstrated	
  that	
  rewriSng	
  the	
  instrucSons	
  in	
  
“Plain	
  English”	
  with	
  fewer	
  of	
  these	
  factors	
  significantly	
  improved	
  comprehension.	
  	
  
	
  
These	
  findings	
  bode	
  well	
  for	
  the	
  movement	
  to	
  reform	
  jury	
  instrucSons.	
  	
  Though	
  
“legalese”	
  is	
  entrenched	
  and	
  reform	
  is	
  difficult,	
  psycholinguisSc	
  research	
  can	
  
provide	
  the	
  empirical	
  evidence	
  to	
  support	
  judges	
  and	
  lawyers	
  advocaSng	
  change.	
  	
  
Clearer	
  instrucSons	
  should	
  give	
  all	
  jurors	
  full	
  parScipaSon	
  in	
  the	
  jusSce	
  system.	
  	
  
The	
  result:	
  their	
  verdicts	
  will	
  be	
  more	
  reliable	
  and,	
  ulSmately,	
  more	
  fair.	
  	
  	
  
We	
  are	
  grateful	
  to	
  the	
  MassachuseOs	
  Bar	
  
AssociaSon	
  for	
  providing	
  grant	
  support	
  for	
  
this	
  research	
  and	
  for	
  sponsoring	
  Professor	
  
Janet	
  Randall	
  as	
  a	
  2012-­‐13	
  VisiSng	
  Research	
  
Fellow.	
  
We	
  also	
  thank	
  the	
  members	
  of	
  the	
  MBA	
  Plain	
  
English	
  Jury	
  InstrucSon	
  Task	
  Force	
  for	
  
comments	
  and	
  suggesSons.	
  	
  	
  
1	
  Book	
  of	
  Approved	
  Jury	
  InstrucSons	
  (BAJI),	
  2.21.	
  Judicial	
  Council	
  of	
  California	
  Civil	
  
Jury	
  InstrucSon	
  (CACI,	
  2003)	
  	
  	
  	
  
Benson,	
  R.	
  (1984)	
  	
  The	
  End	
  of	
  Legalese:	
  the	
  game	
  is	
  over.	
  	
  NYU	
  Review	
  of	
  Law	
  and	
  
Social	
  Change	
  13,	
  519-­‐574.	
  
hOp://www.courts.ca.gov/partners/documents/caci_2012_edSon.pdf	
  
Diana,	
  Rachel	
  A.	
  and	
  Lynne	
  M.	
  Reder	
  (2006)	
  	
  The	
  Low-­‐Frequency	
  Encoding	
  
Disadvantage:	
  Word	
  Frequency	
  Affects	
  Processing	
  Demands.	
  	
  Journal	
  of	
  
Experimental	
  Psychology:	
  Learning,	
  Memory,	
  and	
  Cogni-on	
  	
  32,	
  805–815.	
  
Ferreira,	
  F.	
  (2003)	
  	
  The	
  misinterpretaSon	
  of	
  non-­‐canonical	
  sentences.	
  	
  	
  Cogni-ve	
  
Psychology	
  47,164–203.	
  
Marder,	
  Nancy	
  (2006)	
  Bringing	
  Jury	
  Instruc-ons	
  into	
  the	
  Twenty-­‐First	
  Century,	
  81	
  
Notre	
  Dame	
  L.	
  Rev.,	
  451-­‐470.	
  
Just,	
  M.	
  A.,	
  &	
  Carpenter,	
  P.	
  A.	
  (1976).	
  Eye	
  fixaSons	
  and	
  cogniSve	
  processes.	
  
Cogni-ve	
  Psychology,	
  8,	
  441–480.	
  
Just,	
  M.	
  A.,	
  &	
  Clark,	
  H.	
  H.	
  (1973).	
  	
  Drawing	
  inferences	
  from	
  the	
  presupposiSons	
  and	
  
implicaSons	
  of	
  affirmaSve	
  and	
  negaSve	
  sentences.	
  Journal	
  of	
  Verbal	
  Learning	
  
and	
  Verbal	
  Behavior,	
  12,	
  21–31.	
  
Klare,	
  G.	
  R.	
  (1976)	
  	
  A	
  second	
  look	
  at	
  the	
  validity	
  of	
  the	
  readability	
  formulas.	
  	
  Journal	
  
of	
  reading	
  behavior	
  8,	
  159-­‐152.	
  
Wason,	
  P.	
  C.	
  (1959).	
  The	
  processing	
  of	
  posiSve	
  and	
  negaSve	
  informaSon.	
  Quarterly	
  
Journal	
  of	
  Experimental	
  Psychology,	
  11,	
  92–107.	
  	
  	
  

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Ähnlich wie RISE Poster (Final)

Research in Literature.docx
Research in Literature.docxResearch in Literature.docx
Research in Literature.docxhzlquirante13
 
Intentional Inhibition
Intentional InhibitionIntentional Inhibition
Intentional Inhibitionmcneeljc86
 
Guiding the Reader: Title, Abstract and Introduction
Guiding the Reader: Title, Abstract and IntroductionGuiding the Reader: Title, Abstract and Introduction
Guiding the Reader: Title, Abstract and IntroductionRon Martinez
 
Recognizing ArgumentsIn this assignment, you will apply key co.docx
Recognizing ArgumentsIn this assignment, you will apply key co.docxRecognizing ArgumentsIn this assignment, you will apply key co.docx
Recognizing ArgumentsIn this assignment, you will apply key co.docxdanas19
 
Discussion Provide at least two references for your initial po
Discussion Provide at least two references for your initial poDiscussion Provide at least two references for your initial po
Discussion Provide at least two references for your initial poLyndonPelletier761
 
Criminal Law and Procedure John Scheb.docx
Criminal Law and Procedure John Scheb.docxCriminal Law and Procedure John Scheb.docx
Criminal Law and Procedure John Scheb.docxwrite22
 
Lab assignment 1 revised 2
Lab assignment 1 revised 2Lab assignment 1 revised 2
Lab assignment 1 revised 2juancarlosrise
 
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular SystemOpen.Michigan
 
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular SystemOpen.Michigan
 
Luchins’ Einstellung (Set) ExperimentsAbraham S. Luchins (1942) .docx
Luchins’ Einstellung (Set) ExperimentsAbraham S. Luchins (1942) .docxLuchins’ Einstellung (Set) ExperimentsAbraham S. Luchins (1942) .docx
Luchins’ Einstellung (Set) ExperimentsAbraham S. Luchins (1942) .docxSHIVA101531
 
Schizophrenia and working memory
Schizophrenia and working memorySchizophrenia and working memory
Schizophrenia and working memoryKelly Burke
 
Copyright 2022 Post University, ALL RIGHTS RESERVED .docx
Copyright 2022 Post University, ALL RIGHTS RESERVED   .docxCopyright 2022 Post University, ALL RIGHTS RESERVED   .docx
Copyright 2022 Post University, ALL RIGHTS RESERVED .docxrichardnorman90310
 

Ähnlich wie RISE Poster (Final) (20)

PR 1 WEEK 1.pptx
PR 1 WEEK 1.pptxPR 1 WEEK 1.pptx
PR 1 WEEK 1.pptx
 
Research in Literature.docx
Research in Literature.docxResearch in Literature.docx
Research in Literature.docx
 
Intentional Inhibition
Intentional InhibitionIntentional Inhibition
Intentional Inhibition
 
Guiding the Reader: Title, Abstract and Introduction
Guiding the Reader: Title, Abstract and IntroductionGuiding the Reader: Title, Abstract and Introduction
Guiding the Reader: Title, Abstract and Introduction
 
Recognizing ArgumentsIn this assignment, you will apply key co.docx
Recognizing ArgumentsIn this assignment, you will apply key co.docxRecognizing ArgumentsIn this assignment, you will apply key co.docx
Recognizing ArgumentsIn this assignment, you will apply key co.docx
 
Discussion Provide at least two references for your initial po
Discussion Provide at least two references for your initial poDiscussion Provide at least two references for your initial po
Discussion Provide at least two references for your initial po
 
JurorComprehension
JurorComprehensionJurorComprehension
JurorComprehension
 
Dooley edpy 741
Dooley   edpy 741Dooley   edpy 741
Dooley edpy 741
 
Error analysis revised
Error analysis revisedError analysis revised
Error analysis revised
 
Results
ResultsResults
Results
 
Criminal Law and Procedure John Scheb.docx
Criminal Law and Procedure John Scheb.docxCriminal Law and Procedure John Scheb.docx
Criminal Law and Procedure John Scheb.docx
 
Lab assignment 1 revised 2
Lab assignment 1 revised 2Lab assignment 1 revised 2
Lab assignment 1 revised 2
 
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
 
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
10.27.08: Introduction Homeostasis/Cardiovascular System
 
Psychology Unit 1
Psychology Unit 1 Psychology Unit 1
Psychology Unit 1
 
Error analysis
Error analysis Error analysis
Error analysis
 
Luchins’ Einstellung (Set) ExperimentsAbraham S. Luchins (1942) .docx
Luchins’ Einstellung (Set) ExperimentsAbraham S. Luchins (1942) .docxLuchins’ Einstellung (Set) ExperimentsAbraham S. Luchins (1942) .docx
Luchins’ Einstellung (Set) ExperimentsAbraham S. Luchins (1942) .docx
 
The Natural Approach
The Natural ApproachThe Natural Approach
The Natural Approach
 
Schizophrenia and working memory
Schizophrenia and working memorySchizophrenia and working memory
Schizophrenia and working memory
 
Copyright 2022 Post University, ALL RIGHTS RESERVED .docx
Copyright 2022 Post University, ALL RIGHTS RESERVED   .docxCopyright 2022 Post University, ALL RIGHTS RESERVED   .docx
Copyright 2022 Post University, ALL RIGHTS RESERVED .docx
 

RISE Poster (Final)

  • 1. Undergraduate/Graduate   Category:   Degree  Level:   Abstract  ID#  206   Results   50%   55%   60%   65%   70%   75%   80%   85%   90%   95%   100%   1   2   3   4   5   6   Figure  1.    Experiment  1:    Six  original  jury  instruc3ons                                      Subject  Comprehension   0%   1%   2%   3%   4%   5%   6%   7%   1   2   3   4   5   6   Figure  2a.  Experiment  1:    Six  original  jury  instruc3ons                                          Passive  verbs/total  words   0%   1%   2%   3%   4%   5%   6%   7%   1   2   3   4   5   6   Figure  2b.  Experiment  1:    Six  original  jury  instruc3ons                                            Presupposed  terms/total  words   0%   10%   20%   30%   40%   50%   60%   70%   80%   90%   100%   Original   Plain  English   p  -­‐value  <  0.01     Figure  3a.    Experiments  1  &  2                      Instruc3on  6:                          Comprehension     0%   1%   2%   3%   4%   5%   Original   Plain  English   Figure  3b.  Experiments  1  &  2                                        Instruc3on  6:                                          Passive  verbs/total  words   0%   1%   2%   3%   4%   5%   Original   Plain  English   Figure  3c.  Experiments  1  &  2                  Instruc3on  6:                                        Presupposed  terms/total  words   You’re  a  juror  in  MassachuseOs.    Before  deliberaSng,  the  judge  reads  you  the  jury   instrucSons  to  explain  the  laws  that  apply  to  the  case.    One  reads:    "Failure  of  recollec-on  is  common.    Innocent  mis-­‐recollec-on  is  not  uncommon."       But  in  California,  the  same  instrucSon  reads,    "People  o7en  forget  things  or  make  mistakes  in  what  they  remember."  1       The  California  jury  instrucSons  were  revised  in  2003  because  jurors  found  many  of  them   incomprehensible  and  someSmes  returned  misinformed  verdicts  (Marder,  2006).    Now,   in  MassachuseOs,  a  task  force  of  judges,  lawyers,  and  linguists  believes  that  our   instrucSons  also  need  rewriSng.    But  for  a  rewriSng  project  to  win  widespread  support,   we  must  show  that:  (1)  our  MassachuseOs  instrucSons  are  too  difficult;  (2)  we  can   pinpoint  why;  and  (3)  we  can  significantly  improve  them.           To  demonstrate  (1),  we  ran  Experiment  1:    subjects  heard  six  MassachuseOs  jury   instrucSons  and  answered  true/false  quesSons  about  them.    The  results  fell  into  two   groups.    Four  of  the  six  instrucSons  confirmed  (1),  with  low  comprehension  rates,   61-­‐77%,  but  two  instrucSons  were  understood  by  88-­‐89%  of  subjects.    A  linguisSc   analysis  suggested  why,  addressing  (2):    the  “difficult”  instrucSons  contained  more   linguisSc  features  known  to  increase  processing  load  than  the  “easy”  instrucSons:   stacked  negaSves  (Wason,  1956;  Just  &  Clark,  1973;  Just  &  Carpenter,  1976),  passive   verbs  (Ferreira,  2003),  mulSply-­‐embedded  clauses  (Klare,  1976),  low-­‐frequency  words   (Diana  &  Reder,  2006),  "legalese  (Benson,  1984),"  and  presupposed  terms.         Experiment  2  improved  subjects’  comprehension  rates    significantly  (p  <  0.001)  using   rewriOen  “Plain  English”  versions  of  the  instrucSons.         Though  legal  language  is  entrenched  and  reform  is  difficult,  psycholinguisSc  research  is   providing  the  jusSficaSon  for  a  full-­‐scale  rewriSng  project  with  promising  outcomes:    to   make  courtroom  verdicts  more  reliable  and  the  judicial  system  more  accessible  and,   ulSmately,  more  fair.   Abstract   Background   Goals   Method     Jury  Instruc3on  6     Direct  and  Circumstan;al  Evidence    Original                    Plain  English                                   Conclusions   References   Acknowledgements   The  California  jury  instrucSons  were  revised  in  2003,  because  jurors  found  many  of  them   incomprehensible  and  someSmes  returned  misinformed  verdicts  (Marder,  2006).  Now,  in   MassachuseOs,  a  task  force  of  judges,  lawyers,  and  linguists  believes  that  our  instrucSons   also  need  rewriSng.  For  a  rewriSng  project  to  win  widespread  support,  we  must  show   that:  (1)  our  MassachuseOs  instrucSons  are  too  difficult  for  jurors  to  understand;  (2)  we   can  pinpoint  why;  and  (3)  we  can  significantly  improve  them.     To  test  (1)  -­‐  (3),  we  ran  two  experiments.    Experiment  1  hypothesized  (1)  that  our  current     “original”  MassachuseOs  jury  instrucSons  are  difficult  to  understand  and  (2)  the  reason   comes  from  their  linguisSc  features:    certain    features  are  known  to  increase  linguisSc   “processing  load.”    Experiment    2    was  designed  to  show  (3)  that  we  could  make  the   instrucSons  easier  by  using  rewriOen  “Plain  English”  versions  containing  fewer  of  these   features.    We  focused  on  two  features:  passive  verbs  and  presupposed  terms.   There  are  two  types  of  evidence  that  you  may  use  to  determine  the   facts  of  a  case:  direct  evidence  and  circumstan-al  evidence.         You  have  direct  evidence  where  a  witness  tes-fies  directly  about  the   fact  that  is  to  be  proved,  based  on  what  (he/she)  claims  to  have  seen   or  heard  or  felt  with  (his/her)  own  senses,  and  the  only  ques-on  is   whether  you  believe  the  witness.         You  have  circumstan-al  evidence  where  no  witness  can  tes-fy  directly   about  the  fact  that  is  to  be  proved,  but  you  are  presented  with   evidence  of  other  facts  and  then  asked  to  draw  reasonable  inferences   from  them  about  the  fact  that  is  to  be  proved.     You  have  heard  evidence  that  you  must  use  to  decide  what  the  facts  are   in  this  case.    There  are  two  types  of  evidence.    One  type  is  called  direct   evidence,  which  is  what  a  witness  claims  to  have  seen  or  heard  or   smelled.    So,  a  witness  saying  that  she  saw  a  mailman  put  mail  into  her   mailbox  is  direct  evidence  that  the  mailman  delivered  her  mail.         The  other  type  of  evidence  is  indirect  or  “circumstan-al”  evidence.    A   witness  saying  that  she  saw  that  her  mailbox  was  empty  when  she  le7   the  house,  and  full  when  she  came  home  is  indirect  evidence  that  the   mailman  delivered  her  mail.            Indirect  evidence  allows  you  to  reach  the  same  conclusion  as  direct   evidence,  but  you  have  to  make  an  inference  -­‐-­‐  a  logical  connec-on  -­‐-­‐  to   get  there.      It  makes  no  difference  whether  evidence  is  direct  or  indirect.     One  is  not  bePer  than  the  other.       Subjects.       •  58  total  subjects  (29  in  Experiment  1,  29  in  Experiment  2)   Materials.       •  Experiment  1:    6  current  MassachuseOs  jury  instrucSons  (“Original”  instrucSons)  +  1   pracSce  instrucSon   •  Experiment  2:    6  re-­‐wriOen  jury  instrucSons  (“Plain  English”  instrucSons)  +  1  pracSce   instrucSon   •  Digital  recordings  of  all  instrucSons  read  by  a  judge     Procedure.     •  Subjects  listened  to  the  instrucSons  one  at  a  Sme,  beginning  with  the  pracSce   instrucSon.       •  Aper  hearing  each  instrucSon,  subjects  answered  a  set  of  true/false  quesSons  about   it,  the  number  of  quesSons  corresponding  to  the  length  of  the  instrucSon.    The   statements  were  the  same  in  Experiments  1  and  2.   •  Each  session  ran  for  approximately  25-­‐30  minutes.   Psycholinguis3cs  meets  “Legalese”:  Improving  courtroom  verdicts  in  MassachuseWs                                                                     Janet  Randall,  Lucas  Graf,  Brian  Kim,  Aaron  McPherson,  Lindi  Nguyen,  Elizabeth  Steyngrob  and  Tong  Zhang       Discussion    The  comprehension  results  of  Experiment  1  fell  into  two  groups.    Four  of  the  six   instrucSons  (instrucSons  3-­‐6)  confirmed  (1):    MassachuseOs  jury  instrucSons  are   difficult  to  understand,  with  low  comprehension  rates    (Fig.  1),  ranging  from   61-­‐77%.    However,  instrucSons  1  and  2  were  understood  by  88  -­‐  89%  of  subjects.   The  difference  between  the  two  sets  was  significant          (p  <  .05).    In  answer  to  (2),   why?,  we  analyzed  the  instrucSons  for  two  features  known  to  increase  processing   load:    passive  verbs  (Fig.  2a)  and  presupposed  terms  (Fig.  2b).    As  hypothesized,  the   “difficult”  instrucSons  3-­‐6  contained  relaSvely  more  passive  verbs  and  presupposed   terms  than  the  “easy”  instrucSons  1-­‐2.  For  example,  InstrucSon  6,  Direct  and   Circumstan;al  Evidence  (shown  in  brown),  contained  5  passives  and  2  presupposed   terms  in  the  Original  jury  instrucSon,  its  comprehension  rate  was  the  lowest,  61%.     In  Experiment  2,  we  rewrote  all  of  the  instrucSons  in  Plain  English,  decreasing  the   number  of  passive  verbs  and  presupposed  terms  (Fig  3b)  and,  as  predicted  by  (3),   comprehension  rates  rose  significantly.    For  instrucSon  6,  for  example,   comprehension  rose  significantly    (p  <  .01)  for  the  Plain  English  version  from  61%  to     82%  (Fig.  3a),  reflecSng  significant  decreases  in  passive  verbs  from  5  to  1  (Fig.  3b)   and  in  presupposed  terms  from  2  to  0  (Fig.  3c).         As  hypothesized,  our  two  experiments  have  shown  that:  (1)  our  MassachuseOs  jury   instrucSons  are  too  difficult  to  understand;  (2)  the  cause  of  difficulty  is  linguisSc;   and  (3)  we  can  significantly  improve  comprehension.         Experiment  1  demonstrated  that  comprehension  correlated  with  two  linguisSc   factors  known  to  increase  processing  load:  (a)  passive  -­‐-­‐  not  acSve  -­‐-­‐  verbs  and  (b)   presupposed  terms.    Experiment  2  demonstrated  that  rewriSng  the  instrucSons  in   “Plain  English”  with  fewer  of  these  factors  significantly  improved  comprehension.       These  findings  bode  well  for  the  movement  to  reform  jury  instrucSons.    Though   “legalese”  is  entrenched  and  reform  is  difficult,  psycholinguisSc  research  can   provide  the  empirical  evidence  to  support  judges  and  lawyers  advocaSng  change.     Clearer  instrucSons  should  give  all  jurors  full  parScipaSon  in  the  jusSce  system.     The  result:  their  verdicts  will  be  more  reliable  and,  ulSmately,  more  fair.       We  are  grateful  to  the  MassachuseOs  Bar   AssociaSon  for  providing  grant  support  for   this  research  and  for  sponsoring  Professor   Janet  Randall  as  a  2012-­‐13  VisiSng  Research   Fellow.   We  also  thank  the  members  of  the  MBA  Plain   English  Jury  InstrucSon  Task  Force  for   comments  and  suggesSons.       1  Book  of  Approved  Jury  InstrucSons  (BAJI),  2.21.  Judicial  Council  of  California  Civil   Jury  InstrucSon  (CACI,  2003)         Benson,  R.  (1984)    The  End  of  Legalese:  the  game  is  over.    NYU  Review  of  Law  and   Social  Change  13,  519-­‐574.   hOp://www.courts.ca.gov/partners/documents/caci_2012_edSon.pdf   Diana,  Rachel  A.  and  Lynne  M.  Reder  (2006)    The  Low-­‐Frequency  Encoding   Disadvantage:  Word  Frequency  Affects  Processing  Demands.    Journal  of   Experimental  Psychology:  Learning,  Memory,  and  Cogni-on    32,  805–815.   Ferreira,  F.  (2003)    The  misinterpretaSon  of  non-­‐canonical  sentences.      Cogni-ve   Psychology  47,164–203.   Marder,  Nancy  (2006)  Bringing  Jury  Instruc-ons  into  the  Twenty-­‐First  Century,  81   Notre  Dame  L.  Rev.,  451-­‐470.   Just,  M.  A.,  &  Carpenter,  P.  A.  (1976).  Eye  fixaSons  and  cogniSve  processes.   Cogni-ve  Psychology,  8,  441–480.   Just,  M.  A.,  &  Clark,  H.  H.  (1973).    Drawing  inferences  from  the  presupposiSons  and   implicaSons  of  affirmaSve  and  negaSve  sentences.  Journal  of  Verbal  Learning   and  Verbal  Behavior,  12,  21–31.   Klare,  G.  R.  (1976)    A  second  look  at  the  validity  of  the  readability  formulas.    Journal   of  reading  behavior  8,  159-­‐152.   Wason,  P.  C.  (1959).  The  processing  of  posiSve  and  negaSve  informaSon.  Quarterly   Journal  of  Experimental  Psychology,  11,  92–107.