SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 14
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




                                                                                                                                     499


                                                                                                                        The
                                                                                                                        British
                                                                                                                        Psychological
                                       British Journal of Developmental Psychology (2007), 25, 499–512
                                                                  q 2007 The British Psychological Society
                                                                                                                        Society

                                                                                                             www.bpsjournals.co.uk




          Pragmatic aspects of communication and
          language comprehension in groups of children
          differentiated by teacher ratings of inattention
          and hyperactivity

          Simon Bignell1 and Kate Cain2*
          1
              University of Essex, UK
          2
              Lancaster University, UK

                   Children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) experience pragmatic
                   language deficits, but it is not known whether these difficulties are primarily associated
                   with high levels of inattention, hyperactivity, or both. We investigated pragmatic aspects
                   of communication and language comprehension in relation to poor attention and/or
                   high hyperactivity in a nondiagnosed population of 7- to 11-year olds. Classroom
                   teachers rated their pupils’ attention and hyperactivity/impulsivity on the ADD-H
                   Comprehensive Teacher Rating scale (ACTeRS). Three groups were formed: children
                   with poor attention and low hyperactivity (poor attention group), children with good
                   attention and high hyperactivity (high hyperactivity group), and children with both poor
                   attention and high hyperactivity (poor attention/high hyperactivity group). Their
                   performance was compared with that of same-age controls in two studies: Study 1
                   (N ¼ 94) investigated the comprehension of figurative language in and out of context
                   and Study 2 (N ¼ 100) investigated the pragmatic aspects of communication using the
                   Children’s Communication Checklist – Second Edition.
                   Two groups, the poor attention and the poor attention/high hyperactivity groups, were
                   impaired in both their comprehension of figurative language and their communication
                   skills. The high hyperactivity group was impaired in their comprehension of figurative
                   language but they did not exhibit communication impairments. The findings extend
                   work with clinical populations of children with ADHD: even in a nondiagnosed sample
                   of children, poor attention and elevated levels of hyperactivity are associated with
                   pragmatic language weaknesses.


          Pragmatics is defined as how language is used to convey meaning (e.g. Adams, 2002).
          Pragmatic language difficulties are specific to the use and comprehension of language
          in context, rather than problems with semantic or structural aspects of language.
          Difficulties in language use include poor turn taking and an inability to stay on topic in

          * Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Kate Cain, Department of Psychology, Fylde College, Lancaster University,
          Lancaster, UK (e-mail: k.cain@lancs.ac.uk).

          DOI:10.1348/026151006X171343
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




500 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain

conversation; difficulties in language comprehension include a tendency to interpret
figurative language literally. We report an investigation of these two aspects of pragmatic
language in groups of 7- to 11-year olds differentiated by teacher ratings of inattention
and hyperactivity.
    ADHD is a behavioural disorder, in which individuals exhibit levels of inattention
and/or hyperactivity and impulsivity that are inappropriate for their age (DSM-IV,
American Psychiatric Association, 1994). DSM-IV distinguishes between the inattentive
and hyperactivity/impulsivity elements of the disorder, which may occur together or
separately, resulting in three subtypes: predominantly inattentive, predominantly
hyperactive–impulsive, and combined type. Some researchers advocate the use of a
categorical approach, in which ADHD is regarded as a distinct syndrome. Others adopt a
framework in which inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity vary throughout the
general population and children with a diagnosis of ADHD lie in the tail end of a normal
distribution (Levy, Hay, McStephen, Wood, & Waldman, 1997).
    ADHD frequently co-occurs with language disorders (Tirosh & Cohen, 1998; Westby &
Cutler, 1994). An analysis of the DSM-IV criteria for the ADHD subtypes demonstrates that
specific aspects of pragmatic language form part of the diagnostic characteristics for all
three subtypes (Camarata & Gibson, 1999; Westby & Cutler, 1994). These deficits may
influence the quality and/or the frequency of language learning experiences which may,
in-turn, lead to wider language impairments, for example, in semantic and in syntactic
skills (Camarata & Gibson, 1999). Pragmatic impairments may also adversely affect the
performance on standardized assessments of language skill if they interfere with the child’s
ability to evaluate the contextual demands of the task (Oram, Fine, Okamoto, & Tannock,
1999). An investigation of the relations between the specific aspects of pragmatic language
skill and the separate symptoms of ADHD is required to fully understand the extent of
pragmatic language deficits in relation to ADHD and how these affect other aspects of
language use (Camarata & Gibson, 1999; Oram et al., 1999; Westby & Cutler, 1994).
    Assessments of the pragmatic aspects of children’s communicative abilities include
whether or not children can introduce and maintain a topic and their turn taking abilities
during a conversation. These pragmatic behaviours typically emerge between 2 and 3
years of age (Adams, 2002). Teachers are more likely to report these difficulties in boys
with attention problems than in groups with learning disability or average achievement
(Humphries, Koltun, Malone, & Roberts, 1994). Children with ADHD also produce more
inappropriate pragmatic behaviours in unstructured spontaneous conversations with
adults than do typically developing children (Kim & Kaiser, 2000). Other pragmatic
language deficits, such as a failure to take a listener’s perspective into account when
retelling a story, have been found in children with ADHD (Purvis & Tannock, 1997).
    Another aspect of pragmatic language, which was considered in the current
research, is the understanding of language in context. Assessments of this skill often
examine how children interpret figurative language in context, to determine whether or
not the child attends to the context when interpreting figurative expressions such as
idioms, e.g. ‘to get into hot water’ (Adams, 2002). The ability to understand figurative
language has an extended course of development from early childhood through early
adolescence (Nippold & Taylor, 1995), although children as young as 5 years are able to
use context to understand these expressions (Gibbs, 1991). A tendency to interpret
language literally, rather than figuratively, is included in some teacher and parent
checklists of communicative ability (e.g. Bishop, 1998). However, on formal tests that
assess this skill, such as defining words that can take different meanings in different
contexts, children with ADHD do not differ from controls (Purvis & Tannock, 1997).
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




                                                        Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language   501

              This review indicates that children with ADHD may experience different types of
          pragmatic language deficit: impairments in communication and in language
          comprehension in context. Work to date has not looked at the relation between
          pragmatic language skills and inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity separately,
          although the need to explore the language skills of the subtypes of ADHD has been
          widely noted (e.g. Baird, Stevenson, & Williams, 2000; Camarata & Gibson, 1999; Kim &
          Kaiser, 2000; Oram et al., 1999; Westby & Cutler, 1994). Theoretically, it is important to
          determine whether or not inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity make separate and
          distinct contributions to language skills. It is also necessary to determine whether one
          subtype is more at risk of a particular language deficit than another, in order to develop
          effective interventions.
              This research, to the authors’ knowledge, represents the first investigation in the
          literature to consider how hyperactivity/impulsivity and poor attention are separately
          related to school children’s pragmatic language skills. To do this, teachers rated
          children’s attention and hyperactivity using a standardized questionnaire based on the
          DSM-IV classification of ADHD. These ratings were used to identify children with poor
          attention and/or high hyperactivity. A similar selection procedure has been used
          previously by Wilding and colleagues (Wilding, 2003; Wilding, Munir, & Cornish, 2001)
          to explore the relations between attention deficits and different components of
          attention, and by Adams and Snowling (2001) to investigate the relations between
          hyperactivity and executive function and reading impairment.
              Teacher ratings of (hyper)activity and (in)attention usually inform the diagnosis of
          ADHD (Power et al., 1998), although a formal diagnosis requires ratings from different
          informants (e.g. parents as well as teachers) and additional information to eliminate
          other causes of the behaviour. The children in our study were not formally diagnosed
          with ADHD, because we were interested to determine the relations between inattention,
          hyperactivity, and pragmatic language skills in children who were (as far as possible)
          unaffected by additional behavioural problems, such as oppositional defiant disorder and
          conduct disorder, which are often co-morbid. However, our findings can inform
          theoretical models of the relations between inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic
          language skills, which have been developed from the ADHD research literature.
              We report two studies, in which we investigated the relations between poor
          attention and high levels of hyperactivity/impulsivity (hereafter hyperactivity) and the
          interpretation of figurative language in and out of context (Study 1), and pragmatic
          aspects of communication (Study 2). Our aims were to determine whether or not
          children with poor attention and/or high hyperactivity (who do not have a formal
          diagnosis of ADHD) show signs of pragmatic language difficulties and whether or not
          children with predominantly poor attention and predominantly high levels of
          hyperactivity are similarly at risk of pragmatic language impairments.



          STUDY 1: INTERPRETATION OF FIGURATIVE LANGUAGE
          IN AND OUT OF CONTEXT

          Method
          Participants
          Three experimental groups and matched controls participated in this experiment.
          The children were selected from five mainstream suburban primary schools serving
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




502 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain

middle and lower-middle class catchment areas in the East of England. Children whose
first language at home was not English, who had a diagnosis of a hearing, speech, or
language disorder, a formal statement of special educational needs, a diagnosis of ADHD,
or for whom parental consent was not given were excluded from the study.


Assessment of inattention and hyperactivity
Teachers of all children aged 7–11 years completed the two subscales of the ADD-H
Comprehensive Teacher Rating scale (ACTeRS: Ullmann, Sleator, & Sprague, 1999) relating
to attention and hyperactive behaviour. The reliabilities of the two subscales are high:
.93–.97. For the values reported throughout, the attention scale has been reversed thus, for
each scale high scores indicate a tendency towards inattention or hyperactivity. To classify
children we used the following criteria. Scores of between 5 and 9 on the reversed
Attention scale and 10 or less on the Hyperactivity scale were equivalent to the 50th
percentile and considered developmentally appropriate. Scores of between 20 and 30 on
the reversed Attention scale and 16 and 25 on the Hyperactive scale were equivalent to the
25th percentile and classified as ‘poor attention’ and ‘high hyperactivity’, respectively.


Measures of verbal and non-verbal ability
Receptive vocabulary was measured with a group-administered version of the British
Picture Vocabulary scales-II (BPVS-II, Dunn, Dunn, Whetton, & Burley, 1997: see
Stanovich & Cunningham, 1992, for a similar modification). The BPVS is a measure
of receptive vocabulary commonly used as a surrogate measure of verbal ability
(e.g. Adams & Snowling, 2001). The modified test comprised one practice item and 50
test words. The experimenter read out the word and the child ticked the corresponding
picture in their individual booklet. One point was awarded for each correct answer.
The reliability was assessed by calculating Cronbach’s alpha over items and found to be
adequate, a ¼ :78. Non-verbal reasoning ability was assessed with the Matrix Analogies
Test-Expanded Form (MAT-EF, Naglieri, 1985).
    The ACTeRs scores informed selection of three experimental groups: the poor
attention group, the high hyperactivity group, and the poor attention/high hyperactivity
group. Each experimental group had their own control group, comprising children who
had developmentally appropriate scores in the key area for which they acted as controls.
Each experimental group was matched with their appropriate control group on the
following: chronological age, vocabulary, MAT-EF scores, and sex (see Table 1).
In addition, one-way ANOVAs demonstrated that the three experimental groups did not
differ significantly in age, MAT-EF, or vocabulary scores: no Fð2; 44Þ exceeded 1.9 and all
p values were greater than .17.


Materials and procedure
Understanding multiple meanings in context
Children completed a modified version of the Multiple Meanings in Context (MMC)
subtest of the Understanding Ambiguity test (Rinaldi, 1996) to assess their ability to
understand pragmatic, or figurative, interpretations of speech. They were presented
with ten short story dialogues, five of which contained an ambiguous phrase, ‘My little
girl’s room is a real pig sty’, and five of which contained a homonym, e.g. ‘I’ve been
getting very short with Suzie recently’. Each item could take a literal and a figurative
interpretation: the context of the story supported the latter. The task was adapted from
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




                                                                Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language   503

          Table 1. Means (standard deviations) for hyperactivity and inattention ratings, age, vocabulary, and
          non-verbal ability measures in Study 1

                                           Chronological       ACTeRS             ACTeRS       Vocabulary     MAT-EF
                                           age in months     hyperactivity      inattention      score       raw score

          Hyperactive                      117.20 (7.18)    18.73 (2.69)***    9.13 (1.30)    34.80 (4.46) 21.40 (5.05)
            (N ¼ 15; G ¼ 2, B ¼ 13)
          Hyperactive controls             115.20 (12.25)    6.47 (1.60)       8.20 (1.94)    35.60 (4.49) 23.33 (4.19)
            (N ¼ 15; G ¼ 2, B ¼ 13)
          Poor attention                   118.13 (13.42)    7.44 (1.32)      21.94 (2.98)*** 32.38 (5.58) 20.69 (4.98)
            (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 10, B ¼ 6)
          Poor attention controls          116.88 (12.81)    6.88 (1.78)      10.38 (2.66)    33.31 (5.29) 24.44 (4.16)
            (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 10, B ¼ 6)
          Poor attention and hyperactive   115.56 (12.79) 19.44 (3.22)*** 24.38 (2.34)*** 31.75 (4.55) 20.13 (5.38)
            (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 2, B ¼ 14)
          Poor attention and hyperactive   114.06 (12.27)    6.25 (1.92)       7.56 (2.00)    34.25 (4.34) 21.25 (5.29)
            controls (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 2,
            B ¼ 14)

          Note. Significant differences between each experimental group and control are marked in bold:
          ***p , :001; for all other comparisons p . :10. G, number of girls; B, number of boys.

          the original to make it suitable to children who were distractible or inattentive: the
          dialogue was spoken by the experimenter, instead of the audio-recorded presentation,
          which had been distracting to children in previous work.
               After each dialogue children selected one from four pictures that illustrated what a
          character had said, e.g. ‘What does Joanna’s mum mean?’ The pictures illustrated the
          correct figurative and the correct literal interpretations, and an incorrect figurative and
          an incorrect literal interpretation. A ‘don’t know’ response was also available.
          Regardless of their first response, each child was asked the question: ‘Could it mean
          something else?’ to determine whether they knew both meanings (literal and figurative)
          of each item. One practice trial with feedback preceded the experimental trials.
               Two scores were computed and analysed. The number of first choice responses made
          by children was calculated (maximum ¼ 10) to determine whether any of the
          experimental groups had a preference for figurative or literal interpretations relative to
          their controls. The sum of the correct figurative and the correct literal choices made
          either on first or second choice was also calculated. For the latter, the maximum
          possible score of 20 indicates knowledge of both the figurative and the literal meanings of
          all items.

          Knowledge of multiple meanings out of context
          Children completed modified versions of the Ambiguous Sentences and Figurative
          Language subtests from The Test of Language Competence-Expanded Edition (TLC-E:
          Wiig & Secord, 1989) to assess their understanding of figurative language without
          supporting context. Each trial comprised a spoken non-predictive sentence context, e.g.
          ‘Mum looks really low today’ with four accompanying pictures. One picture represented
          the figurative interpretation, one the literal interpretation and two were foils. The child was
          asked ‘Point to the two pictures that it could mean’ with additional prompts, if necessary.
          A demonstration item was presented first, followed by a practice trial. There were five trials
          each of homonyms and phrases. One point was awarded for each target item (figurative or
          literal) chosen on each trial and the results were summed (maximum ¼ 20).
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




504 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain

Results
Understanding multiple meanings in context
First choice responses
The numbers of first choice responses are shown in Table 2. Each experimental group was
significantly less likely to select the correct figurative interpretation relative to their controls:
high hyperactivity vs. controls, tð28Þ ¼ 2:95, p , :01, d ¼ 1:07; poor attention vs.
controls, tð30Þ ¼ 4:14, p , :001, d ¼ 1:89; poor attention/high hyperactivity type vs.
controls, tð30Þ ¼ 3:57, p , :001, d ¼ 1:47. A one-way ANOVA with the experimental
groups demonstrated that their performance did not differ, Fð2; 44Þ ¼ 1:74, p . :17. The
control groups made few errors indicating a strong tendency towards a figurative
interpretation strategy. Therefore, paired sample t tests comparing figurative and literal
responses were computed for the experimental groups, only. None of the experimental
groups revealed either a preference for figurative or literal interpretation: no t exceeded 1.06.


Table 2. Mean number of responses (and standard deviations) obtained on figurative language in
context assessment in Study 1

                                         Total score first choices (max ¼ 10)                             First and second
                                                                                                        choice (max ¼ 20)
                            Figurative         Literal       Figurative Literal            Don’t          Figurative and
                             correct          correct        incorrect incorrect           know           literal correct

Hyperactive               5.53 (3.40)**      3.93   (3.71)   .40   (.63)    .07   (.26)   .07   (.26)     17.33 (2.94)
Hyperactive controls      8.40 (1.68)         .93   (1.34)   .33   (.72)    .33   (.49)   .00   (0)       18.00 (2.36)
Poor attention            3.69 (2.85)***     5.25   (3.17)   .88   (1.14)   .13   (.34)   .63   (.25)     16.13 (1.92)*
Poor attention controls   8.31 (1.96)        1.50   (1.75)   .12   (.34)    .00   (0)     .63   (.25)     18.44 (2.73)
Poor attention and        5.50 (3.29)***     4.00   (3.42)   .25   (.58)    .25   (.45)   .00   (0)       17.00 (2.88)*
  hyperactive
Poor attention and        9.00 (.73)          .88 (.72)      .13 (.34)      .00 (0)       .00 (0)         18.81 (1.22)
  hyperactive controls

Note. Significant differences between each experimental group and their control are in bold:
*p , .05; **p , .01; ***p , .001.


First and second choice responses
The sum of first and second responses is reported in Table 2. The difference between
the high hyperactivity group and their controls was not significant, tð28Þ , 1:0.
The poor attention and poor attention/high hyperactivity groups obtained significantly
lower scores than did their controls: tð30Þ ¼ 2:77, p ¼ :010, d ¼ :98, tð30Þ ¼ 2:32,
p , :03, d ¼ :82, in order.


Knowledge of multiple meanings out of context
The experimental groups obtained lower scores than did their controls. There were no
differences in performance between the high hyperactivity group and the controls:
Ms ¼ 15:60; 16:67; SDs ¼ 3:58; 1:99, tð28Þ ¼ 1:00, p . :20, nor between the poor
attention group and the controls: Ms ¼ 14:44; 15:81; SDs ¼ 3:52; 2:97, tð30Þ ¼ 1:19,
p . :20. The group with poor attention and high hyperactivity differed significantly
from their controls: Ms ¼ 15:44; 17:31; SDs ¼ 2:56; 1:66, tð30Þ ¼ 2:46, p , :025,
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




                                                        Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language   505

          d ¼ :87. A one-way ANOVA with the experimental groups revealed no significant group
          differences, Fð2; 44Þ , 1:0.



          Summary and discussion
          All groups with poor attention and/or high hyperactivity were poor at using context to
          select appropriate meanings for ambiguous forms of language. The effect size for the
          comparison between each experimental group and their controls indicates a sizable
          difference. Furthermore, the experimental groups did not differ from each other,
          suggesting comparable levels of impairments. The analysis of first and second responses
          was used as an indicator of knowledge: did children know both the literal and the
          figurative interpretations of the expression? In this analysis, children with high levels of
          inattention obtained lower scores than did their controls. Children with elevated levels
          of hyperactivity alone were not similarly impaired, indicating that their difficulties on
          the task were not simply attributable to knowledge deficits.
              The out of context task assessed knowledge directly: there was no supportive context
          from which to infer the figurative meaning of the expression. The total scores indicated
          that the experimental groups knew both meanings of most items, although the group
          with both poor attention and elevated hyperactivity differed from their controls on this
          measure. This finding indicates that difficulties with the in context task may be primarily
          due to difficulties in the interpretation of language in context, rather than knowledge.
          In both tasks, the experimental groups demonstrated a reasonable level of knowledge
          for both meanings of the words and phrases, even when they differed significantly
          from their control group. However, when presented with ambiguous expressions in
          contexts that supported a figurative interpretation, the experimental groups were less
          likely than controls, to select the figurative interpretation as their first response.



          STUDY 2: PARENTAL REPORTS OF COMMUNICATION SKILL
          Study 2 focuses on a different aspect of pragmatics: the pragmatic aspects of communi-
          cation. To examine this ability, we used the second edition of the Children’s Communi-
          cation Checklist (CCC: Bishop, 1998; CCC-2: Bishop, 2003), which was designed to
          measure pragmatic and structural aspects of a child’s communication by parents and
          professionals who have regular contact with the child. In the original CCC, five subscales
          were designed to assess the following pragmatic aspects of communication: coherence,
          use of stereotyped language and use of context, instances of inappropriate initiation, and
          conversational rapport. A pragmatic composite derived from these scores reliably
          discriminates children with specific language impairment from children with pragmatic
          language impairment (Bishop, 1998; Botting, 2004). Thus, the CCC is considered a
          reliable indicator of pragmatic language difficulties. Children with ADHD show deficits on
          the pragmatic language subscales of the CCC (Bishop & Baird, 2001; Geurts et al., 2004). It
          is not known whether the subtypes are equally at risk of communicative deficits.
              The CCC-2 is the latest version of this widely used assessment of communication
          skills. It provides standardized scores for two composite scores: a General Communi-
          cation composite, which indicates children who have a significant communication
          problem and a Social Interaction Deviance composite, which indicates children who
          have a communication profile characteristic of children with autistic spectrum disorder
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




506 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain

(Bishop, 2003). The pragmatic composite, included in the CCC, is no longer available,
although the five subscales relating to pragmatic language skills are retained. We
calculated the pragmatic composite to determine whether pragmatic language deficits
were associated with poor attention and high hyperactivity in general, or specific to a
particular behavioural profile (compare with Bishop & Baird, 2001; Geurts et al., 2004).
    The parents of the children who participated in Study 1 and an additional cohort
of children, selected in the same way, were sent the CCC-2 to complete. The aims were
to extend previous work with the CCC (Bishop & Baird, 2001; Geurts et al., 2004) to
investigate: i) whether or not children with poor attention and/or high hyperactivity
(who do not have a formal diagnosis of ADHD) show signs of pragmatic language difficulties
and ii) whether or not children with predominantly poor attention and predominantly
high levels of hyperactivity are similarly at risk of pragmatic language impairments.

Method
Participants
We obtained completed questionnaires for 16 children with high hyperactivity and 18 of
their controls, for 16 children with poor attention and 19 of their controls, and for 14
children with poor attention/high hyperactivity and 17 of their controls. The return rate
of questionnaires from the children who participated in Study 1 was 46% and the return
rate for the additional cohort of children was 56%. Each experimental group was
matched with their appropriate control group on the following: chronological age, sex,
and vocabulary. Two of the experimental groups (poor attention and high hyperactivity)
differed from their controls in the scores obtained on the MAT-EF (an unavoidable
consequence of the sample of returns). One-way ANOVAs demonstrated that the three
experimental groups did not differ significantly in age, MAT-EF, or vocabulary scores: no
Fð2; 43Þ exceeded 1.75 and all p values were greater than .19. The characteristics of
each experimental group and their controls are reported in Table 3.

Table 3. Means (standard deviations) for hyperactivity and inattention ratings, age, vocabulary, and
non-verbal ability measures in Study 2

                            Chronological      ACTeRS            ACTeRS      Vocabulary       MAT-EF
                            age in months    hyperactivity     inattention     score         raw score

Hyperactive                 115.63 (12.34) 18.36 (2.68)***   10.00 (1.90)    33.56 (5.63) 18.25 (4.78)**
  (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 3, B ¼ 13)
Hyperactive controls        117.83 (12.03)   6.11 (1.45)      8.56 (2.57)    34.83 (5.98) 23.44 (4.55)
  (N ¼ 18; G ¼ 7, B ¼ 11)
Poor attention              110.31 (13.50)   7.31 (1.30)     22.75 (2.98)*** 30.19 (5.56) 19.38 (5.93)*
  (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 9, B ¼ 7)
Poor attention controls     118.53 (13.53)   6.95 (1.78)     10.26 (3.05)    33.53 (5.68) 22.95 (3.60)
  (N ¼ 19; G ¼ 13, B ¼ 6)
Poor attention and          114.07 (11.55) 18.86 (2.91)*** 24.00 (3.37)*** 32.07 (4.01)     18.64 (4.87)
  hyperactive (N ¼ 14;
  G ¼ 6, B ¼ 8)
Poor attention and          113.71 (13.61)   5.94 (1.52)      8.24 (2.31)    32.88 (5.21)   20.76 (4.48)
  hyperactive controls
  (N ¼ 17; G ¼ 4, B ¼ 13)

Note. Significant differences between each experimental group and control are marked in bold and
significance levels indicated as follows: *p , :05; **p , :01; ***p , :001; for all other comparisons
p . :10. G, number of girls; B, number of boys.
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




                                                        Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language   507

          Results
          We calculated the General Communication (GCC) and the Social Interaction Deviance
          (SIDC) composites as directed in the CCC-2. A pragmatic composite based on the
          original CCC was also calculated. These scores are reported in Table 4.
             The high hyperactivity group did not differ from their controls on any of the
          composite scores: no t exceeded 1.0, and the three experimental groups did not differ
          from each other on any of the composite scores, all Fsð2; 43Þ , 1:03, all ps . :37.
          The poor attention group obtained significantly lower scores than their controls on the
          GCC, tð33Þ ¼ 2:44, p ¼ :02, d ¼ :81. The mean percentile equivalents were 24 and 50,
          respectively. The poor attention and high hyperactivity group obtained lower scores
          than their controls, but the difference was not significant, tð29Þ ¼ 1:86, p ¼ :078,
          d ¼ :68. The mean percentile equivalents were 32 and 58, respectively. Neither of these
          groups differed from their controls on the SIDC, both ts , 1:0, however, both groups
          with poor attention differed from their respective controls on the pragmatic composite:
          poor attention group vs. controls, tð33Þ ¼ 2:44, p , :025, d ¼ :81; poor attention/high
          hyperactivity group vs. controls, tð29Þ ¼ 2:37, p , :03, d ¼ :84. Neither vocabulary
          scores nor non-verbal reasoning scores (MAT-EF) were correlated with any of the
          composite scores, no r exceeded .19 and thus none were significant (N ¼ 100).


          Summary and discussion
          Children with poor attention showed evidence of weak communication skills compared
          with their controls; children with high hyperactivity but good attention did not show
          signs of communicative impairments. Children with both poor attention and high levels
          of hyperactivity did not have wider-ranging difficulties with social interaction: no
          differences were found on the SIDC. Of note, there was considerable variability within
          both groups with poor attention (indexed by the standard deviations) suggested that
          communication difficulties experienced by these children were not uniform.


          GENERAL DISCUSSION
          The important finding from this research was that the primary behavioural deficits
          of ADHD, poor attention, and elevated hyperactivity, were associated with impairments
          in pragmatic aspects of communication and language comprehension. Children
          with poor attention obtained lower scores than matched controls on a formal test of
          language interpretation in context and a parental assessment of communicative skills.
          Children with elevated hyperactivity were impaired on the assessment of language
          interpretation in context but their communication skills were not rated as impaired.
          This study should be considered exploratory: we did not include a full range of
          pragmatic language assessments and we did not assess children with a diagnosis
          of ADHD. However, this work has important implications for theoretical models that
          posit relations between pragmatic language skills, inattention, hyperactivity, and it
          indicates directions for future research with children who have ADHD. We discuss
          theoretical models, how they relate to our current findings, and directions for future
          work, below.
              Theoretical models of the relations between inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic
          language skills have come from the ADHD literature. These models propose that children
          with ADHD may experience pragmatic language difficulties for, at least, two reasons.
508 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain
Table 4. Mean standard and composite scores (and standard deviations) obtained on parental CCC

                                                                                                                    Poor attention       Poor attention and
                          Hyperactive      Hyperactive controls      Poor attention     Poor attention controls     and hyperactive     hyperactive controls

Pragmatic composite      47.25 (13.56)         48.22 (13.16)        39.75* (15.42)          50.37* (10.17)           41.71* (17.32)        54.53* (12.82)
GC composite             77.81 (18.92)         77.56 (18.55)        66.38* (23.38)          82.05* (14.27)            71.50 (25.44)         86.00 (17.93)
SID composite             6.69 (6.71)           6.56 (4.48)           4.63 (8.07)             4.84 (3.84)              3.79 (10.14)          3.94 (4.78)

Note. Significant difference between each experimental group and their control are signalled in bold, *p , :05; GC composite, General Communication composite;
SID composite, Social Interaction Deviance composite.
                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                              Copyright © The British Psychological Society
                                                                                                                                                                                                  Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




                                                        Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language   509

          ADHD may result from poor behavioural inhibition, which affects executive control and
          leads to problems with attention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity (Barkley, 1997).
          Pragmatic language use taps into executive skills such as planning, organizing, and/or
          monitoring behaviours. Thus, pragmatic language deficits might arise from the cognitive
          deficits that cause the behavioural symptoms of ADHD (e.g. Purvis & Tannock, 1997).
          This theory might explain the difficulties with figurative language in context experienced
          by all groups in this work. An inability to monitor and evaluate the appropriate context
          could lead to the literal interpretation of a figurative expression.
              An alternative, though not mutually exclusive, hypothesis is that the behavioural
          characteristics of the different subtypes of ADHD influence the quality and the
          frequency of their interactions with caregivers, which adversely affects their pragmatic
          language development (e.g. Camarata & Gibson, 1999). For example, poor attention
          may lead children to miss important environmental and conversational cues, which
          limits their ability to learn the range of meanings conveyed by different words and
          phrases. There was little evidence for deficits in knowledge of multiple meanings of
          words and phrases in the current work. However, the children in this study did not have
          a diagnosis of ADHD, thus we might infer that any impairment in attention or
          hyperactivity was less severe than that found in clinical populations and therefore might
          have less serious consequences for language learning. We found no evidence for a
          greater impairment in any group: that may not necessarily be the case for diagnosed
          children where predominantly inattentive or hyperactive children might suffer more.
          Further work is needed to disentangle the relative contributions of attention and
          hyperactivity to language learning and language comprehension in both nondiagnosed
          and clinical populations.
              There was some evidence that children with poor attention had poorer
          communication skills than the children with only high hyperactivity. The effect sizes
          indicated moderately sized differences from controls but the three experimental
          groups did not obtain statistically different ratings from each other on the CCC-2. Our
          measure of communicative skill was questionnaire based and, although it was
          completed by parents who presumably reflected on their own interactions with their
          child, analysis of naturalistic interactions with adults is also desirable as well as
          replication of these findings. Analysis of the behavioural characteristics of ADHD,
          suggests that both subtypes should show evidence of conversational weaknesses. Again,
          difficulties that were not apparent in our high hyperactive group might be apparent in a
          diagnosed sample.
              Children with high levels of inattention may be at risk of a greater range of pragmatic
          language impairments than children who are predominantly hyperactive, but we did not
          find an association between elevated hyperactivity and communicative weaknesses. In
          relation to children with ADHD, these findings suggest that all subtypes may be weak at
          interpreting figurative language in context and that the inattention and combined
          subtypes may be at risk of communicative impairments in addition. Our tight group
          matching means that such weaknesses may be apparent over and above any other
          deficits in verbal ability. We studied an age range (8–11 years) in which substantial
          development in figurative language comprehension is found (Gibbs, 1991; Nippold &
          Taylor, 1995). This may have lead to the large standard deviations apparent on some
          measures. Future work might compare the time course of the development of different
          forms of figurative language comprehension and pragmatic language in ADHD subtypes
          to establish which aspects are to delayed and which are deviant in children with poor
          attention and/poor high hyperactivity.
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




510 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain

    Cohen et al. (1998) suggest that the language difficulties experienced by children
with ADHD may lead to their social difficulties. Therefore, a greater understanding of
this population’s language deficits and why they arise has important implications and
needs to be addressed. We have begun to address this issue in the reported work. In
contrast to work with diagnosed populations (see Milich, Balentine, & Lynham, 2001),
we found no evidence for impaired social skills in our sample. However, our test
instrument (the CCC-2) was developed to look at the difficulties with language and
social interaction experienced by children with autism spectrum disorders; assessments
of more general social skills may reveal difficulties where we found none. In addition,
it must be remembered that the children in this study were not diagnosed with
ADHD, thus their difficulties may not have been as pronounced as those who had been
clinically assessed.
    In summary, we have shown that poor attention and high hyperactivity are
differently associated with pragmatic language skills: high levels of hyperactivity and
poor attention are associated with impairments in the comprehension of figurative
language in context; poor attention is additionally associated with impairments in
pragmatic aspects of communication.
    These findings are consistent with the idea of a continuum of attentional and activity
problems. They indicate clear directions for future work. First, we need to consider how
individual differences in attention and/or hyperactivity affect the language and learning of
nondiagnosed children. Our methodology enables the investigation of the symptoms of
ADHD in relation to cognitive skills of theoretical interest without the confounding factors
of the accompanying deficits found in many clinically diagnosed individuals. Second,
future work should determine whether clinically diagnosed subtypes can be differentiated
on the basis of their pragmatic language skills: our work suggests that this might be the
case. If so, different types of intervention will be required for the subtypes of ADHD.


Acknowledgements
This work was conducted as part of the first author’s doctoral thesis and was supported by a University
of Essex teaching studentship. We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their
constructive comments on an earlier version of this work. Finally, many thanks to the teachers and
children at the following primary schools in Essex who participated in this work: Capel St Mary, Eight
Ash Green, Great Bentley, Holland Park, Kirby, Morland, Rolph, Springfield, St Johns, Wentworth.


References
Adams, C. (2002). Practitioner review: The assessment of language pragmatics. Journal of Child
    Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 973–987.
Adams, J. W., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). Executive function and reading impairments in children
    reported by their teachers as hyperactive. British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
    19, 293–306.
American Psychological Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
    disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: APA.
Baird, J., Stevenson, J. C., & Williams, D. C. (2000). The evolution of ADHD: A disorder of
    communication? Quarterly Review of Biology, 75, 17–35.
Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford Press.
Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC):
    A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal
    of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–891.
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




                                                              Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language   511

          Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Children’s Communication Checklist-Revised (CCC-2) (2nd. ed.).
              London: The Psychological Corporation.
          Bishop, D. V. M., & Baird, G. (2001). Parent and teacher report of pragmatic aspects of
              communication: Use of the Children’s Communication Checklist in a clinical setting.
              Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 809–818.
          Botting, N. (2004). Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC) scores in 11-year-old children with
              communication impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication
              Disorders, 39, 215–227.
          Camarata, S. M., & Gibson, T. (1999). Pragmatic language deficits in attention-deficit hyperactivity
              disorder (ADHD). Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews,
              5, 207–214.
          Cohen, N. J., Menna, R., Vallance, D. D., Barwick, M. A., Im, N., & Horodeszky, N. B. (1998).
              Language, social cognitive processing, and behavioral characteristics, of psychiatrically
              disturbed children with previously identified and unsuspected language impairments. Journal
              of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 853–864.
          Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). British picture vocabulary scale
              (2nd. ed). Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson.
                               ´
          Geurts, H. M., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Hartman, C. A., Mulder, E. J., et al. (2004). Can the
              Children’s Communication Checklist differentiate between children with autism, children with
              ADHD, and normal controls? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1437–1453.
          Gibbs, R. W. (1991). Semantic analyzability in children’s understanding of idioms. Journal of
              Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 613–620.
          Humphries, T., Koltun, H., Malone, M., & Roberts, W. (1994). Teacher-identified oral language
              difficulties among boys with attention problems. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
              15, 92–98.
          Kim, O. H., & Kaiser, A. P. (2000). Language characteristics of children with ADHD.
              Communication Disorders Quarterly, 21, 154–165.
          Levy, F., Hay, D. A., Mcstephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-deficit
              hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study.
              Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 737–744.
          Milich, R., Balentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD combined type and ADHD
              predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology:
              Science and Practice, 8, 463–488.
          Naglieri, J. A. (1985). The matrix analogies test: Expanded form. New York: The Psychological
              Corporation.
          Nippold, M. A., & Taylor, C. L. (1995). Idiom understanding in youth: Further examination of
              familiarity and transparency. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 426–433.
          Oram, J., Fine, J., Okamoto, C., & Tannock, R. (1999). Assessing the language of children with atten-
              tion deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 72–80.
          Power, T. J., Andrews, T. J., Eiraldi, R. B., Doherty, B. J., Ikeda, M. J., Dupaul, G. J., & Landau, S.
              (1998). Evaluating attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using multiple informants:
              The incremental utility of combining teacher with parent reports. Psychological Assessment,
              10, 250–260.
          Purvis, K. L., & Tannock, R. (1997). Language abilities in children with attention deficit
              hyperactivity disorder, reading disabilities, and normal controls. Journal of Abnormal Child
              Psychology, 25, 133–144.
          Rinaldi, W. (1996). Understanding ambiguity: An assessment of pragmatic meaning
              comprehension. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
          Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1992). Studying the consequences of literacy within a lite-
              rate society: The cognitive correlates of print exposure. Memory and Cognition, 20, 51–68.
          Tirosh, E., & Cohen, A. (1998). Language deficit with attention-deficit disorder: A prevalent
              comorbidity. Journal of Child Neurology, 13, 493–497.
Copyright © The British Psychological Society
Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society




512 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain

Ullmann, R. K., Sleator, E. K., & Sprague, R. L. (1999). The ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher’s
   Rating Scale (ACTeRS) (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: MetriTech Inc.
Westby, C. E., & Cutler, S. K. (1994). Language and ADHD – understanding the bases and treatment
   of self-regulatory deficits. Topics in Language Disorders, 14, 58–76.
Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (1989). Test of Language Competence – Expanded Edition (TLC-E).
   San Antonio: Psychological Corporation.
Wilding, J. M. (2003). Attentional difficulties in children: Weakness in executive function or
   problems in coping with difficult tasks? British Journal of Psychology, 94, 427–436.
Wilding, J., Munir, F., & Cornish, K. (2001). The nature of attentional differences between groups
   of children differentiated by teacher ratings of attention and hyperactivity. British Journal of
   Psychology, 92, 357–371.

Received 17 January 2006; revised version received 20 November 2006

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

Unsuspected language impairments
Unsuspected language impairmentsUnsuspected language impairments
Unsuspected language impairmentsRALLICampaign
 
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"Dorothy Bishop
 
Argentina talk references
Argentina talk referencesArgentina talk references
Argentina talk referencesDorothy Bishop
 
16.arias trejo barrn-martnez-lgskillsindownsyndromeinauza2017
16.arias trejo barrn-martnez-lgskillsindownsyndromeinauza201716.arias trejo barrn-martnez-lgskillsindownsyndromeinauza2017
16.arias trejo barrn-martnez-lgskillsindownsyndromeinauza2017samuel vizcarra grossverger
 
Age As An Individual Difference In Sla
Age As An Individual Difference In SlaAge As An Individual Difference In Sla
Age As An Individual Difference In SlaDr. Cupid Lucid
 
SLI: diagnostic dilemmas
SLI: diagnostic dilemmasSLI: diagnostic dilemmas
SLI: diagnostic dilemmasDorothy Bishop
 
Autism and SLI: diagnostic distinctions
Autism and SLI: diagnostic distinctionsAutism and SLI: diagnostic distinctions
Autism and SLI: diagnostic distinctionsDorothy Bishop
 
The influence of the basic difference between everyday meaning of English wor...
The influence of the basic difference between everyday meaning of English wor...The influence of the basic difference between everyday meaning of English wor...
The influence of the basic difference between everyday meaning of English wor...iosrjce
 
State-of-Science Review: Specific Language Impairment
State-of-Science Review: Specific Language ImpairmentState-of-Science Review: Specific Language Impairment
State-of-Science Review: Specific Language ImpairmentDorothy Bishop
 
Genes, cognition and communication
Genes, cognition and communicationGenes, cognition and communication
Genes, cognition and communicationDorothy Bishop
 
My New Article- Kamalata Lukama
My New Article- Kamalata LukamaMy New Article- Kamalata Lukama
My New Article- Kamalata Lukamakamalata lukama
 
Social Communication Disorder
Social Communication DisorderSocial Communication Disorder
Social Communication DisorderVeer Singh
 
Identification of SLI: references
Identification of SLI: referencesIdentification of SLI: references
Identification of SLI: referencesRALLICampaign
 
How does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageHow does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageDorothy Bishop
 

Was ist angesagt? (20)

Unsuspected language impairments
Unsuspected language impairmentsUnsuspected language impairments
Unsuspected language impairments
 
Apttde
ApttdeApttde
Apttde
 
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
 
b
bb
b
 
Argentina talk references
Argentina talk referencesArgentina talk references
Argentina talk references
 
16.arias trejo barrn-martnez-lgskillsindownsyndromeinauza2017
16.arias trejo barrn-martnez-lgskillsindownsyndromeinauza201716.arias trejo barrn-martnez-lgskillsindownsyndromeinauza2017
16.arias trejo barrn-martnez-lgskillsindownsyndromeinauza2017
 
Age
AgeAge
Age
 
Age As An Individual Difference In Sla
Age As An Individual Difference In SlaAge As An Individual Difference In Sla
Age As An Individual Difference In Sla
 
SLI: diagnostic dilemmas
SLI: diagnostic dilemmasSLI: diagnostic dilemmas
SLI: diagnostic dilemmas
 
Autism and SLI: diagnostic distinctions
Autism and SLI: diagnostic distinctionsAutism and SLI: diagnostic distinctions
Autism and SLI: diagnostic distinctions
 
The influence of the basic difference between everyday meaning of English wor...
The influence of the basic difference between everyday meaning of English wor...The influence of the basic difference between everyday meaning of English wor...
The influence of the basic difference between everyday meaning of English wor...
 
State-of-Science Review: Specific Language Impairment
State-of-Science Review: Specific Language ImpairmentState-of-Science Review: Specific Language Impairment
State-of-Science Review: Specific Language Impairment
 
How common is SLI?
How common is SLI?How common is SLI?
How common is SLI?
 
Individual Differences2
Individual Differences2Individual Differences2
Individual Differences2
 
Genes, cognition and communication
Genes, cognition and communicationGenes, cognition and communication
Genes, cognition and communication
 
My New Article- Kamalata Lukama
My New Article- Kamalata LukamaMy New Article- Kamalata Lukama
My New Article- Kamalata Lukama
 
Social Communication Disorder
Social Communication DisorderSocial Communication Disorder
Social Communication Disorder
 
Practical implications for_the_monolingual_therapist
Practical implications for_the_monolingual_therapistPractical implications for_the_monolingual_therapist
Practical implications for_the_monolingual_therapist
 
Identification of SLI: references
Identification of SLI: referencesIdentification of SLI: references
Identification of SLI: references
 
How does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn languageHow does the brain learn language
How does the brain learn language
 

Andere mochten auch

Section 7 - Comorbidity in ADHD and Autism
Section 7 - Comorbidity in ADHD and AutismSection 7 - Comorbidity in ADHD and Autism
Section 7 - Comorbidity in ADHD and AutismSimon Bignell
 
Birtud Para sa Kabutihang Panlahat, Aking NiIinang
Birtud Para sa Kabutihang Panlahat, Aking NiIinangBirtud Para sa Kabutihang Panlahat, Aking NiIinang
Birtud Para sa Kabutihang Panlahat, Aking NiIinangLea Sandra F. Banzon
 
Chapter 4 - The Earth's Atmosphere
Chapter 4 - The Earth's AtmosphereChapter 4 - The Earth's Atmosphere
Chapter 4 - The Earth's AtmosphereLea Sandra F. Banzon
 
Cognition & Development: Conceptualisations of Self and Identity
Cognition & Development: Conceptualisations of Self and IdentityCognition & Development: Conceptualisations of Self and Identity
Cognition & Development: Conceptualisations of Self and IdentitySimon Bignell
 
Cognition & Development: Vygotsky
Cognition & Development: VygotskyCognition & Development: Vygotsky
Cognition & Development: VygotskySimon Bignell
 
Cognitive Develepment - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 2
Cognitive Develepment - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 2Cognitive Develepment - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 2
Cognitive Develepment - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 2Simon Bignell
 
Lecture 15:Impairment, disabilty & handicap-DR.Naif
Lecture 15:Impairment, disabilty & handicap-DR.NaifLecture 15:Impairment, disabilty & handicap-DR.Naif
Lecture 15:Impairment, disabilty & handicap-DR.NaifAHS_student
 
The Development of the Self - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 4
The Development of the Self - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 4The Development of the Self - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 4
The Development of the Self - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 4Simon Bignell
 
Medical Quackery and Traditional Medicine.
Medical Quackery and Traditional Medicine.Medical Quackery and Traditional Medicine.
Medical Quackery and Traditional Medicine.Lea Sandra F. Banzon
 
Module 2- The Stages of Development and Developmental Tasks
Module 2- The Stages of Development and Developmental TasksModule 2- The Stages of Development and Developmental Tasks
Module 2- The Stages of Development and Developmental Taskstin072787
 
Early Childhood Care and Development (ECCD)
Early Childhood Care and Development (ECCD)Early Childhood Care and Development (ECCD)
Early Childhood Care and Development (ECCD)Julius Patrick Saletrero
 
Stages of Development and Developmental Tasks
Stages of Development and Developmental TasksStages of Development and Developmental Tasks
Stages of Development and Developmental TasksLea Sandra F. Banzon
 
Field Study 6: Episode 1
Field Study 6: Episode 1Field Study 6: Episode 1
Field Study 6: Episode 1Jenny Reyes
 
Early Childhood Development Module 2
Early Childhood Development Module 2Early Childhood Development Module 2
Early Childhood Development Module 2Future Managers
 
Early childhood education powerpoint
Early childhood education powerpointEarly childhood education powerpoint
Early childhood education powerpointsamb97
 
Early Childhood Education SlideShare- Final Draft
Early Childhood Education SlideShare- Final DraftEarly Childhood Education SlideShare- Final Draft
Early Childhood Education SlideShare- Final DraftBreanna Bennett
 
Vygotsky Theory
Vygotsky Theory Vygotsky Theory
Vygotsky Theory jkravit
 
Stages of child development
Stages of child developmentStages of child development
Stages of child developmentLiris Thomas
 

Andere mochten auch (20)

Section 7 - Comorbidity in ADHD and Autism
Section 7 - Comorbidity in ADHD and AutismSection 7 - Comorbidity in ADHD and Autism
Section 7 - Comorbidity in ADHD and Autism
 
5 x 7 in. (2)
5 x 7 in. (2)5 x 7 in. (2)
5 x 7 in. (2)
 
Birtud Para sa Kabutihang Panlahat, Aking NiIinang
Birtud Para sa Kabutihang Panlahat, Aking NiIinangBirtud Para sa Kabutihang Panlahat, Aking NiIinang
Birtud Para sa Kabutihang Panlahat, Aking NiIinang
 
Chapter 4 - The Earth's Atmosphere
Chapter 4 - The Earth's AtmosphereChapter 4 - The Earth's Atmosphere
Chapter 4 - The Earth's Atmosphere
 
Cognition & Development: Conceptualisations of Self and Identity
Cognition & Development: Conceptualisations of Self and IdentityCognition & Development: Conceptualisations of Self and Identity
Cognition & Development: Conceptualisations of Self and Identity
 
Arnold gessell pp
Arnold gessell ppArnold gessell pp
Arnold gessell pp
 
Cognition & Development: Vygotsky
Cognition & Development: VygotskyCognition & Development: Vygotsky
Cognition & Development: Vygotsky
 
Cognitive Develepment - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 2
Cognitive Develepment - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 2Cognitive Develepment - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 2
Cognitive Develepment - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 2
 
Lecture 15:Impairment, disabilty & handicap-DR.Naif
Lecture 15:Impairment, disabilty & handicap-DR.NaifLecture 15:Impairment, disabilty & handicap-DR.Naif
Lecture 15:Impairment, disabilty & handicap-DR.Naif
 
The Development of the Self - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 4
The Development of the Self - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 4The Development of the Self - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 4
The Development of the Self - Fundamentals of Psychology 2 - Lecture 4
 
Medical Quackery and Traditional Medicine.
Medical Quackery and Traditional Medicine.Medical Quackery and Traditional Medicine.
Medical Quackery and Traditional Medicine.
 
Module 2- The Stages of Development and Developmental Tasks
Module 2- The Stages of Development and Developmental TasksModule 2- The Stages of Development and Developmental Tasks
Module 2- The Stages of Development and Developmental Tasks
 
Early Childhood Care and Development (ECCD)
Early Childhood Care and Development (ECCD)Early Childhood Care and Development (ECCD)
Early Childhood Care and Development (ECCD)
 
Stages of Development and Developmental Tasks
Stages of Development and Developmental TasksStages of Development and Developmental Tasks
Stages of Development and Developmental Tasks
 
Field Study 6: Episode 1
Field Study 6: Episode 1Field Study 6: Episode 1
Field Study 6: Episode 1
 
Early Childhood Development Module 2
Early Childhood Development Module 2Early Childhood Development Module 2
Early Childhood Development Module 2
 
Early childhood education powerpoint
Early childhood education powerpointEarly childhood education powerpoint
Early childhood education powerpoint
 
Early Childhood Education SlideShare- Final Draft
Early Childhood Education SlideShare- Final DraftEarly Childhood Education SlideShare- Final Draft
Early Childhood Education SlideShare- Final Draft
 
Vygotsky Theory
Vygotsky Theory Vygotsky Theory
Vygotsky Theory
 
Stages of child development
Stages of child developmentStages of child development
Stages of child development
 

Ähnlich wie Bignell_&_Cain_(2007)

A Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum Conditions
A Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum ConditionsA Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum Conditions
A Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum ConditionsKim Daniels
 
dual language
dual language dual language
dual language hemathiaga
 
Selecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docx
Selecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docxSelecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docx
Selecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docxbagotjesusa
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxcroysierkathey
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLkarenahmanny4c
 
Presentation
PresentationPresentation
Presentationnuurza
 
Full document of asdc sign language for all english
Full document of asdc sign language for all englishFull document of asdc sign language for all english
Full document of asdc sign language for all englishCfreeland1
 
Early Language Acquisition
Early Language AcquisitionEarly Language Acquisition
Early Language AcquisitionAsma Almashad
 
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptxPROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptxSamuelAgboola11
 
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptxPROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptxSamuelAgboola11
 
Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explained
Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explainedDevelopmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explained
Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explainedRADLD
 
Report on teaching beginning readers
Report on teaching beginning readersReport on teaching beginning readers
Report on teaching beginning readersWriters Per Hour
 
Preschool Children With Special Needs:communication and language development
Preschool Children With Special Needs:communication and language developmentPreschool Children With Special Needs:communication and language development
Preschool Children With Special Needs:communication and language developmentArianny Calcagno
 
ch. 10 - toward a theory of 2nd language acquisition revised new.pptx
ch. 10 - toward a theory of 2nd language acquisition revised new.pptxch. 10 - toward a theory of 2nd language acquisition revised new.pptx
ch. 10 - toward a theory of 2nd language acquisition revised new.pptxGabrielaUrdea
 

Ähnlich wie Bignell_&_Cain_(2007) (20)

A Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum Conditions
A Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum ConditionsA Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum Conditions
A Qualitative Case Study On A Bilingual Child With Autism Spectrum Conditions
 
dual language
dual language dual language
dual language
 
Selecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docx
Selecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docxSelecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docx
Selecting A topicJob PositionFor the first part of the project,.docx
 
Age
AgeAge
Age
 
bilingualism
bilingualism bilingualism
bilingualism
 
Exx 502 Eb Chapter9 Part 1 2
Exx 502  Eb Chapter9 Part 1 2Exx 502  Eb Chapter9 Part 1 2
Exx 502 Eb Chapter9 Part 1 2
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docxJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL.docx
 
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILLJOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSISIMPROVING SOCIAL SKILL
 
Presentation
PresentationPresentation
Presentation
 
Full document of asdc sign language for all english
Full document of asdc sign language for all englishFull document of asdc sign language for all english
Full document of asdc sign language for all english
 
Principles of language learning and teaching
Principles of language learning and teachingPrinciples of language learning and teaching
Principles of language learning and teaching
 
Early Language Acquisition
Early Language AcquisitionEarly Language Acquisition
Early Language Acquisition
 
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptxPROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
 
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptxPROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
PROPOSAL POWERPOINT PRESENTATION.pptx
 
Apa citation
Apa citationApa citation
Apa citation
 
Apa citation
Apa citationApa citation
Apa citation
 
Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explained
Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explainedDevelopmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explained
Developmental Language Disorder (DLD): The consensus explained
 
Report on teaching beginning readers
Report on teaching beginning readersReport on teaching beginning readers
Report on teaching beginning readers
 
Preschool Children With Special Needs:communication and language development
Preschool Children With Special Needs:communication and language developmentPreschool Children With Special Needs:communication and language development
Preschool Children With Special Needs:communication and language development
 
ch. 10 - toward a theory of 2nd language acquisition revised new.pptx
ch. 10 - toward a theory of 2nd language acquisition revised new.pptxch. 10 - toward a theory of 2nd language acquisition revised new.pptx
ch. 10 - toward a theory of 2nd language acquisition revised new.pptx
 

Mehr von Simon Bignell

MCS Info Sheet 1 Download
MCS Info Sheet 1 DownloadMCS Info Sheet 1 Download
MCS Info Sheet 1 DownloadSimon Bignell
 
The Psychology of Human-Computer Interaction
The Psychology ofHuman-Computer InteractionThe Psychology ofHuman-Computer Interaction
The Psychology of Human-Computer InteractionSimon Bignell
 
Using Multi-User Virtual Worlds for Research and Education
Using Multi-User Virtual Worlds for Research and Education Using Multi-User Virtual Worlds for Research and Education
Using Multi-User Virtual Worlds for Research and Education Simon Bignell
 
Technology in Teaching, Research & Admin’: Some Quick Wins & Data Protection
Technology in Teaching, Research & Admin’: Some Quick Wins & Data ProtectionTechnology in Teaching, Research & Admin’: Some Quick Wins & Data Protection
Technology in Teaching, Research & Admin’: Some Quick Wins & Data ProtectionSimon Bignell
 
Autism Asperger's & ADHD - Introduction to the Module (2014)
Autism Asperger's & ADHD - Introduction to the Module (2014)Autism Asperger's & ADHD - Introduction to the Module (2014)
Autism Asperger's & ADHD - Introduction to the Module (2014)Simon Bignell
 
Cognition & Development: Social Development
Cognition & Development: Social DevelopmentCognition & Development: Social Development
Cognition & Development: Social DevelopmentSimon Bignell
 
3D Virtual Avatars and Machinima to Educate Students about Sustainability
3D Virtual Avatars and Machinima to Educate Students about Sustainability3D Virtual Avatars and Machinima to Educate Students about Sustainability
3D Virtual Avatars and Machinima to Educate Students about SustainabilitySimon Bignell
 
Social and Problem-Based Learning in Cyberspace: Tools, Techniques & Technolo...
Social and Problem-Based Learning in Cyberspace: Tools, Techniques & Technolo...Social and Problem-Based Learning in Cyberspace: Tools, Techniques & Technolo...
Social and Problem-Based Learning in Cyberspace: Tools, Techniques & Technolo...Simon Bignell
 
Sustainability Education Using an Online Virtual World: The PREVIEW-Sustain...
Sustainability Education Using an Online Virtual World:  The  PREVIEW-Sustain...Sustainability Education Using an Online Virtual World:  The  PREVIEW-Sustain...
Sustainability Education Using an Online Virtual World: The PREVIEW-Sustain...Simon Bignell
 
Using Second Life Avatars and Machinima to Introduce Sustainability into the ...
Using Second Life Avatars and Machinima to Introduce Sustainability into the ...Using Second Life Avatars and Machinima to Introduce Sustainability into the ...
Using Second Life Avatars and Machinima to Introduce Sustainability into the ...Simon Bignell
 
MyChild Services Information Sheet (755kb)
MyChild Services Information Sheet (755kb)MyChild Services Information Sheet (755kb)
MyChild Services Information Sheet (755kb)Simon Bignell
 
AAA Section 04 Asperger's Disorder Ver 04 2013
AAA Section 04 Asperger's Disorder Ver 04 2013AAA Section 04 Asperger's Disorder Ver 04 2013
AAA Section 04 Asperger's Disorder Ver 04 2013Simon Bignell
 
AAA Section 03 Autism Spectrum Disorder Ver 03 2013
AAA Section 03 Autism Spectrum Disorder Ver 03 2013AAA Section 03 Autism Spectrum Disorder Ver 03 2013
AAA Section 03 Autism Spectrum Disorder Ver 03 2013Simon Bignell
 
AAA Section 02 ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder Ver 04 2013
AAA Section 02 ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder Ver 04 2013AAA Section 02 ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder Ver 04 2013
AAA Section 02 ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder Ver 04 2013Simon Bignell
 
AAA Section 01 Introduction to the Module Ver 02 2013
AAA Section 01 Introduction to the Module Ver 02 2013AAA Section 01 Introduction to the Module Ver 02 2013
AAA Section 01 Introduction to the Module Ver 02 2013Simon Bignell
 
FOP2 Supplementary Coursework Instructions
FOP2 Supplementary Coursework InstructionsFOP2 Supplementary Coursework Instructions
FOP2 Supplementary Coursework InstructionsSimon Bignell
 
AAA Section 11 Revision and the Campus Exam
AAA Section 11 Revision and the Campus ExamAAA Section 11 Revision and the Campus Exam
AAA Section 11 Revision and the Campus ExamSimon Bignell
 
The literature review
The literature reviewThe literature review
The literature reviewSimon Bignell
 
Critically reading a paper
Critically reading a paperCritically reading a paper
Critically reading a paperSimon Bignell
 
Section 11 (On-Campus) - Revision and the Exam
Section 11 (On-Campus) - Revision and the ExamSection 11 (On-Campus) - Revision and the Exam
Section 11 (On-Campus) - Revision and the ExamSimon Bignell
 

Mehr von Simon Bignell (20)

MCS Info Sheet 1 Download
MCS Info Sheet 1 DownloadMCS Info Sheet 1 Download
MCS Info Sheet 1 Download
 
The Psychology of Human-Computer Interaction
The Psychology ofHuman-Computer InteractionThe Psychology ofHuman-Computer Interaction
The Psychology of Human-Computer Interaction
 
Using Multi-User Virtual Worlds for Research and Education
Using Multi-User Virtual Worlds for Research and Education Using Multi-User Virtual Worlds for Research and Education
Using Multi-User Virtual Worlds for Research and Education
 
Technology in Teaching, Research & Admin’: Some Quick Wins & Data Protection
Technology in Teaching, Research & Admin’: Some Quick Wins & Data ProtectionTechnology in Teaching, Research & Admin’: Some Quick Wins & Data Protection
Technology in Teaching, Research & Admin’: Some Quick Wins & Data Protection
 
Autism Asperger's & ADHD - Introduction to the Module (2014)
Autism Asperger's & ADHD - Introduction to the Module (2014)Autism Asperger's & ADHD - Introduction to the Module (2014)
Autism Asperger's & ADHD - Introduction to the Module (2014)
 
Cognition & Development: Social Development
Cognition & Development: Social DevelopmentCognition & Development: Social Development
Cognition & Development: Social Development
 
3D Virtual Avatars and Machinima to Educate Students about Sustainability
3D Virtual Avatars and Machinima to Educate Students about Sustainability3D Virtual Avatars and Machinima to Educate Students about Sustainability
3D Virtual Avatars and Machinima to Educate Students about Sustainability
 
Social and Problem-Based Learning in Cyberspace: Tools, Techniques & Technolo...
Social and Problem-Based Learning in Cyberspace: Tools, Techniques & Technolo...Social and Problem-Based Learning in Cyberspace: Tools, Techniques & Technolo...
Social and Problem-Based Learning in Cyberspace: Tools, Techniques & Technolo...
 
Sustainability Education Using an Online Virtual World: The PREVIEW-Sustain...
Sustainability Education Using an Online Virtual World:  The  PREVIEW-Sustain...Sustainability Education Using an Online Virtual World:  The  PREVIEW-Sustain...
Sustainability Education Using an Online Virtual World: The PREVIEW-Sustain...
 
Using Second Life Avatars and Machinima to Introduce Sustainability into the ...
Using Second Life Avatars and Machinima to Introduce Sustainability into the ...Using Second Life Avatars and Machinima to Introduce Sustainability into the ...
Using Second Life Avatars and Machinima to Introduce Sustainability into the ...
 
MyChild Services Information Sheet (755kb)
MyChild Services Information Sheet (755kb)MyChild Services Information Sheet (755kb)
MyChild Services Information Sheet (755kb)
 
AAA Section 04 Asperger's Disorder Ver 04 2013
AAA Section 04 Asperger's Disorder Ver 04 2013AAA Section 04 Asperger's Disorder Ver 04 2013
AAA Section 04 Asperger's Disorder Ver 04 2013
 
AAA Section 03 Autism Spectrum Disorder Ver 03 2013
AAA Section 03 Autism Spectrum Disorder Ver 03 2013AAA Section 03 Autism Spectrum Disorder Ver 03 2013
AAA Section 03 Autism Spectrum Disorder Ver 03 2013
 
AAA Section 02 ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder Ver 04 2013
AAA Section 02 ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder Ver 04 2013AAA Section 02 ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder Ver 04 2013
AAA Section 02 ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder Ver 04 2013
 
AAA Section 01 Introduction to the Module Ver 02 2013
AAA Section 01 Introduction to the Module Ver 02 2013AAA Section 01 Introduction to the Module Ver 02 2013
AAA Section 01 Introduction to the Module Ver 02 2013
 
FOP2 Supplementary Coursework Instructions
FOP2 Supplementary Coursework InstructionsFOP2 Supplementary Coursework Instructions
FOP2 Supplementary Coursework Instructions
 
AAA Section 11 Revision and the Campus Exam
AAA Section 11 Revision and the Campus ExamAAA Section 11 Revision and the Campus Exam
AAA Section 11 Revision and the Campus Exam
 
The literature review
The literature reviewThe literature review
The literature review
 
Critically reading a paper
Critically reading a paperCritically reading a paper
Critically reading a paper
 
Section 11 (On-Campus) - Revision and the Exam
Section 11 (On-Campus) - Revision and the ExamSection 11 (On-Campus) - Revision and the Exam
Section 11 (On-Campus) - Revision and the Exam
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDThiyagu K
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajansocial pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajanpragatimahajan3
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...anjaliyadav012327
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...fonyou31
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajansocial pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
JAPAN: ORGANISATION OF PMDA, PHARMACEUTICAL LAWS & REGULATIONS, TYPES OF REGI...
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 

Bignell_&_Cain_(2007)

  • 1. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society 499 The British Psychological British Journal of Developmental Psychology (2007), 25, 499–512 q 2007 The British Psychological Society Society www.bpsjournals.co.uk Pragmatic aspects of communication and language comprehension in groups of children differentiated by teacher ratings of inattention and hyperactivity Simon Bignell1 and Kate Cain2* 1 University of Essex, UK 2 Lancaster University, UK Children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) experience pragmatic language deficits, but it is not known whether these difficulties are primarily associated with high levels of inattention, hyperactivity, or both. We investigated pragmatic aspects of communication and language comprehension in relation to poor attention and/or high hyperactivity in a nondiagnosed population of 7- to 11-year olds. Classroom teachers rated their pupils’ attention and hyperactivity/impulsivity on the ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher Rating scale (ACTeRS). Three groups were formed: children with poor attention and low hyperactivity (poor attention group), children with good attention and high hyperactivity (high hyperactivity group), and children with both poor attention and high hyperactivity (poor attention/high hyperactivity group). Their performance was compared with that of same-age controls in two studies: Study 1 (N ¼ 94) investigated the comprehension of figurative language in and out of context and Study 2 (N ¼ 100) investigated the pragmatic aspects of communication using the Children’s Communication Checklist – Second Edition. Two groups, the poor attention and the poor attention/high hyperactivity groups, were impaired in both their comprehension of figurative language and their communication skills. The high hyperactivity group was impaired in their comprehension of figurative language but they did not exhibit communication impairments. The findings extend work with clinical populations of children with ADHD: even in a nondiagnosed sample of children, poor attention and elevated levels of hyperactivity are associated with pragmatic language weaknesses. Pragmatics is defined as how language is used to convey meaning (e.g. Adams, 2002). Pragmatic language difficulties are specific to the use and comprehension of language in context, rather than problems with semantic or structural aspects of language. Difficulties in language use include poor turn taking and an inability to stay on topic in * Correspondence should be addressed to Dr Kate Cain, Department of Psychology, Fylde College, Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK (e-mail: k.cain@lancs.ac.uk). DOI:10.1348/026151006X171343
  • 2. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society 500 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain conversation; difficulties in language comprehension include a tendency to interpret figurative language literally. We report an investigation of these two aspects of pragmatic language in groups of 7- to 11-year olds differentiated by teacher ratings of inattention and hyperactivity. ADHD is a behavioural disorder, in which individuals exhibit levels of inattention and/or hyperactivity and impulsivity that are inappropriate for their age (DSM-IV, American Psychiatric Association, 1994). DSM-IV distinguishes between the inattentive and hyperactivity/impulsivity elements of the disorder, which may occur together or separately, resulting in three subtypes: predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive–impulsive, and combined type. Some researchers advocate the use of a categorical approach, in which ADHD is regarded as a distinct syndrome. Others adopt a framework in which inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity vary throughout the general population and children with a diagnosis of ADHD lie in the tail end of a normal distribution (Levy, Hay, McStephen, Wood, & Waldman, 1997). ADHD frequently co-occurs with language disorders (Tirosh & Cohen, 1998; Westby & Cutler, 1994). An analysis of the DSM-IV criteria for the ADHD subtypes demonstrates that specific aspects of pragmatic language form part of the diagnostic characteristics for all three subtypes (Camarata & Gibson, 1999; Westby & Cutler, 1994). These deficits may influence the quality and/or the frequency of language learning experiences which may, in-turn, lead to wider language impairments, for example, in semantic and in syntactic skills (Camarata & Gibson, 1999). Pragmatic impairments may also adversely affect the performance on standardized assessments of language skill if they interfere with the child’s ability to evaluate the contextual demands of the task (Oram, Fine, Okamoto, & Tannock, 1999). An investigation of the relations between the specific aspects of pragmatic language skill and the separate symptoms of ADHD is required to fully understand the extent of pragmatic language deficits in relation to ADHD and how these affect other aspects of language use (Camarata & Gibson, 1999; Oram et al., 1999; Westby & Cutler, 1994). Assessments of the pragmatic aspects of children’s communicative abilities include whether or not children can introduce and maintain a topic and their turn taking abilities during a conversation. These pragmatic behaviours typically emerge between 2 and 3 years of age (Adams, 2002). Teachers are more likely to report these difficulties in boys with attention problems than in groups with learning disability or average achievement (Humphries, Koltun, Malone, & Roberts, 1994). Children with ADHD also produce more inappropriate pragmatic behaviours in unstructured spontaneous conversations with adults than do typically developing children (Kim & Kaiser, 2000). Other pragmatic language deficits, such as a failure to take a listener’s perspective into account when retelling a story, have been found in children with ADHD (Purvis & Tannock, 1997). Another aspect of pragmatic language, which was considered in the current research, is the understanding of language in context. Assessments of this skill often examine how children interpret figurative language in context, to determine whether or not the child attends to the context when interpreting figurative expressions such as idioms, e.g. ‘to get into hot water’ (Adams, 2002). The ability to understand figurative language has an extended course of development from early childhood through early adolescence (Nippold & Taylor, 1995), although children as young as 5 years are able to use context to understand these expressions (Gibbs, 1991). A tendency to interpret language literally, rather than figuratively, is included in some teacher and parent checklists of communicative ability (e.g. Bishop, 1998). However, on formal tests that assess this skill, such as defining words that can take different meanings in different contexts, children with ADHD do not differ from controls (Purvis & Tannock, 1997).
  • 3. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language 501 This review indicates that children with ADHD may experience different types of pragmatic language deficit: impairments in communication and in language comprehension in context. Work to date has not looked at the relation between pragmatic language skills and inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity separately, although the need to explore the language skills of the subtypes of ADHD has been widely noted (e.g. Baird, Stevenson, & Williams, 2000; Camarata & Gibson, 1999; Kim & Kaiser, 2000; Oram et al., 1999; Westby & Cutler, 1994). Theoretically, it is important to determine whether or not inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity make separate and distinct contributions to language skills. It is also necessary to determine whether one subtype is more at risk of a particular language deficit than another, in order to develop effective interventions. This research, to the authors’ knowledge, represents the first investigation in the literature to consider how hyperactivity/impulsivity and poor attention are separately related to school children’s pragmatic language skills. To do this, teachers rated children’s attention and hyperactivity using a standardized questionnaire based on the DSM-IV classification of ADHD. These ratings were used to identify children with poor attention and/or high hyperactivity. A similar selection procedure has been used previously by Wilding and colleagues (Wilding, 2003; Wilding, Munir, & Cornish, 2001) to explore the relations between attention deficits and different components of attention, and by Adams and Snowling (2001) to investigate the relations between hyperactivity and executive function and reading impairment. Teacher ratings of (hyper)activity and (in)attention usually inform the diagnosis of ADHD (Power et al., 1998), although a formal diagnosis requires ratings from different informants (e.g. parents as well as teachers) and additional information to eliminate other causes of the behaviour. The children in our study were not formally diagnosed with ADHD, because we were interested to determine the relations between inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language skills in children who were (as far as possible) unaffected by additional behavioural problems, such as oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder, which are often co-morbid. However, our findings can inform theoretical models of the relations between inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language skills, which have been developed from the ADHD research literature. We report two studies, in which we investigated the relations between poor attention and high levels of hyperactivity/impulsivity (hereafter hyperactivity) and the interpretation of figurative language in and out of context (Study 1), and pragmatic aspects of communication (Study 2). Our aims were to determine whether or not children with poor attention and/or high hyperactivity (who do not have a formal diagnosis of ADHD) show signs of pragmatic language difficulties and whether or not children with predominantly poor attention and predominantly high levels of hyperactivity are similarly at risk of pragmatic language impairments. STUDY 1: INTERPRETATION OF FIGURATIVE LANGUAGE IN AND OUT OF CONTEXT Method Participants Three experimental groups and matched controls participated in this experiment. The children were selected from five mainstream suburban primary schools serving
  • 4. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society 502 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain middle and lower-middle class catchment areas in the East of England. Children whose first language at home was not English, who had a diagnosis of a hearing, speech, or language disorder, a formal statement of special educational needs, a diagnosis of ADHD, or for whom parental consent was not given were excluded from the study. Assessment of inattention and hyperactivity Teachers of all children aged 7–11 years completed the two subscales of the ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher Rating scale (ACTeRS: Ullmann, Sleator, & Sprague, 1999) relating to attention and hyperactive behaviour. The reliabilities of the two subscales are high: .93–.97. For the values reported throughout, the attention scale has been reversed thus, for each scale high scores indicate a tendency towards inattention or hyperactivity. To classify children we used the following criteria. Scores of between 5 and 9 on the reversed Attention scale and 10 or less on the Hyperactivity scale were equivalent to the 50th percentile and considered developmentally appropriate. Scores of between 20 and 30 on the reversed Attention scale and 16 and 25 on the Hyperactive scale were equivalent to the 25th percentile and classified as ‘poor attention’ and ‘high hyperactivity’, respectively. Measures of verbal and non-verbal ability Receptive vocabulary was measured with a group-administered version of the British Picture Vocabulary scales-II (BPVS-II, Dunn, Dunn, Whetton, & Burley, 1997: see Stanovich & Cunningham, 1992, for a similar modification). The BPVS is a measure of receptive vocabulary commonly used as a surrogate measure of verbal ability (e.g. Adams & Snowling, 2001). The modified test comprised one practice item and 50 test words. The experimenter read out the word and the child ticked the corresponding picture in their individual booklet. One point was awarded for each correct answer. The reliability was assessed by calculating Cronbach’s alpha over items and found to be adequate, a ¼ :78. Non-verbal reasoning ability was assessed with the Matrix Analogies Test-Expanded Form (MAT-EF, Naglieri, 1985). The ACTeRs scores informed selection of three experimental groups: the poor attention group, the high hyperactivity group, and the poor attention/high hyperactivity group. Each experimental group had their own control group, comprising children who had developmentally appropriate scores in the key area for which they acted as controls. Each experimental group was matched with their appropriate control group on the following: chronological age, vocabulary, MAT-EF scores, and sex (see Table 1). In addition, one-way ANOVAs demonstrated that the three experimental groups did not differ significantly in age, MAT-EF, or vocabulary scores: no Fð2; 44Þ exceeded 1.9 and all p values were greater than .17. Materials and procedure Understanding multiple meanings in context Children completed a modified version of the Multiple Meanings in Context (MMC) subtest of the Understanding Ambiguity test (Rinaldi, 1996) to assess their ability to understand pragmatic, or figurative, interpretations of speech. They were presented with ten short story dialogues, five of which contained an ambiguous phrase, ‘My little girl’s room is a real pig sty’, and five of which contained a homonym, e.g. ‘I’ve been getting very short with Suzie recently’. Each item could take a literal and a figurative interpretation: the context of the story supported the latter. The task was adapted from
  • 5. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language 503 Table 1. Means (standard deviations) for hyperactivity and inattention ratings, age, vocabulary, and non-verbal ability measures in Study 1 Chronological ACTeRS ACTeRS Vocabulary MAT-EF age in months hyperactivity inattention score raw score Hyperactive 117.20 (7.18) 18.73 (2.69)*** 9.13 (1.30) 34.80 (4.46) 21.40 (5.05) (N ¼ 15; G ¼ 2, B ¼ 13) Hyperactive controls 115.20 (12.25) 6.47 (1.60) 8.20 (1.94) 35.60 (4.49) 23.33 (4.19) (N ¼ 15; G ¼ 2, B ¼ 13) Poor attention 118.13 (13.42) 7.44 (1.32) 21.94 (2.98)*** 32.38 (5.58) 20.69 (4.98) (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 10, B ¼ 6) Poor attention controls 116.88 (12.81) 6.88 (1.78) 10.38 (2.66) 33.31 (5.29) 24.44 (4.16) (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 10, B ¼ 6) Poor attention and hyperactive 115.56 (12.79) 19.44 (3.22)*** 24.38 (2.34)*** 31.75 (4.55) 20.13 (5.38) (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 2, B ¼ 14) Poor attention and hyperactive 114.06 (12.27) 6.25 (1.92) 7.56 (2.00) 34.25 (4.34) 21.25 (5.29) controls (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 2, B ¼ 14) Note. Significant differences between each experimental group and control are marked in bold: ***p , :001; for all other comparisons p . :10. G, number of girls; B, number of boys. the original to make it suitable to children who were distractible or inattentive: the dialogue was spoken by the experimenter, instead of the audio-recorded presentation, which had been distracting to children in previous work. After each dialogue children selected one from four pictures that illustrated what a character had said, e.g. ‘What does Joanna’s mum mean?’ The pictures illustrated the correct figurative and the correct literal interpretations, and an incorrect figurative and an incorrect literal interpretation. A ‘don’t know’ response was also available. Regardless of their first response, each child was asked the question: ‘Could it mean something else?’ to determine whether they knew both meanings (literal and figurative) of each item. One practice trial with feedback preceded the experimental trials. Two scores were computed and analysed. The number of first choice responses made by children was calculated (maximum ¼ 10) to determine whether any of the experimental groups had a preference for figurative or literal interpretations relative to their controls. The sum of the correct figurative and the correct literal choices made either on first or second choice was also calculated. For the latter, the maximum possible score of 20 indicates knowledge of both the figurative and the literal meanings of all items. Knowledge of multiple meanings out of context Children completed modified versions of the Ambiguous Sentences and Figurative Language subtests from The Test of Language Competence-Expanded Edition (TLC-E: Wiig & Secord, 1989) to assess their understanding of figurative language without supporting context. Each trial comprised a spoken non-predictive sentence context, e.g. ‘Mum looks really low today’ with four accompanying pictures. One picture represented the figurative interpretation, one the literal interpretation and two were foils. The child was asked ‘Point to the two pictures that it could mean’ with additional prompts, if necessary. A demonstration item was presented first, followed by a practice trial. There were five trials each of homonyms and phrases. One point was awarded for each target item (figurative or literal) chosen on each trial and the results were summed (maximum ¼ 20).
  • 6. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society 504 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain Results Understanding multiple meanings in context First choice responses The numbers of first choice responses are shown in Table 2. Each experimental group was significantly less likely to select the correct figurative interpretation relative to their controls: high hyperactivity vs. controls, tð28Þ ¼ 2:95, p , :01, d ¼ 1:07; poor attention vs. controls, tð30Þ ¼ 4:14, p , :001, d ¼ 1:89; poor attention/high hyperactivity type vs. controls, tð30Þ ¼ 3:57, p , :001, d ¼ 1:47. A one-way ANOVA with the experimental groups demonstrated that their performance did not differ, Fð2; 44Þ ¼ 1:74, p . :17. The control groups made few errors indicating a strong tendency towards a figurative interpretation strategy. Therefore, paired sample t tests comparing figurative and literal responses were computed for the experimental groups, only. None of the experimental groups revealed either a preference for figurative or literal interpretation: no t exceeded 1.06. Table 2. Mean number of responses (and standard deviations) obtained on figurative language in context assessment in Study 1 Total score first choices (max ¼ 10) First and second choice (max ¼ 20) Figurative Literal Figurative Literal Don’t Figurative and correct correct incorrect incorrect know literal correct Hyperactive 5.53 (3.40)** 3.93 (3.71) .40 (.63) .07 (.26) .07 (.26) 17.33 (2.94) Hyperactive controls 8.40 (1.68) .93 (1.34) .33 (.72) .33 (.49) .00 (0) 18.00 (2.36) Poor attention 3.69 (2.85)*** 5.25 (3.17) .88 (1.14) .13 (.34) .63 (.25) 16.13 (1.92)* Poor attention controls 8.31 (1.96) 1.50 (1.75) .12 (.34) .00 (0) .63 (.25) 18.44 (2.73) Poor attention and 5.50 (3.29)*** 4.00 (3.42) .25 (.58) .25 (.45) .00 (0) 17.00 (2.88)* hyperactive Poor attention and 9.00 (.73) .88 (.72) .13 (.34) .00 (0) .00 (0) 18.81 (1.22) hyperactive controls Note. Significant differences between each experimental group and their control are in bold: *p , .05; **p , .01; ***p , .001. First and second choice responses The sum of first and second responses is reported in Table 2. The difference between the high hyperactivity group and their controls was not significant, tð28Þ , 1:0. The poor attention and poor attention/high hyperactivity groups obtained significantly lower scores than did their controls: tð30Þ ¼ 2:77, p ¼ :010, d ¼ :98, tð30Þ ¼ 2:32, p , :03, d ¼ :82, in order. Knowledge of multiple meanings out of context The experimental groups obtained lower scores than did their controls. There were no differences in performance between the high hyperactivity group and the controls: Ms ¼ 15:60; 16:67; SDs ¼ 3:58; 1:99, tð28Þ ¼ 1:00, p . :20, nor between the poor attention group and the controls: Ms ¼ 14:44; 15:81; SDs ¼ 3:52; 2:97, tð30Þ ¼ 1:19, p . :20. The group with poor attention and high hyperactivity differed significantly from their controls: Ms ¼ 15:44; 17:31; SDs ¼ 2:56; 1:66, tð30Þ ¼ 2:46, p , :025,
  • 7. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language 505 d ¼ :87. A one-way ANOVA with the experimental groups revealed no significant group differences, Fð2; 44Þ , 1:0. Summary and discussion All groups with poor attention and/or high hyperactivity were poor at using context to select appropriate meanings for ambiguous forms of language. The effect size for the comparison between each experimental group and their controls indicates a sizable difference. Furthermore, the experimental groups did not differ from each other, suggesting comparable levels of impairments. The analysis of first and second responses was used as an indicator of knowledge: did children know both the literal and the figurative interpretations of the expression? In this analysis, children with high levels of inattention obtained lower scores than did their controls. Children with elevated levels of hyperactivity alone were not similarly impaired, indicating that their difficulties on the task were not simply attributable to knowledge deficits. The out of context task assessed knowledge directly: there was no supportive context from which to infer the figurative meaning of the expression. The total scores indicated that the experimental groups knew both meanings of most items, although the group with both poor attention and elevated hyperactivity differed from their controls on this measure. This finding indicates that difficulties with the in context task may be primarily due to difficulties in the interpretation of language in context, rather than knowledge. In both tasks, the experimental groups demonstrated a reasonable level of knowledge for both meanings of the words and phrases, even when they differed significantly from their control group. However, when presented with ambiguous expressions in contexts that supported a figurative interpretation, the experimental groups were less likely than controls, to select the figurative interpretation as their first response. STUDY 2: PARENTAL REPORTS OF COMMUNICATION SKILL Study 2 focuses on a different aspect of pragmatics: the pragmatic aspects of communi- cation. To examine this ability, we used the second edition of the Children’s Communi- cation Checklist (CCC: Bishop, 1998; CCC-2: Bishop, 2003), which was designed to measure pragmatic and structural aspects of a child’s communication by parents and professionals who have regular contact with the child. In the original CCC, five subscales were designed to assess the following pragmatic aspects of communication: coherence, use of stereotyped language and use of context, instances of inappropriate initiation, and conversational rapport. A pragmatic composite derived from these scores reliably discriminates children with specific language impairment from children with pragmatic language impairment (Bishop, 1998; Botting, 2004). Thus, the CCC is considered a reliable indicator of pragmatic language difficulties. Children with ADHD show deficits on the pragmatic language subscales of the CCC (Bishop & Baird, 2001; Geurts et al., 2004). It is not known whether the subtypes are equally at risk of communicative deficits. The CCC-2 is the latest version of this widely used assessment of communication skills. It provides standardized scores for two composite scores: a General Communi- cation composite, which indicates children who have a significant communication problem and a Social Interaction Deviance composite, which indicates children who have a communication profile characteristic of children with autistic spectrum disorder
  • 8. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society 506 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain (Bishop, 2003). The pragmatic composite, included in the CCC, is no longer available, although the five subscales relating to pragmatic language skills are retained. We calculated the pragmatic composite to determine whether pragmatic language deficits were associated with poor attention and high hyperactivity in general, or specific to a particular behavioural profile (compare with Bishop & Baird, 2001; Geurts et al., 2004). The parents of the children who participated in Study 1 and an additional cohort of children, selected in the same way, were sent the CCC-2 to complete. The aims were to extend previous work with the CCC (Bishop & Baird, 2001; Geurts et al., 2004) to investigate: i) whether or not children with poor attention and/or high hyperactivity (who do not have a formal diagnosis of ADHD) show signs of pragmatic language difficulties and ii) whether or not children with predominantly poor attention and predominantly high levels of hyperactivity are similarly at risk of pragmatic language impairments. Method Participants We obtained completed questionnaires for 16 children with high hyperactivity and 18 of their controls, for 16 children with poor attention and 19 of their controls, and for 14 children with poor attention/high hyperactivity and 17 of their controls. The return rate of questionnaires from the children who participated in Study 1 was 46% and the return rate for the additional cohort of children was 56%. Each experimental group was matched with their appropriate control group on the following: chronological age, sex, and vocabulary. Two of the experimental groups (poor attention and high hyperactivity) differed from their controls in the scores obtained on the MAT-EF (an unavoidable consequence of the sample of returns). One-way ANOVAs demonstrated that the three experimental groups did not differ significantly in age, MAT-EF, or vocabulary scores: no Fð2; 43Þ exceeded 1.75 and all p values were greater than .19. The characteristics of each experimental group and their controls are reported in Table 3. Table 3. Means (standard deviations) for hyperactivity and inattention ratings, age, vocabulary, and non-verbal ability measures in Study 2 Chronological ACTeRS ACTeRS Vocabulary MAT-EF age in months hyperactivity inattention score raw score Hyperactive 115.63 (12.34) 18.36 (2.68)*** 10.00 (1.90) 33.56 (5.63) 18.25 (4.78)** (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 3, B ¼ 13) Hyperactive controls 117.83 (12.03) 6.11 (1.45) 8.56 (2.57) 34.83 (5.98) 23.44 (4.55) (N ¼ 18; G ¼ 7, B ¼ 11) Poor attention 110.31 (13.50) 7.31 (1.30) 22.75 (2.98)*** 30.19 (5.56) 19.38 (5.93)* (N ¼ 16; G ¼ 9, B ¼ 7) Poor attention controls 118.53 (13.53) 6.95 (1.78) 10.26 (3.05) 33.53 (5.68) 22.95 (3.60) (N ¼ 19; G ¼ 13, B ¼ 6) Poor attention and 114.07 (11.55) 18.86 (2.91)*** 24.00 (3.37)*** 32.07 (4.01) 18.64 (4.87) hyperactive (N ¼ 14; G ¼ 6, B ¼ 8) Poor attention and 113.71 (13.61) 5.94 (1.52) 8.24 (2.31) 32.88 (5.21) 20.76 (4.48) hyperactive controls (N ¼ 17; G ¼ 4, B ¼ 13) Note. Significant differences between each experimental group and control are marked in bold and significance levels indicated as follows: *p , :05; **p , :01; ***p , :001; for all other comparisons p . :10. G, number of girls; B, number of boys.
  • 9. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language 507 Results We calculated the General Communication (GCC) and the Social Interaction Deviance (SIDC) composites as directed in the CCC-2. A pragmatic composite based on the original CCC was also calculated. These scores are reported in Table 4. The high hyperactivity group did not differ from their controls on any of the composite scores: no t exceeded 1.0, and the three experimental groups did not differ from each other on any of the composite scores, all Fsð2; 43Þ , 1:03, all ps . :37. The poor attention group obtained significantly lower scores than their controls on the GCC, tð33Þ ¼ 2:44, p ¼ :02, d ¼ :81. The mean percentile equivalents were 24 and 50, respectively. The poor attention and high hyperactivity group obtained lower scores than their controls, but the difference was not significant, tð29Þ ¼ 1:86, p ¼ :078, d ¼ :68. The mean percentile equivalents were 32 and 58, respectively. Neither of these groups differed from their controls on the SIDC, both ts , 1:0, however, both groups with poor attention differed from their respective controls on the pragmatic composite: poor attention group vs. controls, tð33Þ ¼ 2:44, p , :025, d ¼ :81; poor attention/high hyperactivity group vs. controls, tð29Þ ¼ 2:37, p , :03, d ¼ :84. Neither vocabulary scores nor non-verbal reasoning scores (MAT-EF) were correlated with any of the composite scores, no r exceeded .19 and thus none were significant (N ¼ 100). Summary and discussion Children with poor attention showed evidence of weak communication skills compared with their controls; children with high hyperactivity but good attention did not show signs of communicative impairments. Children with both poor attention and high levels of hyperactivity did not have wider-ranging difficulties with social interaction: no differences were found on the SIDC. Of note, there was considerable variability within both groups with poor attention (indexed by the standard deviations) suggested that communication difficulties experienced by these children were not uniform. GENERAL DISCUSSION The important finding from this research was that the primary behavioural deficits of ADHD, poor attention, and elevated hyperactivity, were associated with impairments in pragmatic aspects of communication and language comprehension. Children with poor attention obtained lower scores than matched controls on a formal test of language interpretation in context and a parental assessment of communicative skills. Children with elevated hyperactivity were impaired on the assessment of language interpretation in context but their communication skills were not rated as impaired. This study should be considered exploratory: we did not include a full range of pragmatic language assessments and we did not assess children with a diagnosis of ADHD. However, this work has important implications for theoretical models that posit relations between pragmatic language skills, inattention, hyperactivity, and it indicates directions for future research with children who have ADHD. We discuss theoretical models, how they relate to our current findings, and directions for future work, below. Theoretical models of the relations between inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language skills have come from the ADHD literature. These models propose that children with ADHD may experience pragmatic language difficulties for, at least, two reasons.
  • 10. 508 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain Table 4. Mean standard and composite scores (and standard deviations) obtained on parental CCC Poor attention Poor attention and Hyperactive Hyperactive controls Poor attention Poor attention controls and hyperactive hyperactive controls Pragmatic composite 47.25 (13.56) 48.22 (13.16) 39.75* (15.42) 50.37* (10.17) 41.71* (17.32) 54.53* (12.82) GC composite 77.81 (18.92) 77.56 (18.55) 66.38* (23.38) 82.05* (14.27) 71.50 (25.44) 86.00 (17.93) SID composite 6.69 (6.71) 6.56 (4.48) 4.63 (8.07) 4.84 (3.84) 3.79 (10.14) 3.94 (4.78) Note. Significant difference between each experimental group and their control are signalled in bold, *p , :05; GC composite, General Communication composite; SID composite, Social Interaction Deviance composite. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society
  • 11. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language 509 ADHD may result from poor behavioural inhibition, which affects executive control and leads to problems with attention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity (Barkley, 1997). Pragmatic language use taps into executive skills such as planning, organizing, and/or monitoring behaviours. Thus, pragmatic language deficits might arise from the cognitive deficits that cause the behavioural symptoms of ADHD (e.g. Purvis & Tannock, 1997). This theory might explain the difficulties with figurative language in context experienced by all groups in this work. An inability to monitor and evaluate the appropriate context could lead to the literal interpretation of a figurative expression. An alternative, though not mutually exclusive, hypothesis is that the behavioural characteristics of the different subtypes of ADHD influence the quality and the frequency of their interactions with caregivers, which adversely affects their pragmatic language development (e.g. Camarata & Gibson, 1999). For example, poor attention may lead children to miss important environmental and conversational cues, which limits their ability to learn the range of meanings conveyed by different words and phrases. There was little evidence for deficits in knowledge of multiple meanings of words and phrases in the current work. However, the children in this study did not have a diagnosis of ADHD, thus we might infer that any impairment in attention or hyperactivity was less severe than that found in clinical populations and therefore might have less serious consequences for language learning. We found no evidence for a greater impairment in any group: that may not necessarily be the case for diagnosed children where predominantly inattentive or hyperactive children might suffer more. Further work is needed to disentangle the relative contributions of attention and hyperactivity to language learning and language comprehension in both nondiagnosed and clinical populations. There was some evidence that children with poor attention had poorer communication skills than the children with only high hyperactivity. The effect sizes indicated moderately sized differences from controls but the three experimental groups did not obtain statistically different ratings from each other on the CCC-2. Our measure of communicative skill was questionnaire based and, although it was completed by parents who presumably reflected on their own interactions with their child, analysis of naturalistic interactions with adults is also desirable as well as replication of these findings. Analysis of the behavioural characteristics of ADHD, suggests that both subtypes should show evidence of conversational weaknesses. Again, difficulties that were not apparent in our high hyperactive group might be apparent in a diagnosed sample. Children with high levels of inattention may be at risk of a greater range of pragmatic language impairments than children who are predominantly hyperactive, but we did not find an association between elevated hyperactivity and communicative weaknesses. In relation to children with ADHD, these findings suggest that all subtypes may be weak at interpreting figurative language in context and that the inattention and combined subtypes may be at risk of communicative impairments in addition. Our tight group matching means that such weaknesses may be apparent over and above any other deficits in verbal ability. We studied an age range (8–11 years) in which substantial development in figurative language comprehension is found (Gibbs, 1991; Nippold & Taylor, 1995). This may have lead to the large standard deviations apparent on some measures. Future work might compare the time course of the development of different forms of figurative language comprehension and pragmatic language in ADHD subtypes to establish which aspects are to delayed and which are deviant in children with poor attention and/poor high hyperactivity.
  • 12. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society 510 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain Cohen et al. (1998) suggest that the language difficulties experienced by children with ADHD may lead to their social difficulties. Therefore, a greater understanding of this population’s language deficits and why they arise has important implications and needs to be addressed. We have begun to address this issue in the reported work. In contrast to work with diagnosed populations (see Milich, Balentine, & Lynham, 2001), we found no evidence for impaired social skills in our sample. However, our test instrument (the CCC-2) was developed to look at the difficulties with language and social interaction experienced by children with autism spectrum disorders; assessments of more general social skills may reveal difficulties where we found none. In addition, it must be remembered that the children in this study were not diagnosed with ADHD, thus their difficulties may not have been as pronounced as those who had been clinically assessed. In summary, we have shown that poor attention and high hyperactivity are differently associated with pragmatic language skills: high levels of hyperactivity and poor attention are associated with impairments in the comprehension of figurative language in context; poor attention is additionally associated with impairments in pragmatic aspects of communication. These findings are consistent with the idea of a continuum of attentional and activity problems. They indicate clear directions for future work. First, we need to consider how individual differences in attention and/or hyperactivity affect the language and learning of nondiagnosed children. Our methodology enables the investigation of the symptoms of ADHD in relation to cognitive skills of theoretical interest without the confounding factors of the accompanying deficits found in many clinically diagnosed individuals. Second, future work should determine whether clinically diagnosed subtypes can be differentiated on the basis of their pragmatic language skills: our work suggests that this might be the case. If so, different types of intervention will be required for the subtypes of ADHD. Acknowledgements This work was conducted as part of the first author’s doctoral thesis and was supported by a University of Essex teaching studentship. We would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments on an earlier version of this work. Finally, many thanks to the teachers and children at the following primary schools in Essex who participated in this work: Capel St Mary, Eight Ash Green, Great Bentley, Holland Park, Kirby, Morland, Rolph, Springfield, St Johns, Wentworth. References Adams, C. (2002). Practitioner review: The assessment of language pragmatics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 973–987. Adams, J. W., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). Executive function and reading impairments in children reported by their teachers as hyperactive. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19, 293–306. American Psychological Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: APA. Baird, J., Stevenson, J. C., & Williams, D. C. (2000). The evolution of ADHD: A disorder of communication? Quarterly Review of Biology, 75, 17–35. Barkley, R. A. (1997). ADHD and the nature of self-control. New York: Guilford Press. Bishop, D. V. M. (1998). Development of the Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative aspects of communicative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879–891.
  • 13. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society Inattention, hyperactivity, and pragmatic language 511 Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). Children’s Communication Checklist-Revised (CCC-2) (2nd. ed.). London: The Psychological Corporation. Bishop, D. V. M., & Baird, G. (2001). Parent and teacher report of pragmatic aspects of communication: Use of the Children’s Communication Checklist in a clinical setting. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 43, 809–818. Botting, N. (2004). Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC) scores in 11-year-old children with communication impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 39, 215–227. Camarata, S. M., & Gibson, T. (1999). Pragmatic language deficits in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 5, 207–214. Cohen, N. J., Menna, R., Vallance, D. D., Barwick, M. A., Im, N., & Horodeszky, N. B. (1998). Language, social cognitive processing, and behavioral characteristics, of psychiatrically disturbed children with previously identified and unsuspected language impairments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 853–864. Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., Whetton, C., & Burley, J. (1997). British picture vocabulary scale (2nd. ed). Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson. ´ Geurts, H. M., Verte, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., Hartman, C. A., Mulder, E. J., et al. (2004). Can the Children’s Communication Checklist differentiate between children with autism, children with ADHD, and normal controls? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1437–1453. Gibbs, R. W. (1991). Semantic analyzability in children’s understanding of idioms. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 613–620. Humphries, T., Koltun, H., Malone, M., & Roberts, W. (1994). Teacher-identified oral language difficulties among boys with attention problems. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 15, 92–98. Kim, O. H., & Kaiser, A. P. (2000). Language characteristics of children with ADHD. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 21, 154–165. Levy, F., Hay, D. A., Mcstephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 737–744. Milich, R., Balentine, A. C., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). ADHD combined type and ADHD predominantly inattentive type are distinct and unrelated disorders. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 8, 463–488. Naglieri, J. A. (1985). The matrix analogies test: Expanded form. New York: The Psychological Corporation. Nippold, M. A., & Taylor, C. L. (1995). Idiom understanding in youth: Further examination of familiarity and transparency. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 426–433. Oram, J., Fine, J., Okamoto, C., & Tannock, R. (1999). Assessing the language of children with atten- tion deficit hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8, 72–80. Power, T. J., Andrews, T. J., Eiraldi, R. B., Doherty, B. J., Ikeda, M. J., Dupaul, G. J., & Landau, S. (1998). Evaluating attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using multiple informants: The incremental utility of combining teacher with parent reports. Psychological Assessment, 10, 250–260. Purvis, K. L., & Tannock, R. (1997). Language abilities in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, reading disabilities, and normal controls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 133–144. Rinaldi, W. (1996). Understanding ambiguity: An assessment of pragmatic meaning comprehension. Windsor: NFER-Nelson. Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1992). Studying the consequences of literacy within a lite- rate society: The cognitive correlates of print exposure. Memory and Cognition, 20, 51–68. Tirosh, E., & Cohen, A. (1998). Language deficit with attention-deficit disorder: A prevalent comorbidity. Journal of Child Neurology, 13, 493–497.
  • 14. Copyright © The British Psychological Society Reproduction in any form (including the internet) is prohibited without prior permission from the Society 512 Simon Bignell and Kate Cain Ullmann, R. K., Sleator, E. K., & Sprague, R. L. (1999). The ADD-H Comprehensive Teacher’s Rating Scale (ACTeRS) (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: MetriTech Inc. Westby, C. E., & Cutler, S. K. (1994). Language and ADHD – understanding the bases and treatment of self-regulatory deficits. Topics in Language Disorders, 14, 58–76. Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (1989). Test of Language Competence – Expanded Edition (TLC-E). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation. Wilding, J. M. (2003). Attentional difficulties in children: Weakness in executive function or problems in coping with difficult tasks? British Journal of Psychology, 94, 427–436. Wilding, J., Munir, F., & Cornish, K. (2001). The nature of attentional differences between groups of children differentiated by teacher ratings of attention and hyperactivity. British Journal of Psychology, 92, 357–371. Received 17 January 2006; revised version received 20 November 2006