SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 159
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
i 
TABLE OF CONTENTS 
List of Figures .............................................................................................................. iv 
List of Tables................................................................................................................. v 
Acknowledgements ...................................................................................................... vii 
Abstract........................................................................................................................ ix 
Chapter 1. General introduction..................................................................................1:1 
Cognitive models of language processing ...................................................................1:1 
A simple model to explore language abilities: The ‘basic model’ ................................1:2 
Two lexicons or four? ............................................................................................. 1:8 
Different accounts of reading aloud ........................................................................ 1:9 
The relationship between reading aloud and oral picture naming........................... 1:11 
Research Aims...................................................................................................... 1:14 
Chapter 2. Method .....................................................................................................2:17 
Participants..............................................................................................................2:17 
Recruitment of aphasic participants ...................................................................... 2:17 
Recruitment of unimpaired controls ...................................................................... 2:18 
Materials..................................................................................................................2:19 
Procedures ...............................................................................................................2:23 
Scoring.....................................................................................................................2:25 
Analyses ...................................................................................................................2:25 
Chapter 3. Control group – results and discussion ...................................................3:31 
Regularity effects of unpublished tests ......................................................................3:31 
Oral naming versus written naming..........................................................................3:32 
Methodological issues ..............................................................................................3:33 
Chapter 4. A simple case to explain? .........................................................................4:37 
Case description.......................................................................................................4:37 
Results......................................................................................................................4:37 
Input processes ..................................................................................................... 4:38 
Reading and repetition of words and nonwords..................................................... 4:38 
The semantic system............................................................................................. 4:38 
Picture naming...................................................................................................... 4:39 
Discussion................................................................................................................4:40
Chapter 5. Three cases of phonological dyslexia ...................................................... 5:45 
ii 
Case 1 – RPD........................................................................................................... 5:45 
Results for RPD........................................................................................................ 5:45 
Input processes..................................................................................................... 5:46 
Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 5:46 
The semantic system ............................................................................................ 5:47 
Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 5:48 
Discussion – RPD .................................................................................................... 5:49 
Case 2 – DHT .......................................................................................................... 5:53 
Results for DHT ....................................................................................................... 5:53 
Input processes..................................................................................................... 5:53 
Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 5:54 
The semantic system ............................................................................................ 5:55 
Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 5:56 
Item consistency and comparisons........................................................................ 5:57 
Discussion – DHT.................................................................................................... 5:58 
Case 3 – DPC .......................................................................................................... 5:62 
Results for DPC ....................................................................................................... 5:62 
Input processes..................................................................................................... 5:63 
Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 5:63 
The semantic system ............................................................................................ 5:64 
Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 5:65 
Discussion – DPC.................................................................................................... 5:67 
Phonological dyslexia – general discussion.............................................................. 5:70 
Chapter 6. Interpreting results for a bilingual aphasic ............................................ 6:73 
Case description....................................................................................................... 6:73 
Control M2........................................................................................................... 6:74 
Results ..................................................................................................................... 6:74 
Input processes..................................................................................................... 6:74 
Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 6:75 
The semantic system ............................................................................................ 6:76 
Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 6:77 
Discussion................................................................................................................ 6:80 
Chapter 7. A case of deep dyslexia ............................................................................ 7:87 
Deep dyslexia........................................................................................................... 7:87 
Case description....................................................................................................... 7:88 
Results ..................................................................................................................... 7:89 
Input processes..................................................................................................... 7:89 
Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 7:90 
The semantic system ............................................................................................ 7:92 
Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 7:92 
Item consistency................................................................................................... 7:95 
Discussion................................................................................................................ 7:96
Chapter 8. Collective results for aphasic participants ............................................ 8:101 
Collective results .................................................................................................... 8:101 
Severity of aphasia and dissociations...................................................................... 8:105 
Severity .............................................................................................................. 8:105 
Dissociations and double dissociations................................................................ 8:106 
Chapter 9. General discussion ................................................................................. 9:109 
The basic model - conclusions ................................................................................ 9:109 
Reading aloud..................................................................................................... 9:109 
Semantic errors on oral naming........................................................................... 9:110 
Comments on methodological issues....................................................................... 9:113 
References.................................................................................................................... 117 
Appendices................................................................................................................... 123 
Appendix 1. Materials................................................................................................ 124 
Appendix 2. Analyses................................................................................................. 132 
Appendix 3. Control group results ............................................................................. 133 
Appendix 4. Nonword reading ................................................................................... 135 
Appendix 5. Error analysis for aphasic participants .................................................. 136 
iii
List of Figures 
Figure 1:1. The ‘basic model’ of language processing. ............................................... 1:3 
Figure 1:2. The three reading routes of the basic model: ........................................... 1:5 
Figure 1:3. The central components of any four-lexicon model. ................................ 1:8 
Figure 1:4. The hypothesis described by Orpwood and Warrington (1995). .......... 1:12 
Figure 2:1. Example Item from the comprehension test: ......................................... 2:21 
Figure 3:1. Control group performance on repetition tasks. ................................... 3:36 
Figure 4:1. The basic model, showing MWN’s proposed lesion site. ....................... 4:41 
Figure 5:1. The basic model as it applies to RPD...................................................... 5:49 
Figure 5:2. The basic model as it applies to DHT. .................................................... 5:59 
Figure 5:3. The basic model as it applies to DPC...................................................... 5:68 
Figure 6:1. Sample of written naming responses for JWS. ...................................... 6:79 
Figure 6:2. Attempted alphabet by JWS................................................................... 6:80 
Figure 6:3. The basic model as it applies to JWS...................................................... 6:82 
Figure 7:1. The basic model as it applies to SJS. ...................................................... 7:97 
iv
v 
List of Tables 
Table 2:1. Descriptive data for the aphasic participants. .........................................2:18 
Table 2:2. List and order of tests in each session. .....................................................2:24 
Table 3:1. Summary of control results on published tests. .......................................3:31 
Table 3:2. Summary of control group results on unpublished tests. ........................3:32 
Table 3:3. Most frequently incorrect items on PPT for controls. .............................3:34 
Table 4:1. MWN’s performance on tests of input processes.....................................4:38 
Table 4:2. MWN’s performance on reading and repetition tests. ............................4:38 
Table 4:3. MWN’s performance on semantic tests. ..................................................4:39 
Table 4:4. MWN’s performance on the oral naming test. ........................................4:39 
Table 4:5. MWN’s performance on the written naming test. ...................................4:40 
Table 5:1. RPD’s performance on tests of input processes. ......................................5:46 
Table 5:2. RPD’s performance on reading and repetition tests................................5:47 
Table 5:3. RPD’s performance on semantic tests. .....................................................5:47 
Table 5:4. RPD’s performance on the oral naming test............................................5:48 
Table 5:5. RPD’s performance on the written naming test. .....................................5:48 
Table 5:6. DHT’s performance on tests of input processes.......................................5:54 
Table 5:7. DHT’s performance on reading and repetition tests. ..............................5:54 
Table 5:8. DHT’s performance on semantic tests. ....................................................5:55 
Table 5:9. DHT’s performance on the oral naming test. ..........................................5:56 
Table 5:10. DHT’s performance on the written naming test. ...................................5:57 
Table 5:11. Item consistency between tests of verbal output for DHT. ....................5:58 
Table 5:12. DPC’s performance on tests of input processes. ....................................5:63 
Table 5:13. DPC’s performance on reading and repetition tests..............................5:64 
Table 5:14. DPC’s performance on semantic tests. ...................................................5:65 
Table 5:15. DPC’s performance on the oral naming test. .........................................5:65 
Table 5:16. DPC’s performance on the written naming test.....................................5:66 
Table 5:17. Item consistency between oral naming and other tasks for DPC. .........5:67 
Table 6:1.JWS’ performance on tests of input processes..........................................6:75 
Table 6:2. JWS’ performance on reading and repetition tests. ................................6:76 
Table 6:3. JWS’ performance on semantic tests. ......................................................6:77 
Table 6:4. JWS’ results on the oral naming test........................................................6:78 
Table 6:5. JWS’ performance on the written naming test. .......................................6:79 
Table 7:1. SJS’ performance on tests of input processes. .........................................7:90
Table 7:2. SJS’ performance on reading and repetition tests. ................................. 7:90 
Table 7:3. Reading errors for SJS. ............................................................................ 7:91 
Table 7:4. SJS’ performance on semantic tests......................................................... 7:92 
Table 7:5. SJS’ performance on the oral naming test............................................... 7:93 
Table 7:6. Examples of oral naming errors for SJS.................................................. 7:93 
Table 7:7. SJS’ performance on the written naming test. ........................................ 7:94 
Table 7:8. Examples of written naming errors for SJS. ........................................... 7:95 
Table 7:9. Item consistency between comprehension and naming for SJS. ............. 7:95 
Table 7:10. Item consistency between several tests for SJS...................................... 7:96 
Table 8:1. Performance of aphasic participants on tests of input processes. ......... 8:101 
Table 8:2. Performance of aphasic participants on reading and repetition tests. . 8:102 
Table 8:3. Performance of aphasic participants on semantic tests......................... 8:103 
Table 8:4. Performance of aphasic participants on the oral naming tests. ............ 8:104 
Table 8:5. Performance of aphasic participants on the written naming test ......... 8:105 
Table 8:6. Comparison of the regular and exception word groups........................ 8:105 
vi
Acknowledgements 
First and foremost, I would like to thank all of the wonderful people who participated in 
this project, without whom none of this would have been possible. For most of these 
individuals, the assessment procedure involved several hours of testing, and I am eternally 
grateful for the time and effort that you all devoted to the project. I must also thank two 
participants, TB and FME, whose results were not included in the final report but who 
gave there time nevertheless. 
Second, I would like to acknowledge the professional assistance I received from various 
people. In particular, my supervisors, Professor Max Coltheart and Associate Professor 
Lindsey Nickels, who gave their time and effort over a period of many years, and who 
never lost faith that I would eventually submit. For your time, advice, and understanding, I 
cannot thank you enough. I am also grateful to the speech pathologists as St Joseph’s 
Hospital and the Royal Rehabilitation Centre Sydney for their assistance in referring 
patients and for being extremely accommodating in providing me with their time and other 
resources. I am also indebted to many other academics and support staff of the Macquarie 
Centre for Cognitive Science and the Psychology Department of Macquarie University for 
their professional advice and assistance with resources. 
Third, I would like to thank the many amazing people in my life who I am lucky enough to 
have as family and friends. I am especially grateful to my parents, who not only provided 
me with the love and support that they always have, but who also went out of their way to 
help me with finding control participants. To all of my friends, including student peers, 
team mates, work colleagues, flat mates, and long-term friends, I cannot express how 
grateful I am for your professional support (including assistance with proof reading, 
material preparation and other advice) and, more importantly, your moral support – I 
would not have attained this feat without your compassion, reassurance, and 
understanding. 
vii
viii
ix 
Abstract 
Over recent decades, research-based cognitive models of language have become 
increasingly sophisticated. However, with increasing sophistication has come an equivalent 
increase in complexity, to the extent that it is now more difficult than ever for clinicians to 
utilise the model for testing hypotheses about patients and devise appropriate therapeutic 
interventions. A series of six cases is presented to explore the capacity of the ‘basic model’ 
to account for various aphasic profiles, with a particular focus on hypotheses about reading 
pathways. To this end, a series of experiments was designed using a single set of picture-word 
items, with a focus on the balance between words with and without regular spelling-sound 
correspondence. Various theoretical positions are discussed including the lexical 
non-semantic route, the summation hypothesis, and the hypothesis that reading aloud and 
oral naming are subserved by different phonological output lexicons (e.g. Orpwood & 
Warrington, 1995). 
Most of the aphasic participants presented with ‘output’ anomia, but for some this was in 
the context of mild semantic deficits that may have contributed to their poor oral naming. 
One of the participants was also completely unable to read nonwords, yet his reading of 
real words, although impaired, did not contain semantic errors. This is an uncommon 
finding and one that is incongruent with the summation hypothesis. Other participants 
demonstrated intact reading of exception words despite being impaired on the oral naming 
task, which further supports the inclusion of the lexical non-semantic route. 
Another of the aphasic participants was considered in the context of being a late-acquired 
bilingual speaker. He was compared not only to the main control group, but also to an 
unimpaired, late-acquired bilingual speaker with the same language background. The basic 
model was unable to account for his pattern of deficits, but it was determined that most 
cognitive models, no matter how intricate, are inadequate to account for aphasic syndromes 
in bilingual speakers. 
The final case examines the profile of a participant with deep dyslexia. Although the basic 
model is able to account for this participant’s profile, consideration is given to the right-hemisphere 
hypothesis and to the notion that, due to wide ranging and as yet unknown 
variables, standard cognitive models of language processing may again be inappropriate 
for use with these cases.
It was concluded that the evidence supported the potential of the basic model and the 
assumptions associated with it, including the lexical non-semantic route and the depiction 
of only two lexicons, one each for spoken and written lexical entries. Additionally, several 
methodological issues are discussed including poor sensitivity of several tests. 
x
1:1 
Chapter 1. General introduction 
Anomia is usually characterised as general word-finding difficulties. It can exist as a 
syndrome in itself or, more often, as a feature of a more general aphasic disorder (Garman, 
1990). Almost every aphasic individual experiences some degree of impaired word 
retrieval (e.g. Garrett, 1992; Weigel-Crump & Koenigsknecht, 1973), which is made 
evident by the fact that the most common finding of aphasic research is the inability to 
name pictures correctly (Goodglass, 1983). Analysis of the various causes of naming 
failure, and the myriad of other lexical deficits associated with it, can reveal a great deal 
about the cognitive architecture of language processing. This chapter introduces and briefly 
discusses a range of issues surrounding cognitive models of language processing. In the 
chapters that follow, some of these issues will be explored through a case series involving 
six individuals with various anomic syndromes and degrees of impairment. In particular, 
the potential for a ‘basic’ model of language processing to account for the deficits of these 
individuals will be examined, and it will be argued that this relatively uncomplicated 
model is sufficient to explain and understand acquired language deficits at a clinical level. 
Cognitive models of language processing 
In any cognitive model of lexical processing, the ability to perform normal linguistic 
functions is explained by an array of processing modules linked to each other by a network 
of pathways. These models do not aim to account for neural processing centres and 
connections, rather, they are attempts to explain the processes involved in normal lexical 
functioning, and are often constructed around hypotheses that are generated from case 
studies of individuals with language impairments. Such hypotheses are generally based on 
dissociations (i.e. when a certain process is impaired while another is intact) and, more 
importantly, double dissociations (i.e. when two separate processes can be differentially 
impaired) – for example, there are cases of impaired written naming with intact oral 
naming and vice versa, indicating a double dissociation between the process involved in 
each form of naming. 
Whilst there are many ways in which the various models differ, by their very nature there 
are many aspects that they must have in common. Specifically, all lexical models must be 
able to explain the different processes involved in understanding and producing language, 
at least at the level of single words. Therefore, all models must account for orthographic 
processing (the way we process written words), phonological processing (spoken words),
recognition of 2- and 3-dimensional objects, and semantic processing (comprehension of 
words and objects). The full range of everyday skills encompassed by a model should 
include: Confrontation naming (naming of pictures and objects), both oral and written; 
spontaneous speech and writing; recognition and comprehension of pictures, written words 
and spoken words; reading aloud; written ‘copying’ and verbal repetition; and writing to 
dictation. Also, models must account not only for our ability to process words that are 
known to us, but also words that are novel or made up. 
A simple model to explore language abilities: The ‘basic model’ 
The primary objective of this report is to show that a simple cognitive model of language is 
sufficient to account for most aphasic individuals. Being able to precisely identify a 
patient’s deficit within the context of a cognitive model can have significant implications 
for the design of therapeutic intervention. However, due to their complex nature, the 
practical application of the more sophisticated research-based models are often difficult for 
clinicians to apply and interpret. Therefore, simplifying models to a degree that they can be 
easily applied to the majority of cases could have significant implications for clinical 
practice. 
Keeping in mind the language abilities of normal speakers, in addition to the most 
commonly reported and generally agreed upon aspects, the simplest model that could be 
considered for clinical application is presented in Figure 1:1 (e.g. Allport, 1984; Allport & 
Funnell, 1981; Jackson & Coltheart, 2001). The most peripheral, non-language features 
such as initial acoustic processing and motor output are omitted, and internal processing of 
modules is not defined. 
At the centre of the basic model in Figure 1:1 is the semantic system, which stores and 
processes conceptual information about the meanings of words and objects; it represents an 
intricate network of semantic features (i.e. all the characteristics of the things that an 
individual knows). To either side of the semantic system are the phonological lexicon and 
orthographic lexicon, stores of all the spoken and written words (respectively) that an 
individual knows. 
1:2
1:3 
Figure 1:1. The ‘basic model’ of language processing. 
Input to the model can be auditory or visual. Auditory information first reaches the 
phonological input buffer, which temporarily stores and processes phonemes (small units 
of sound) before forwarding the information on to the phonological lexicon for activation 
of the appropriate word forms, and to the phonological output buffer, where phonological 
information is reorganised as speech. The pathway between the phonological input and 
output buffers is the sublexical repetition route, and allows auditory input to be re-processed 
as speech output – this is the mechanism that allows us to quickly repeat verbal 
information (both real words and nonwords). Repetition of known words can also occur via 
the phonological lexicon. Information from the phonological lexicon is also forwarded to 
the semantic system where relevant semantic nodes are activated, enabling comprehension 
of spoken words. 
Visual input to the system can take two forms. Firstly, 2- and 3-dimensional objects are 
identified and processed by the object recognition system, which then activates relevant 
nodes in the semantic system. Naming of these objects is then made possible via the 
phonological lexicon and phonological output buffer (for oral naming) or the orthographic 
lexicon and orthographic output buffer (for written naming). Secondly, written input is
processed initially by a stage of letter identification, which associates the almost infinite 
array of forms that each letter of the alphabet can take with the single letter that they 
represent (i.e. no matter how the letter a is written – e.g. a, a, A, or A – it is usually 
recognisable). 
According to the basic model, reading aloud is made possible by three different routes, all 
beginning at the stage of letter identification. The first, called the semantic route (Figure 
1:2a), proceeds to the orthographic lexicon, through the semantic system, and on to the 
phonological lexicon and phonological output buffer. The second (Figure 1:2b) is called 
the lexical non-semantic route, and also proceeds to the orthographic lexicon. At this point 
however, information is sent directly to the phonological lexicon, bypassing the semantic 
system, before being forwarded on to the phonological output buffer. This pathway allows 
for written words to be read aloud without necessarily activating semantic representations, 
and is discussed in greater detail later in the chapter. The third route (Figure 1:2c) is a 
direct connection from letter identification to the phonological output buffer via grapheme-phoneme 
1:4 
conversion. This pathway, also known as the sublexical route, allows for the 
processing of strings of graphemes (a grapheme is a letter or group of letters that represent 
a single phoneme) that do not have entries in the lexicons – that is, unfamiliar words, 
foreign words and nonwords (i.e. plausible made-up words such as ploon and chup). 
a.
1:5 
b. 
c. 
Figure 1:2. The three reading routes of the basic model: 
(a) the semantic route; (b) the lexical non-semantic route; and (c) grapheme-phoneme conversion. 
From the perspective of the basic model, reading aloud is made possible by these three 
pathways. Words with regular spelling (i.e. those that have predictable grapheme-phoneme 
correspondence and therefore sound the way they are spelled, such as dog and arm) can be 
read via any of the three routes. In contrast, exception words (words that do not sound the 
way they are spelled, such as bowl and yacht) cannot be read via grapheme-phoneme 
conversion – since grapheme-phoneme conversion only allows for direct translation of 
graphemes into phonemes, this would cause regularisation errors (e.g. bowl would be read
as ‘bowel’ and yacht would be read as ‘yatched’ or ‘yacked’). However, exception words 
can be read using either the semantic or lexical non-semantic route, since all words are 
represented in the lexicons and simply need to be activated, first in the orthographic 
lexicon, then in the phonological lexicon. Finally, novel words and nonwords can only be 
read via grapheme-phoneme conversion, since these letter strings are not represented in the 
lexicons. Damage to grapheme-phoneme conversion impairs the individual’s ability to read 
nonwords, which will often (but not always) be read as lexicalisations (e.g. ploon might be 
read as ‘plume’ or ‘prune,’ while chup might be read as ‘chap’). 
Finally, the model needs to include components that can process novel words not only in 
their written form, but also via auditory input. Repetition of novel words is achieved by the 
sublexical repetition route, which connects the phonological input and output buffers. 
Written dictation of novel words is achieved via phoneme-grapheme conversion, which is 
responsible for converting sequences of phonemes into graphemes, thus allowing a person 
to write novel strings of sounds that are heard. This process is not examined in the case 
series, but is shown in the model because its existence is well supported by evidence in the 
literature (e.g. Alario, Schiller, Domoto-Reilly, & Caramazza, 2003; Miceli, Capasso, & 
Caramazza, 1999). 
Damage to the model will result in a variety of deficits, depending on which component or 
components are damaged, and the degree to which the components are still able to function 
(see Allport, 1984; Allport & Funnell, 1981; Jackson & Coltheart, 2001). In broad terms, 
there are two ways that lesions might affect the functioning of the core components of the 
language system (i.e. the semantic system, phonological lexicon and orthographic lexicon) 
– damage to the representations within the process, or reduced activation of those 
representations. Generally, damage to the representations should lead to consistency of 
errors. In other words, if the actual representations are damaged, then the same errors will 
appear repeatedly, and for all tasks that rely on that module. On the other hand, reduced 
activation, which is generally conceptualised as damage to the connections between 
modules, is less likely to result in error consistency. 
Damage to individual components will obviously lead to a particular set of impairments. If 
the semantic system is damaged, comprehension will be impaired regardless of the method 
of input (i.e. the individual will have difficulty understanding the meanings of pictures, 
spoken words and written words). However, the most distinctive feature of ‘semantic 
1:6
anomia’ is bimodal naming failure to all forms of input (Rothi, Raymer, Maher, 
Greenwald, & Morris, 1991). That is, an impaired ability to name words both orally and in 
writing, whether the stimuli are presented as pictures, written definitions or auditory 
definitions. Semantic errors (meaning-related errors e.g. naming a car as a truck) should be 
common because damage to particular semantic representations increases the likelihood of 
lexical entries that are related by meaning being activated in the relevant lexicon (Miceli, 
Amitrano, Capasso, & Caramazza, 1996). 
Damage to a lexicon will lead to the inability to activate representations within that 
lexicon. From the perspective of the basic model, this will lead to: a) reduced ability to 
name pictures in that modality, with a range of error types including semantic and 
phonological errors; b) difficulties with lexical decision (i.e. distinguishing between real 
and made-up words) in that modality; and c) difficulties with comprehension of words 
input from that modality. Other abilities might be partially affected. In particular, reading 
aloud of exception words should lead to regularisation errors if either lexicon is damaged, 
and if grapheme-phoneme conversion is intact. Likewise, writing of exception words to 
dictation should be affected by damage to the phonological lexicon. However, nonword 
reading, repetition and writing to dictation should all be possible, even if both lexicons are 
damaged. In contrast, reduced activation of the lexicons from the semantic system should 
lead to impaired picture naming of that modality, without affecting any other language 
skill. Errors should be similar in nature to those seen for lexicon damage, including 
semantic errors, but with less consistency predicted. 
Post-lexical damage should also have similarities to lexical damage. In particular, damage 
to the connection between the phonological lexicon and phonological output buffer should 
impact on oral naming and reading of exception words. For naming, semantic errors would 
not be expected since the lexical entry has already been selected. On the other hand, 
auditory lexical decision should still be possible, as should repetition of words (via the 
sublexical repetition route). Damage to the connection between the orthographic lexicon 
and orthographic output buffer should mirror this pattern for writing. Finally, damage to 
the input or output buffers should affect all input or output for that modality, while damage 
to the object recognition process should affect all tasks that involve some aspect of 
interpreting pictures or objects. 
1:7
Two lexicons or four? 
Perhaps the most audacious argument presented in the basic model is that only two 
lexicons are defined, one each for spoken and written words. In contrast, the majority of 
mainstream models describe separate lexicons for input and for output for each modality, 
as depicted in Figure 1:3 below. Only the central components are shown, with peripheral 
features omitted (e.g. input and output buffers, grapheme-phoneme conversion and direct 
links between the lexicons), as are any hypothesised feedback mechanisms and 
connections between the lexicons. This is because of the diverse range of configurations 
that the various models hypothesise. On the other hand, the central features that are 
pictured are common to most cognitive models of language processing (e.g. Hillis & 
Caramazza, 1991; Martin & Saffran, 2002; Miceli et al., 1996; Nickels, 2000; Southwood 
& Chatterjee, 2001). 
Figure 1:3. The central components of any four-lexicon model. 
A considerable number of debates surround this issue (refer to Howard, 1995, for an 
extensive discussion on the topic; also see Martin & Saffran, 2002). However, despite the 
general consensus of four lexicons, the objective of this report is to show that a simple 
model is sufficient to account for the language of people with aphasia in a clinical setting. 
Therefore, judgments as to whether or not aspects of particular models are fundamental 
should not be restricted to peripheral components and pathways; determining the relevance 
of core components, in particular the number of lexicons, is just as crucial. A 
demonstration that two lexicons are sufficient would considerably decrease the complexity 
of cognitive models of language. The majority of evidence that favours the position of a 
distinction between input and output lexicons is not based on cases for which 
1:8
representations are clearly lost in one but preserved in the other. Rather, the arguments are 
based primarily on findings from intricate research methodology such as ‘dual-task 
decrement’ (Shallice, McLeod, & Lewis, 1985) and research findings as they relate to 
certain theoretical assumptions (see Howard & Franklin, 1988). However, at a clinical 
level there is often a lack of distinction between input and output modules when 
identifying deficits. Therefore, for the sake of simplicity the lexicons were not divided into 
input and output processes, in accordance with previous advocates of this approach (e.g. 
Allport & Funnell, 1981; Funnell, 1983; Jackson & Coltheart, 2001) 
1:9 
Different accounts of reading aloud 
At first glance, the lexical non-semantic reading route might appear to be redundant. 
Indeed, it is not entirely clear what purpose it serves for normal language, and is not 
included in many models, such as the summation hypothesis (e.g. Hillis & Caramazza, 
1991) and Plaut’s computational model (Plaut, McClelland, Seidenberg, & Patterson, 
1996). However, omission of the lexical non-semantic route leads to certain predictions 
concerning word reading for individuals with damage to the semantic reading route. First 
of all, if grapheme-phoneme conversion is impaired, then reading aloud of real words 
should include frequent semantic intrusions or omissions (Miceli et al., 1996). Indeed, this 
is the profile observed for deep dyslexia. However, not all individuals with impaired 
semantic processing and non-functional grapheme-phoneme conversion produce semantic 
errors on word reading. For example, WB (Funnell, 1983) performed poorly on tests of 
semantic processing and was completely unable to read nonwords, to the extent that he was 
unable to generate a response for more than half of the items. Nevertheless, he performed 
reading tasks with very few semantic errors or omissions. 
The second prediction is that even if reading aloud is impaired (and includes semantic 
errors) in addition to non-functional grapheme-phoneme conversion, as is generally the 
case in deep dyslexia, then this function should be just as severely compromised as oral 
naming, since the absence of grapheme-phoneme conversion should lead to a complete 
reliance on the semantic reading route. However, reading aloud is consistently reported to 
be superior to oral naming provided orthographic input is intact, even for individuals with 
deep dyslexia. This phenomenon is “only consistent with the partial operation of (the 
lexical non-semantic route)” (Howard, 1985, p403).
A third prediction is that if grapheme-phoneme conversion is intact or at least partially 
active, then regularisation errors should occur on reading of exception words. In other 
words, surface dyslexia should be evident (Patterson, Marshall, & Coltheart, 1985). 
However, Weekes and Robinson (1997) report BP, whose performance on semantic tasks 
such as word-picture and picture-picture matching was impaired. Furthermore, he was able 
to name barely more than half of the picture items in the Snodgrass and Vanderwart 
corpus, and nearly half of his errors were semantic errors. His nonword reading was also 
impaired, though he successfully read approximately half of the items on a nonword 
reading task. Nevertheless, on a set of 40 exception words, BP made only one error 
(reading thumb as thump, most likely a visual error). This is considered by Weekes as 
strong evidence that BP is reading via a lexical pathway that does not involve semantic 
processing. This prediction also applies to post-semantic naming impairments – MRF was 
considered to have an intact semantic system but was impaired on oral naming, with 
partially active grapheme-phoneme conversion, yet there was no effect of regularity 
observed (Orpwood & Warrington, 1995). 
Although the predictions made by most cognitive models of language processing with only 
two reading pathways are not supported by the literature, the summation hypothesis (e.g. 
Hillis & Caramazza, 1991; Hillis, Rapp, & Caramazza, 1999; Miceli et al., 1996; Miceli, 
Capasso, & Caramazza, 1994; Miceli, Giustolisi, & Caramazza, 1991), considers reading 
aloud of real words to be achieved by the ‘summation’ of lexical and sublexical processes. 
If the semantic reading route is only partially operational but the sublexical process is also 
providing full or partial activation, reading of words, both regular and irregular, is still 
possible. Partial semantic activation means that semantically appropriate representations in 
the phonological output lexicon, including the target, are partially activated (e.g. the word 
yacht will activate representations such as boat, mast, sail, and of course yacht). At the 
same time, the sublexical process activates all phonologically appropriate representations 
in the phonological lexicon (so yacht might activate representations for words such as yet, 
yurt and, again, yacht). Therefore, the only node in the phonological output lexicon that 
will be activated above threshold is the target word, yacht. All other representations that 
are activated will fail to reach threshold. 
However, an important assumption of the summation hypothesis is that a complete lack of 
input from the sublexical process, in conjunction with a lesion at some stage of the 
semantic reading route, should lead to frequent semantic errors in word reading (i.e. deep 
1:10
dyslexia). On the other hand, partial activation from the sublexical process should all but 
eliminate semantic errors (and reduce total errors), as seen in phonological dyslexia. As 
mentioned, however, evidence from the phonological dyslexic WB (Funnell, 1983) 
suggests that this distinction between phonological and deep dyslexia does not always hold 
true. This debate is examined further in the case of DHT, who is presented in Chapter 5. 
The relationship between reading aloud and oral picture naming 
While there are many different interpretations of how word reading can be achieved 
through lexical, sublexical and lexical non-semantic processes, language researchers agree 
almost universally that the phonological process of reading words aloud overlaps with the 
phonological process of oral picture naming. However, a challenge to this principle was 
the suggestion that reading and oral naming have distinct phonological stores that can each 
be selectively damaged. The first clear presentation of this hypothesis appeared in a 1995 
article by Orpwood and Warrington. They described MRF, an individual with poor oral 
naming of pictures and poor naming to definition, with frequent semantic errors. As 
demonstrated by his poor nonword reading, MRF had only partial access to grapheme-phoneme 
conversion. MRF was able to read real words, with no difference between regular 
and exception items, and his repetition of nonwords was intact; therefore he must have had 
a lesion affecting the grapheme-phoneme conversion process. Comprehension was also 
intact, suggesting a lesion of the phonological output lexicon. However, from the 
perspective of most serial models, this should also impair word reading. 
As can be seen in Figure 1:4, the authors propose that the lesion affecting oral naming is 
located at a phonological output lexicon that is unique for oral naming tasks (lesion a). 
Grapheme-phoneme conversion is also impaired (lesion b), but all other processes are 
intact, including an additional phonological output store for word reading; the presence of 
semantic errors in oral naming, and complete absence of them in reading, is considered 
further justification for their position. The authors reject the summation hypothesis as a 
plausible account on the basis that his grapheme-phoneme conversion is too severely 
impaired to adequately contribute to reading. 
Support for this hypothesis was provided by an apparent double dissociation between 
reading and oral naming. BF was described by Goldblum (1985), and was remarkable in 
that he was described as having intact oral naming yet impaired word reading (despite 
intact comprehension for words). According to Breen and Warrington (1995), BF contrasts 
with the many individuals reported for whom reading is intact while oral naming is 
1:11
impaired, thus representing a double dissociation between these two abilities. In the 
context of most mainstream models, there is no way to account for this phenomenon. The 
solution, according to the authors, is independent stores for each task. Extending this 
hypothesis, the authors conducted a series of priming experiments with participant NOR. 
They found that priming by first reading the word had very little effect on NOR’s oral 
naming unless the delay was extremely short. Although they concede that very little is 
known about the specific effects of priming at the level of the phonological lexicon, they 
consider this finding to represent a possible dissociation between the phonological 
processes involved in reading aloud and oral naming. 
Figure 1:4. The hypothesis described by Orpwood and Warrington (1995). 
Green boxes and arrows indicate intact processing; red boxes and arrows represent the hypothesised 
lesions. Only relevant processes are shown. 
The most significant feature of the hypothesis of independent phonological stores is the 
claim of a double dissociation between oral naming and reading aloud. However, this 
position is challenged by Lambon Ralph, Cippoloti and Patterson (1999), who argue that 
BF’s naming was not necessarily superior to oral reading, as purported by Goldblum 
(1985). Three reason are given for this challenge: First, BF’s profile represented a complex 
pattern of various dyslexic syndromes, rather than a single syndrome that could be 
accounted for by an isolated lesion of output phonology; second, and most significantly, 
1:12
reading and oral naming were not compared for the same set of words; finally, Goldblum 
considered BF’s naming to be less impaired than reading partly because the majority of his 
errors were almost always corrected – Lambon Ralph and colleagues (1999) argue that this 
is far from a clear demonstration of normal functioning. 
The claim that the summation hypothesis is unable to account for NOR and MRF is also 
disputed by Lambon Ralph and colleagues. Rather, the presence of semantic errors in 
naming but not in reading can be attributed to direct input from sublexical processes 
because only minimal orthographic information is needed to block semantic errors. For 
their participant MOS, who also performed poorly on oral naming and well on reading 
aloud, they suggest that the phonological output lexicon itself is preserved, as is the 
semantic system. Instead, it is the connection between these systems that is severed, with 
reading aided by the sublexical process. 
In accordance with the summation hypothesis, Lambon Ralph and colleagues (1999) go 
further by suggesting that the reason why oral naming is frequently found to be impaired in 
the context of intact reading is that oral naming is simply more vulnerable. There are two 
factors that contribute to this vulnerability. First, there is no direct correspondence between 
conceptual knowledge about an object and the phonological representation of that object’s 
name, while reading is largely aided by the ‘quasi-regular’ mapping between orthography 
and phonology. Second, only one source of phonological activation is available to oral 
naming, while reading has at least two. In support of this claim is evidence that oral 
naming in anomic participants can be improved by an additional source of phonological 
activation such as phonemic cueing, making it as robust as reading aloud with its two 
sources of phonological activation (Lambon Ralph, 1998; Lambon Ralph et al., 1999). 
In a third article aimed at supporting the notion of multiple phonological output stores, 
Crutch and Warrington (2001) present VYG, whose spontaneous speech was intact, but 
whose oral naming and reading aloud were both impaired. Oral naming responses 
consisted mostly of circumlocutions, with few phonological errors, while reading errors 
were all phonological. Since VYG was able to comprehend words that he was unable to 
read aloud, the authors concluded that the site of damage must be at the level of a 
phonological output store, or perhaps access to the output store from semantics. Despite 
the fact that VYG’s naming was more severely impaired than his reading, the authors 
claim that damage to the ‘stronger’ reading process should affect naming in the same way 
1:13
– the high number of phonological errors in reading, and almost complete absence of them 
in naming, is therefore considered evidence for a double dissociation between the tasks. 
However, there are several flaws in the logic of the articles discussed above. Firstly, if 
reading aloud and oral naming are enabled by separate phonological stores, then the double 
dissociation between them should not be restricted to differences in error patterns. There 
should be individuals reported in the literature for whom reading is worse than oral naming 
for the same items, a phenomenon which has not yet been described. Secondly, Crutch and 
Warrington (2001) claim that VYG’s comprehension of words that he is unable to read 
aloud indicates that his semantic system is unaffected. They fail to observe the principle 
that receptive tasks such as word-picture matching place considerably less strain on the 
semantic system than do expressive tasks (e.g. Howard, 1985; Laine, Kujala, Niemi, & 
Uusipaikka, 1992; Lambon Ralph, Sage, & Roberts, 2000). If VYG does have a mild 
semantic deficit, this could have a noticeable impact on oral naming, including generation 
of semantic errors, with less of an impact on reading, which is assisted by partially intact 
grapheme-phoneme conversion – thus leading to more phonological errors. Thirdly, many 
authors argue that discrepancies of error types should actually be expected for the same 
reason that reading aloud is considered to be less vulnerable to impairment than oral 
naming (e.g. Newcombe & Marshall, 1980; Southwood & Chatterjee, 2000, 2001). If 
additional phonological input constrains the responses, then more phonological errors, and 
less semantic/circumlocutory errors should be evident. 
Research Aims 
The general aim of this report is to demonstrate that the basic model of language 
processing, as described in this chapter, could be a useful clinical tool to aid the 
understanding of aphasic patients. To this end, the following predictions were made: 
1) The basic model will be sufficient to account for each individual’s profile, or at least as 
capable as any existing model. 
2) Two lexicons, one each for phonological and orthographic representations, are 
sufficient to explain the majority of aphasic participants. 
3) The lexical non-semantic route is an essential component of serial models. Therefore: 
a) Participants with significantly impaired oral naming (that is not caused by pre-semantic 
1:14 
damage) but with intact reading are best accounted for by the existence of 
this pathway.
b) If grapheme-phoneme conversion is completely abolished for an individual with 
damage to the semantic reading route, deep dyslexia will only result if the lexical 
non-semantic route is also damaged. 
c) Reading impairments exhibited by anomic participants will conform to models of 
language retrieval that assume a shared phonological process for reading aloud and 
for oral naming (i.e. Orpwood and Warrington’s (1995) hypothesis of distinct 
phonological stores will not be supported). 
The critical motivation for this study was the paucity of literature in which a single set of 
stimuli is used for a variety of language tasks. By developing a range of tests with a single 
set of words-picture items, aphasic participants could be assessed in such a way that intact 
and defective functions could be determined with much greater confidence than if a variety 
of different tests had been used, thus providing insight into what aspects of cognitive 
architecture are required to account for the participants. Furthermore, by carefully 
balancing the group of items so that half would have word names with regular spelling and 
the other half irregular, it was expected that a great deal more might be revealed about the 
process of reading aloud. 
The next chapter describes the processes involved in material preparation, recruitment of 
suitable participants, assessment procedures and analysis of results. 
1:15
1:16
2:17 
Chapter 2. Method 
As was described in the previous section, one aim of this project was to assess the validity 
of claims made initially by Orpwood and Warrington (1995) that reading aloud and oral 
naming are subserved by distinct phonological stores. This chapter describes the 
recruitment of participants and the tests used, including the development of the five 
unpublished tests that were designed to investigate the Orpwood and Warrington (1995) 
hypothesis. It also describes the procedures that were followed for administration, scoring 
and analysis of the battery of tests. As will be made clear, the lack of evidence for or 
against this hypothesis did little to diminish the value of the results. 
Participants 
Recruitment of aphasic participants 
Aphasic participants were recruited with the assistance of Speech Pathologists at the Royal 
Rehabilitation Centre Sydney and St Joseph’s Hospital, through the Macquarie University 
Psychology Clinic, and researchers at the Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science. The 
criteria for recruitment were adults with aphasia sustained at least 6 months prior to the 
assessment, who presented primarily with anomia, without excessive interference from 
complicating factors such as impaired hearing or vision, global cognitive dysfunction or 
prominent motor-speech deficits, including dysarthria or verbal dyspraxia. Individuals with 
mild complicating deficits were still requested with the understanding that they would be 
excluded if necessary, though none were excluded on this basis. Individuals were also 
excluded if they were identified as having recent psychiatric risk factors such as suicidal 
ideation, depression or heightened anxiety. 
A total of 12 potential participants were recruited. Of these, 7 were considered appropriate 
based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria. One was excluded due to a near-ceiling 
performance on most tests, two had recovered to the point that they were speaking fluently 
in conversational speech, and two individuals who showed interest were excluded on the 
basis of psychiatric conditions as it was considered unethical to risk placing them into a 
potentially stressful situation. One participant, FME, was described in a separate report in 
relation to her diagnosis of herpes simplex encephalitis, and is not discussed any further in 
this dissertation. Descriptive data for the six remaining participants appear in Table 2:1 
below. Each participant is described in detail in the following chapters.
Chapter 
Partic-ipant 
2:18 
Age 
Education 
Sex 
Description of 
injury/illness 
Months 
Since 
Injury 
Acute deficits 
(immediately 
post-onset) 
Relevant Medical 
History 
Vision/ 
Glasses 
4 MWN 76 10 F 
LMCA 
ischaemic with 
minor cortical 
atrophy 
8 
Broca's aphasia; 
dysarthria; mild 
verbal dyspraxia; 
mild right arm 
weakness 
AMI 1990; mitral 
valve repair; TIA; 
hypercholesterole 
mia 
Bifocals 
5 RPD 65 10 M LMCA infarct 29 Unknown 
Right meningioma 
and debulking 
surgery; CABG; 
high cholesterol 
Glasses 
(short and 
reading) 
5 DPC 51 11 F 
LMCA 
haemorrhagic 
56 
Confusion; 
aphasia 
Type II DM; 
migraines; anxiety 
disorder 
Reading 
5 DHT 62 9 M 
LMCA cerebral 
embolic infarct 
35 
Right hemiplegia; 
non-fluent 
aphasia, 
agrammatism 
Infective 
endocarditis; 
CABG 
Bifocals 
6 JWS 69 9 M 
LMCA 
ischaemic 
24 
Right hemiparesis; 
hemisensory loss; 
global aphasia 
Unknown Reading 
7 SJS 43 10 M 
LMCA 
haemorrhagic 
with bifurcation 
aneurism 
83 
Severe frontal 
headache; 
vomiting; global 
aphasia/dysphonia 
Hypertension 
Glasses 
(short) 
Table 2:1. Descriptive data for the aphasic participants. 
Education = total years of formal education; LMCA = left middle cerebral artery; CABG = coronary 
artery bypass graft; AMI = acute myocardial infarction; TIA = transient ischaemic attack; DM = 
diabetes mellitus. 
Recruitment of unimpaired controls 
Unimpaired controls were recruited through personal contacts, and were seen in two 
groups. The first group took part in the validation stage, and consisted of 10 age 
appropriate controls (M = 59.63, SD = 4.35) with appropriate anticipated years of 
education (M = 11.7, SD = 2.63). These participants were selected on the basis of expected 
age and education levels of the ABI participants, who had not yet been identified. For the 
second control group, 16 unimpaired participants were initially recruited, of which two had 
also been involved in the validation stage. One participant, M2, emigrated from the 
Netherlands at the age of 21. Because English is his second language, he was excluded 
from the main control group. However, his data are presented in Chapter 6 as a comparison 
for JWS, an aphasic participant with a similar background.
The remaining 15 individuals, 8 females and 7 males, were included in the main group. 
Independent t-tests revealed no significant difference between the seven original aphasics 
and the control group for either age (aphasics M = 61.00, SD = 12.08; controls M = 60.20, 
SD = 6.35; t(19) = 0.20, p = 0.84) or years of formal education (aphasics M = 9.83, SD = 
0.75; controls M = 10.07, SD = 1.16; t(19) = 0.45, p = 0.66). 
Many of the control participants wore glasses, and several had mild visual impairments 
(e.g. cataracts) though testing did not reveal any obvious visual difficulties (i.e. they did 
not perform any worse than other controls on tests that might be sensitive to visual 
impairment). Also, four members of the main control group (three males, one female) 
reported mild hearing difficulties, which were not identified until nonword repetition was 
attempted. The justification for including these individuals is that such mild hearing loss 
and visual difficulties are clearly common in this population, and difficult to identify. 
Therefore, similar difficulties cannot be eliminated as a cause of poor performance for 
some of the aphasic participants; the effect of mild hearing loss on repetition tasks is 
discussed in Chapter 3. Two participants emigrated from England about 25 years ago, and 
the results of these individuals are also examined more closely in Chapter 3. 
2:19 
Materials 
One of the key predictions made by the Orpwood and Warrington (1995) hypothesis is that 
if oral naming is impaired, and the cause of this impairment can be localised to the 
phonological output lexicon, then words with regular spelling should be less affected on a 
reading task than words with irregular spelling, assuming that grapheme-phoneme 
conversion is still involved. Determining the effects of regularity on reading performance 
is also tantamount to hypotheses relating to the lexical non-semantic route. Therefore, the 
primary objective when preparing the materials was to focus on this contrast between 
regular and exception words by gathering two word lists that differed only in this respect. 
That is, the word items needed to be matched on criteria such as frequency and linguistic 
complexity. In order to further limit potential differences in linguistic complexity, only 
monosyllabic words were chosen. Since the items also needed to be named, only words 
that could be easily elicited by their pictures were appropriate, which considerably limited 
the number of appropriate items. For example, a picture of a yacht will just as often be 
named as a boat; pictures of a buoy and a raft proved to be difficult to identify for many 
people.
After an extensive period of item selection and refinement, including informal testing and 
discussion with peers, 104 items were selected from the list of monosyllabic words in the 
CELEX lexical database (Baayen, Piepenbrock, & Van Rijn, 1993). The pictures were 
obtained primarily from Hemera Photo Objects (Hemera, 1997-2000), with gaps filled by 
non-copyright pictures obtained from the internet. Alterations were made where necessary 
to exclude distracting aspects of the images or to highlight the relevant part of the picture. 
The regular and exception word sets were matched for spoken and written frequency 
(Baayen et al., 1993), number of phonemes, number of letters, the number of plural words 
(only one item in each set (shorts/blinds) was a plural word), and whether the item was 
animate or inanimate. Since many nouns also act as verbs (e.g. axe, bowl, or comb), which 
can have a considerable impact on frequency effects, only items that were deemed to be 
used most often as nouns were selected. Comparisons were analysed using t-test and 
Fisher’s exact calculations, with the results presented in Appendix 1. Following the 
validation phase of the research (see the Procedures section that follows) the final 
word/picture set included 40 items with regular spelling and 40 exception items, with 
classification determined by the set of grapheme to phoneme correspondence rules listed 
by Rastle and Coltheart (1999). 
These 80 items were used for four simple tests of language ability: Oral naming, written 
naming, reading aloud and repetition. As it was anticipated that some participants might 
have considerable difficulties with written picture naming and that they would be unable to 
complete the test, the first 20 items on this test were also matched as per the criteria listed 
above. Again, comparisons were by way of t-tests and Fisher’s exact, with the results 
appearing in Appendix 1. Presentation order of items in each test was pseudorandom – 
items were selected at random but relocated to ensure that no more than three consecutive 
items were related by regularity, semantic field or phonological similarity. 
Additionally, a word-picture matching task was designed to determine whether or not 
participants had intact access to the semantic representations of the test items from the 
written word. A multiple-choice format was used. For each item, the target word appeared 
in the middle, with four pictures around the word. The pictures were equated in size as 
much as possible, but often needed to be slightly different to remain size appropriate (e.g. a 
picture of a cat needs to be larger than a picture of a mouse). An example item from the 
2:20
2:21 
comprehension test appears in Figure 2:1. For each written word item, the pictures 
included: 
a) The target picture; 
b) A semantic distractor – the regular and exception word groups were matched for 
degree of semantic relatedness between the distractor and the target based on 
figures sourced from Maki, McKinley and Thompson (2004) as well as the type of 
semantic relationship (each pair was broadly classified as either related by 
association, such as bowl and spoon, or simply being members of the same 
category, such as an axe and a saw); 
c) A phonological/orthographic distractor – the two groups were matched for degree 
of phonological relatedness; and 
d) An unrelated distractor. 
bowl 
Figure 2:1. Example Item from the comprehension test: 
The given word item (bowl), the target picture, the semantic distractor (spoon), the phonological 
distractor (bell), and the unrelated distractor (tricycle). 
Most pictures appeared more than once throughout the test, though none appeared more 
than three times in total (including once as the target, for many of the pictures). The full 
list of items for the comprehension test appears in Appendix 1, along with relatedness 
figures and classifications, and statistical calculations. 
Other tests: Aphasic participants were also assessed on several published tests in order to 
assess the integrity of other aspects of the lexical system. The following tests were 
administered: 
• Tests from the Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language Processing in Aphasia 
(PALPA, Kay, Lesser, & Coltheart, 1992):
2:22 
o Visual lexical decision (subtest 25) – spelling-sound regularity 
(distinguishing real words (regular and exception) from nonwords 
(pseudohomophones and non-homophonic nonwords)). This test was used 
to assess the integrity of the orthographic lexicon and input to it. Chance is 
50% on this test. 
o Homophone decision (subtest 28) – judging whether or not pairs of words 
(with regular and irregular spelling) or nonwords sound the same. This test 
relies on the integrity of multiple components of lexical processing, 
including the orthographic lexicon, phonological lexicon, grapheme-phoneme 
conversion and the phonological output buffer. The error pattern 
of this task, in particular the contrast between real word and nonword pairs, 
is often more important than the total score. Chance is 50% for this test also. 
o Nonword reading and repetition (subtest 36) – grapheme-phoneme 
conversion and the sublexical repetition route can potentially play an 
important role in processing of words, particularly when other abilities are 
impaired. Therefore, assessment of nonword reading and repetition was 
vital. To enable relevant comparisons, it was also crucial that the nonword 
items be comparable to items used for the unpublished tests (i.e. the 80 
regular and exception words discussed previously). Indeed, two-tailed 
independent t-tests revealed no significant difference between the 80 test 
items and the 24 nonwords used in PALPA for either number of letters (for 
real words M = 4.30, SD = 0.79; for nonwords M = 4.50, SD = 1.14, t (102) 
= 0.98, p = 0.33) or number of phonemes (for real words M = 3.30, SD = 
0.80; for nonwords M = 3.42, SD = 0.72, t (102) = 0.64, p = 0.52). 
o Cross-case matching (subtest 19) and, for participants who made errors on 
this test, mirror reversal (subtest 18). These tests were intended to eliminate 
an impairment of letter identification as the cause of a participant’s 
difficulties with processing written words. 
• Pyramids and Palm Trees test (PPT, Howard & Patterson, 1992) – this test requires 
the participant to match the stimulus item (picture, written word or spoken word) to an 
associated item from a choice of two semantically related pictures. Three versions 
were utilised in order to assess the integrity of the semantic system and input to it: 
o 3 pictures version – poor performance relative to the other versions might 
suggest reduced input from object recognition.
o 2 pictures + 1 written word version – relatively poor performance suggests 
2:23 
reduced input to the semantic system from the orthographic lexicon. 
o 2 pictures + 1 spoken word version – relatively poor performance suggests 
reduced input to the semantic system from the phonological lexicon. 
Equal difficulty with all three versions is indicative of damage to representations 
within the semantic system. 
• From the Birmingham Object Recognition Battery (BORB, Riddoch & Humphreys, 
1993): 
o Subtest A (hard). This subtest is comprised of 32 black and white drawings 
of which half are real and half are made up from two different objects (e.g. 
the body of a cow with the head of a horse). This tests the integrity of the 
object recognition process. 
Procedures 
Validation phase: The original 104 pictures were shown to the validation group of controls 
on the screen of a 17” laptop computer using Microsoft PowerPoint. In cases where the 
target was provided in conjunction with an appropriate non-target word (e.g. ‘crow, bird’ 
for the desired target of crow), the target was considered to have been achieved (on testing, 
aphasic participants and members of the second control group were prompted to provide 
another response if they answered with an appropriate non-target word). Likewise, if the 
target response was included as part of a larger, similarly appropriate response (e.g. steak 
 ‘t-bone steak’; plane  ‘aeroplane’), the item was considered appropriate for inclusion, 
and hence correct if produced by the aphasic participants and members of the second 
control group. Items were only included if the target word was achieved by nine out of ten 
controls in the validation group, and the two word groups (regular/exception words) were 
matched for the number of participants who named each word correctly (mean number 
correct out of 10 for the regular group was 9.85 (SD = 0.33) and for the exception word 
group 9.75 (SD = 0.44), t(78) = 0.42; p = 0.68). 
Experimental phase: The items for four tests were shown to all participants on a 17” laptop 
screen using Microsoft PowerPoint – the items for the repetition task were read by the 
examiner. For picture naming (oral and written) and reading, five seconds was allowed for 
the response, with the timing controlled by the computer (a further 5 seconds was allowed 
if the participant was prompted to provide a different response, as described for the 
validation study). For written naming, the time limit only applied to the commencement of
writing a name to allow for any motor difficulties (i.e. extra time was allowed for slow 
writing, within reason). For repetition, the 80 items were read to the participant, with 5 
seconds allowed for each response. Ten seconds was allowed for each item on the 
comprehension test. A five second gap (a blank screen) separated each item on all tests 
except for repetition, for which one to two seconds separated each response from the 
following item. Participants were permitted to move through the computerised tests faster 
by pressing an appropriate key on the keyboard. 
The assessments with all participants were conducted over four sessions, with each session 
a week apart (or within 2 days). The tests administered in each session are listed in Table 
2:2 below. The unpublished tests were spread out over the sessions to reduce the effects of 
priming. The exception was the last session, during which written naming was followed 
soon after by repetition; it was considered too impractical and burdensome on the 
participants to extend testing beyond four sessions. Controls were assessed on all tests 
except for cross-case matching, on which unimpaired individuals are assumed to be 100% 
accurate. 
2:24 
Session 1 
 Interview 
 Comprehension test 
 PPT (3 pictures) 
Session 2 
 Oral naming test 
 PPT (2 pictures, 1 written word) 
 Visual lexical decision – regularity (PALPA: 25) 
 Object decision (BORB: Subtest A – Hard) 
 Homophone decision (PALPA: 28) 
 Nonword reading (PALPA: 36) 
Session 3 
 Reading test 
 PPT (2 pictures, 1 spoken word) 
 Nonword repetition (PALPA: 36) 
 Cross-case matching (PALPA: 19) 
Session 4 
 Written naming test 
 Repetition test 
Table 2:2. List and order of tests in each session. 
Italics indicate unpublished tests. 
The structure of testing was not varied between participants; all aphasic and unimpaired 
participants completed the tests in the same order. This was to ensure consistency with, and 
therefore enable accurate interpretation of, practice effects and priming.
2:25 
Scoring 
For the unpublished tests, clarification of certain error types is needed: 
• Phonological error was scored when at least half of the target phonemes were 
produced in the correct position. 
• Spelling error was scored for written naming if at least half of the target letters were 
produced in the correct position (e.g. chefchark). 
• Mixed errors were considered unrelated unless there was an obvious connection 
with the target item (e.g. bone  dag (presumably dog) in written naming was 
considered a semantic error). 
• Errors that were self-corrected within the time limit were considered correct without 
further consideration. 
• Morphological errors were primarily inflectional errors (mostly addition or deletion 
of the plural –s). 
• Based on the responses of controls, plural variation in picture naming was 
considered acceptable for two items, blind/s, for which both variants are common, and 
gate/s (which was generally named as the singular, but since the picture was of a two-part 
gate this could not be considered an error). Also, the pronunciation of vase varied 
(either pronounced /vaz/ or /veIs/). 
• No response errors included items for which some effort was made but nothing 
meaningful (i.e. only one phoneme or letter) was generated. 
Finally, although errors on the comprehension test appear fairly straightforward, there is at 
least two ways that the actual error types could reflect problems such as reduced visual 
acuity or scanning. First, the phonological distractors more often than not had names that 
were visually similar to the target (e.g. ball/bell; nose/hose) – therefore, many phonological 
errors could actually be visual or orthographic errors. Second, many of the semantic 
distractors were not only visually similar to the target, but in some cases were actually 
more prominent (especially when the distractor picture, but not the target, had the 
background removed) – therefore, some semantic errors could actually reflect failure to 
adequately scan all components of the item, which might account for the rare control 
errors. 
Analyses 
Measures of impairment: To ascertain whether or not an aphasic participant performed 
significantly worse than the control group, the Bayesian methodology of Crawford and
Garthwaite (2007) was employed (using the software for simple difference, cited in the 
same article). This was the primary calculation used for determining whether or not a 
participant had performed significantly worse than the control group on a particular task. 
Because the regular and exception groups were so well matched in terms of control 
performance, Fisher’s exact test (an unstardardised method of comparing independent 
groups) was used to determine differences, rather than Crawford and Garthwaite’s 
standardised calculation, which was influenced by ceiling effects. 
For certain participants, the discrepancy between two unpublished tests was measured with 
McNemar’s Test, with the obvious caveat that the tests differ slightly in their levels of 
difficulty, meaning a certain level of subjective interpretation was unavoidable. The 
Crawford and Garthwaite method (2007) proved to be inappropriate for judging these 
discrepancies and dissociations due to the differing influences of ceiling effects on the 
different tests. 
Item consistency: An important consideration for error analysis is item consistency, or the 
comparison between two tasks for a particular set of items. Since language based entries 
are conceptualised as representations stored within the semantic system and each of the 
lexicons, damage to particular representations should lead to errors on the relevant items 
regardless of the task, assuming that the same processing module is necessary for each of 
the tasks being compared. For example, damage to representations in the semantic system 
might lead to item consistency for oral naming, written naming and word-picture matching 
for particular items, but not necessarily for repetition or reading; damage to representations 
in the orthographic lexicon might lead to consistency for reading, word-picture matching 
and written naming, but not repetition or oral naming. On the other hand, a lesion that 
causes reduced activation of a processing module, rather than damage to the 
representations in the module, would not be expected to result in such consistency. 
Therefore, item consistency can, in certain conditions, provide an indication of the extent 
to which two deficits might be related by a single lesion. 
However, there are several aspects of item consistency that warrant caution when 
interpreting the results. First of all, not all tasks have the same ‘degree of difficulty’ – even 
for unimpaired individuals, written naming is usually performed less well than oral 
naming, at least for English in which written naming entails not only naming the picture, 
but also retrieving details about complex spelling rules and a large number of memorised 
2:26
word spellings that do not abide by rules or even a consistent exception to the rule (for 
example, it would not be unusual for some unimpaired individuals to be unable to spell 
words such as yacht). Furthermore, impaired participants could easily have multiple lesion 
sites affecting particular abilities, yet it is still relevant to investigate the possibility that 
one of the lesions is at least partially responsible for two or more of the deficits. Therefore, 
calculation of item consistency between different tasks should include an element of 
maximum consistency or ‘maximum overlap,’ which is discussed shortly. 
The second caution relating to item consistency is that a certain level of similarity is often 
expected between two tasks even if the difficulties on the tasks are not the result of a single 
lesion. This argument relates most prominently to the relationship between oral and written 
naming, and arose from observations that certain participants with post-semantic naming 
impairments would demonstrate statistical consistency between the two tasks, suggesting 
to many that there could be an additional process after the semantic system but before the 
lexicons (e.g. Levelt et al., 1991; Raymer et al., 1997; Raymer, Maher, Foundas, Rothi,  
Heilman, 2000). However, several authors have questioned the need for this additional 
process in accounting for item consistency. For example, Miceli and colleagues (1991) 
consider a certain level of consistency to simply represent deficits resulting from co-occurring 
2:27 
lesions affected by the same linguistic factors such as word frequency, 
imageability and linguistic complexity. That is, for any particular set of words, it is likely 
that the least frequent and most complex words will be the most vulnerable. This can lead 
to consistency between any tasks that happen to share the same common pressures. 
Furthermore, the particular common pressures are different for different pairs of tasks. For 
example, for oral and written naming, word frequency and imageability are likely to play a 
role, while for reading aloud and written naming, word frequency and grapheme-phoneme 
regularity might lead to consistency, and the effect of imageability is perhaps less 
predictable. Therefore, it is important to keep in mind that a certain level of consistency 
between tasks, even beyond what would be predicted from mathematical chance, could 
simply be the result of the factors that affect both tasks. 
Despite these cautions about interpreting item consistency, the benefit of being able to 
judge the relationship between two deficits makes this form of analysis extremely 
worthwhile. There are numerous methodologies for calculating and interpreting 
consistency (see Howard, 1995, for a statistical procedure that attempts to negate the 
effects of some of the variables that affect word retrieval). Although it is theoretically
possible to use or devise a procedure for determining statistically significant consistency 
that takes into account frequency, visual complexity, phonological and orthographic 
complexity and so on, the nature of comparing the results of two different tests is so 
complex that it is not reasonable to consider such a method to be entirely accurate. 
Furthermore, attempting to compare five different tests with such a methodology would 
mean calculating the effects of the various factors for up to ten comparisons, each with 
different common factors with varying degrees of impact for each. For these reasons, a 
straightforward method was used to allow qualitative judgement of item consistency 
between tests. 
Simply put, the actual overlap (of correct plus incorrect responses) is compared to the 
maximum overlap and the chance overlap. The maximum overlap is the greatest that the 
overlap between two tests can be, given the difference in test scores, and is found by 
adding the number of errors of the more accurate test to the number of correct responses on 
the less accurate test. For example, if the score on oral naming is 60/80 and the score on 
written naming is 30/80, then the maximum overlap is 50 (20 errors on oral naming plus 30 
correct on written naming). In other words, the overlap between the two tests, given the 
difference in performance, cannot be higher than 50. The closer the scores are for two tests, 
the higher the maximum overlap. The chance overlap, which is derived from Cohen’s 
Kappa, is the overlap that would be predicted by chance alone, given the difference in 
scores (assuming complete independence). This figure is found by multiplying the number 
of errors on test a by the percentage of errors on test b, added to the number correct on test 
a multiplied by the percentage correct on test b. Since the figure does not attempt to 
incorporate item frequency or complexity, there is no illusion that the comparison can 
render a statistically sound comparison. Rather, it simply allows an estimate that can be 
used for all of the comparison regardless of the common pressures that would be expected. 
This allows for a more honest comparison by allowing a much greater depth of 
interpretation and debate, instead of relying on a statistical procedure that may or may not 
encompass all of the relevant factors. Calculation of overlap is explained further in 
Appendix 2. 
By considering the actual overlap as it compares with the chance overlap and maximum 
overlap, a qualitative judgement can be made about the relationship between two tasks: An 
overlap closer to chance than to the maximum suggests little or no relationship; a score 
midway between chance and the maximum suggests a possible relationship, with possible 
2:28
involvement from common pressures; finally, an overlap that is close to the maximum is a 
good indication of a relationship between the tasks, provided the maximum is reasonably 
high (if tests differ too greatly in score, the maximum overlap can be too low to allow a 
meaningful interpretation). 
Nonword reading and repetition: In addition to presenting the results of the two nonword 
tasks in terms of number of items correct, an additional calculation was performed to 
assess the level of accuracy of the individual phonemes produced. This phoneme overlap is 
a simple method of displaying a participant’s accuracy when their total score on the test is 
below normal levels. For each item, the number of phonemes in the target response is 
compared with the number of correct phonemes in the actual response. The lesser of the 
two is then divided by the greater to achieve a figure that represents the percentage of 
correct phonemes that were achieved for that item. For example, if ploon is read as ‘foon,’ 
two of the 4 target phonemes have been achieved, or a 50% overlap. The mean overlap for 
all 24 items can then be calculated. 
The number of lexicalisations is also recorded. These are items that are generated as real 
words that are similar to the target (usually within a single phoneme or grapheme of the 
target). A relatively high number of lexicalisations for a particular test suggests that the 
lexicons are being employed to process novel grapheme or phoneme sequences, rather than 
grapheme-phoneme conversion (for nonword reading) and the sublexical repetition route 
(for nonword repetition). A high number of lexicalisations also reduces the relevance of 
phoneme overlap; for example, DHT (Chapter 4) had an overlap of 31% on nonword 
reading, but it came almost entirely from his lexicalisations suggesting that nonwords were 
being read via lexical processes and not at all by grapheme-phoneme conversion. On the 
other hand, a reasonable overlap with a lower number of lexicalisations would suggest at 
least partial access to grapheme-phoneme conversion. 
The following chapter presents and discusses the results of the 15 unimpaired participants 
whose data allowed enabled effective analysis of the aphasic participants. 
2:29
2:30
3:31 
Chapter 3. Control group – results and discussion 
Before reporting and discussing each of the aphasic participants, several issues arose from 
the control group data that are worth discussing. Summary data of testing with control 
participants appear in Tables 3:1 and 3:2. Full results are reported in Appendix 3. 
BORB PALPA PPT 
Object 
decision 
Lexical 
decision 
Homophone 
decision 
Nonword 
reading 
Nonword 
repetition 
Mean 3P 2P1W 2P1S 
n 32 60 60 24 24 52 52 52 52 
Mean 25.93 58.33 55.65 22.94 21.73 50.44 50.07 50.73 50.53 
StDev 2.66 2.38 3.46 1.18 3.16 1.42 1.94 1.33 1.30 
Lowest 20 53 50 21 13 46.33 45 47 47 
Table 3:1. Summary of control results on published tests. 
Mean, standard deviation (StDev) and lowest score for each of the published tests. For PPT, Mean = 
mean of all 3 versions; 3P = 3-picture version; 2P1W = 2-picture/1-written word version; 2P1S = 2- 
picture/1-spoken word version. 
Regularity effects of unpublished tests 
Since one of the aims of the research focused on the issue of regularity effects, the first 
comparison was between regular and exception words for the unpublished tests. Although 
there is a slight discrepancy between word groups for each test, efforts to match the groups 
were fairly successful. The only meaningful difference was for written naming, 
presumably due to less predictable spelling of some words. This was a significant 
discrepancy (t(14) = 2.69, p = 0.02). Since the balance of imageability between the two 
groups was based only on the oral naming performance of the validation control group, it 
was not surprising that differences were revealed for the written naming test, for which 
regularity should, theoretically, play a much greater role. Having said that, the effect of 
regularity should only be evident in spelling errors. While there mean of spelling errors 
was higher for the exception words (0.93 to 0.53), so too was the mean of semantic errors 
(0.67 to 0.20). 
The only other significant discrepancy was for the comprehension test (t(14) = 2.26, p = 
0.04), though the actual difference was minimal (summed across the 15 participants, there 
were 4 errors on regular words and none on exception). Means and standard deviations for 
these tests, as well as the discrepancy between means of the regular and exception word 
groups, appear in Table 3:1.
3:32 
Compre-hension 
Oral 
Naming 
Reading 
Written 
Naming 
Repetition 
n 80 80 80 80 80 
Mean 79.73 79.00 79.87 77.07 79.20 
Standard Deviation 0.46 1.00 0.35 2.63 1.08 
Regular - exception -0.27 0.33 0.13 1.07 0.40 
Table 3:2. Summary of control group results on unpublished tests. 
Descriptive data for each test and for the discrepancies between regular and exception word groups on 
each test (mean of total scores for each participant on regular words minus mean for exception words). 
Oral naming versus written naming 
It was not unexpected that some aphasic participants would have greater difficulty with 
written naming than with oral. Therefore, it is important to be able to judge whether this 
discrepancy is the result of differing effects of lesions, or the effects of a single lesion 
affecting both output modalities, with the written naming score lower simply because this 
task is more difficult. Indeed, control data suggest that written naming is significantly more 
difficult, with the range of scores much lower (lowest control score 72 for written naming, 
77 for oral). Therefore, a small difference between scores might simply relate to the degree 
of difficulty on each task, and this needs to be taken into account when comparing 
participants’ scores for each task. On the other hand, a significant difference in the 
opposite direction is a strong indication that oral naming is defective, and more so than 
written naming if both are impaired. Written naming was significantly more difficult than 
oral naming for the control group t(14) = 3.08, p  0.01. Strangely, however, it was not just 
spelling errors that distinguished the two naming tasks (a mean of 1.47 compared with 0 
phonological errors on oral naming) – the mean number of semantic errors also increased 
from oral naming (0.47) to written naming (0.87). Other error types were fairly consistent 
between the two tasks. In terms of item consistency across participants, one item on written 
naming (scroll) was scored incorrectly by three participants, while 10 other items were 
incorrect for two different participants. 
Written naming was also significantly more difficult than comprehension (t(14) = 3.73, p  
0.01), reading (t(14) = 2.85, p = 0.01), and repetition (t(14) = 4.05, p  0.01), though on 
each of these tasks the mean number of errors was less than one, as can be seen in Table 
3:1.
3:33 
Methodological issues 
Several methodological issues became apparent when the results of the control group were 
analysed: 
1. Written naming: One problem with the added difficulty of written naming is a reduction 
of sensitivity for the test. Some people are simply ‘bad spellers,’ which lowers the mean 
and range of the control scores. Unfortunately, this erodes the test’s ability to detect mild 
deficits of written naming amongst aphasic participants who were premorbidly ‘good 
spellers.’ If an individual who would have premorbidly scored close to 100% on written 
naming then sustains an injury that reduces their performance to the level of unimpaired 
‘bad spellers,’ any assertion of a deficit is much less conclusive. This problem can be 
addressed to some extent with error patterns: Errors of unimpaired individuals with low 
scores should be predominantly spelling mistakes, with ‘appropriate’ misspellings (e.g. 
waspwosp). A high number of semantic errors (no control made more than 2) or very 
unusual spellings (e.g. beeeeb; kitekert) might indicate that a lesion is affecting this 
process. 
2. PPT: Two members of the control group, F6 and M6, emigrated from England about 25 
years ago. Despite performing at similar levels to the rest of the control group on most 
tests, there was an obvious advantage for F6 and M6 on PPT, achieving 100% accuracy on 
all three versions. Only one other control, F5, achieved full scores on all three version – 
predictably, F5 was also educated in England. Only one Australian educated control 
achieved a perfect score, and only on one version. The difference between controls 
educated in England and those educated in Australia was significant (t (13) = 2.47, p = 
0.03), Nevertheless, merging the scores of all 15 control participants had only a small 
impact on group scores – the mean for the 12 Australian-educated controls across all three 
versions was 50.06 (SD = 1.32) compared with 50.44 (SD = 1.42) when the three England-educated 
participants are included. 
For most of the aphasic participants, the inclusion of the England-educated controls made 
little difference. Nevertheless, the apparent cultural bias in PPT suggests that there should 
be concern as to the appropriateness of using English norms for a clinical population 
educated in Australia. Howard and Patterson (1992) report a mean score of 98-99% (less 
than one error) for the 3-picture and 3-word versions, with no participant making more 
than three errors. In contrast, three of the twelve unimpaired, Australian-educated
participants made more than three errors on at least one occasion, and the mean for the 3- 
picture version was considerably lower than that given by the authors of the test (95%, or 
about 2 errors more on average). This finding should serve as a warning to clinicians who 
use the PPT in practice that the results of the PPT, although very useful, need to be 
interpreted within the context of this possible cultural bias. On the other hand, lowering the 
cut-off score for PPT reduces its sensitivity, so it is important to balance these 
considerations. The items that were most unreliable for the Australian educated members 
of the control group are listed below. While most items were unreliable on just a few 
occasions and for only two to three participants, the most noticeable difficulty was on the 
acorn item, for which Australian-educated controls consistently chose the distractor. 
Percent 
accuracy 
3:34 
Item 
number 
Given 
item Target Distractor 
14 40 acorn pig donkey 
86 16 windmill tulip daffodil 
89 4 thimble needle cotton 
92 31 puddle cloud sun 
92 32 rocket moon star 
94 12 pyramid palm tree pine tree 
94 14 ticket bus car 
94 26 nun church house 
Table 3:3. Most frequently incorrect items on PPT for controls. 
Only items that were incorrect for at least two different control participants are included. 
3. Object decision: Perhaps the most concerning test result was for the object recognition 
task from BORB (Riddoch  Humphreys, 1993). The performance of the control group on 
this task varied so greatly that it effectively had very little capacity for detecting 
impairments, with the worst-performing control scoring just 20/32 (chance is 16/32). The 
mean of 25.93 was also noticeably lower than that of the original normative sample (M = 
27.0, SD = 2.2). Fortunately, the lack of sensitivity of this test did not matter for the 
aphasic participants, for whom the lowest score was 26/32. 
Although an English advantage might again be predicted, given that many of the animals 
represented in the test might be unfamiliar to people raised and educated in Australia, this 
was certainly not the case – three of the Australian-educated controls outperformed their 
England-educated peers. However, the clearest outcome of error analysis was the obvious 
response bias towards real objects (M = 95%, SD = 5%; for unreal objects, M = 67%, SD = 
13%). This lack of ability for unimpaired controls to reliably identify made-up pictures 
suggests possible problems with the materials. Feedback from the controls who found the
test difficult indicated that the drawings were not clear enough, and it is important to note 
that the lowest scoring control did not report any diagnosed visual problems (aside from 
the need to wear glasses). A qualitative observation was that participants appeared to 
improve as the test proceeded, suggesting that practice items or coaching might improve 
reliability, validity and sensitivity. 
4. Nonword repetition: Lastly, it was noted in Chapter 2 that several control participants 
had minor hearing difficulties which were considered age appropriate and were not 
obvious in conversation. These difficulties seemed irrelevant on all of the tasks with the 
exception of nonword repetition. As can be seen in Figure 3:1, those with minor hearing 
loss performed considerably worse on this task (for the 4 hearing impaired controls, out of 
24, M = 16.75, SD = 2.87, for the 11 unimpaired controls M = 23.55, SD = 0.69). This is in 
contrast to word repetition, on which the score out of a possible 80 differed only slightly 
(hearing impaired M = 78.25, SD = 1.26; for unimpaired M = 79.55, SD = 0.82). A two-way 
analysis of variance revealed a significant effect of hearing on the tests (F(1, 13) = 
3:35 
55.67, p  0.01). 
The inclusion of these participants was justified on the grounds that they were considered 
representative of the general population. The advantage of this decision is that allowances 
can be made for aphasic participants with mild hearing difficulties (to the extent that it 
seems relatively ‘normal’ for this age group). The disadvantage is the lack of sensitivity in 
detecting impairments of nonword repetition in participants with good hearing. Although 
the outcome of nonword repetition is therefore somewhat less transparent, the results are 
nevertheless useful. Obviously, a good score on this task (22/24 or higher) is indicative of 
intact abilities (within the confines of the basic model, which means intact auditory input, 
phoneme input and output buffers, and speech/motor output). Scores below this point were 
interpreted within the context of the basic model (which in this instance complies with 
most mainstream language models), and are examined more closely in the relevant 
chapters.
3:36 
100 
80 
60 
40 
20 
0 
Words Nonwords Words Nonwords 
Hearing impaired Normal hearing 
Percent correct 
Figure 3:1. Control group performance on repetition tasks. 
Mean scores in percent for word and nonword repetition for the 4 members of the control group with 
minor hearing loss and the 11 without.
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams
Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

1. unit 5, proofreading
1. unit 5, proofreading1. unit 5, proofreading
1. unit 5, proofreading
En Chomrong
 
National Human Rights Institutions In The EU
National Human Rights Institutions In The EUNational Human Rights Institutions In The EU
National Human Rights Institutions In The EU
Thomas Müller
 
Standards of review on civil proceedings 9th circuit 201-pages
Standards of review on civil proceedings   9th circuit   201-pagesStandards of review on civil proceedings   9th circuit   201-pages
Standards of review on civil proceedings 9th circuit 201-pages
Umesh Heendeniya
 
538352main sts134 presskit_508
538352main sts134 presskit_508538352main sts134 presskit_508
538352main sts134 presskit_508
Sérgio Sacani
 
Campaign Business Plan - Irish - Spring 2014
Campaign Business Plan - Irish - Spring 2014Campaign Business Plan - Irish - Spring 2014
Campaign Business Plan - Irish - Spring 2014
Jacquelyn Tolliver
 
New microsoft word document (2)
New microsoft word document (2)New microsoft word document (2)
New microsoft word document (2)
GeminiVuive
 
AND.©OFFICE..the advertising design and brand identity agency.©Copyright2015
AND.©OFFICE..the advertising design and brand identity agency.©Copyright2015AND.©OFFICE..the advertising design and brand identity agency.©Copyright2015
AND.©OFFICE..the advertising design and brand identity agency.©Copyright2015
Eric Nuijten
 
RVulic Project Samples
RVulic Project SamplesRVulic Project Samples
RVulic Project Samples
Rebecca Vulic
 

Was ist angesagt? (19)

SHOPO contract
SHOPO contractSHOPO contract
SHOPO contract
 
English Idioms Tests
English Idioms TestsEnglish Idioms Tests
English Idioms Tests
 
8.topic sentence
8.topic sentence8.topic sentence
8.topic sentence
 
1. unit 5, proofreading
1. unit 5, proofreading1. unit 5, proofreading
1. unit 5, proofreading
 
Emerging ed tech free_education_technology_resources_ebook
Emerging ed tech free_education_technology_resources_ebookEmerging ed tech free_education_technology_resources_ebook
Emerging ed tech free_education_technology_resources_ebook
 
National Human Rights Institutions In The EU
National Human Rights Institutions In The EUNational Human Rights Institutions In The EU
National Human Rights Institutions In The EU
 
The hottest big data startups of 2014
The hottest big data startups of 2014 The hottest big data startups of 2014
The hottest big data startups of 2014
 
6.subject verb agreement
6.subject verb agreement6.subject verb agreement
6.subject verb agreement
 
Standards of review on civil proceedings 9th circuit 201-pages
Standards of review on civil proceedings   9th circuit   201-pagesStandards of review on civil proceedings   9th circuit   201-pages
Standards of review on civil proceedings 9th circuit 201-pages
 
538352main sts134 presskit_508
538352main sts134 presskit_508538352main sts134 presskit_508
538352main sts134 presskit_508
 
Fiche
FicheFiche
Fiche
 
I am the guard
I am the guardI am the guard
I am the guard
 
Campaign Business Plan - Irish - Spring 2014
Campaign Business Plan - Irish - Spring 2014Campaign Business Plan - Irish - Spring 2014
Campaign Business Plan - Irish - Spring 2014
 
A pilot model test for 4th primary
A pilot model  test  for 4th primaryA pilot model  test  for 4th primary
A pilot model test for 4th primary
 
New microsoft word document (2)
New microsoft word document (2)New microsoft word document (2)
New microsoft word document (2)
 
Michiel Schaeffer: Observed and future climate change: Causes, consequenses...
Michiel Schaeffer: Observed and future climate change:   Causes, consequenses...Michiel Schaeffer: Observed and future climate change:   Causes, consequenses...
Michiel Schaeffer: Observed and future climate change: Causes, consequenses...
 
Why Choose Magento For Your Online Business?
Why Choose Magento For Your Online Business?Why Choose Magento For Your Online Business?
Why Choose Magento For Your Online Business?
 
AND.©OFFICE..the advertising design and brand identity agency.©Copyright2015
AND.©OFFICE..the advertising design and brand identity agency.©Copyright2015AND.©OFFICE..the advertising design and brand identity agency.©Copyright2015
AND.©OFFICE..the advertising design and brand identity agency.©Copyright2015
 
RVulic Project Samples
RVulic Project SamplesRVulic Project Samples
RVulic Project Samples
 

Ähnlich wie Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams

Native Plant Revegetation Guide For Colorado
Native Plant Revegetation Guide For ColoradoNative Plant Revegetation Guide For Colorado
Native Plant Revegetation Guide For Colorado
Fiona9864
 
User manual
User manualUser manual
User manual
lolly632
 
Manual smart ideas 5
Manual smart ideas 5Manual smart ideas 5
Manual smart ideas 5
spejo
 
cops-p317-pub_Ferguson
cops-p317-pub_Fergusoncops-p317-pub_Ferguson
cops-p317-pub_Ferguson
ralston2152003
 
Diveintogreasemonkey
DiveintogreasemonkeyDiveintogreasemonkey
Diveintogreasemonkey
guest02103b55
 

Ähnlich wie Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams (20)

Press Kit for Space Shuttle Endeavour's Final Mission, STS-134
Press Kit for Space Shuttle Endeavour's Final Mission, STS-134Press Kit for Space Shuttle Endeavour's Final Mission, STS-134
Press Kit for Space Shuttle Endeavour's Final Mission, STS-134
 
Native Plant Revegetation Guide for Colorado
Native Plant Revegetation Guide for ColoradoNative Plant Revegetation Guide for Colorado
Native Plant Revegetation Guide for Colorado
 
Native Plant Revegetation Guide For Colorado
Native Plant Revegetation Guide For ColoradoNative Plant Revegetation Guide For Colorado
Native Plant Revegetation Guide For Colorado
 
Windows XP Registry Guide
Windows XP Registry GuideWindows XP Registry Guide
Windows XP Registry Guide
 
HSK 2 Chinese Intensive Reading H21003 汉语水平考试二级考试
HSK 2 Chinese Intensive Reading H21003 汉语水平考试二级考试HSK 2 Chinese Intensive Reading H21003 汉语水平考试二级考试
HSK 2 Chinese Intensive Reading H21003 汉语水平考试二级考试
 
Elevator pitch
Elevator pitchElevator pitch
Elevator pitch
 
Design note ded mhp bayan 1
Design note ded mhp bayan 1Design note ded mhp bayan 1
Design note ded mhp bayan 1
 
TeamD_final_report
TeamD_final_reportTeamD_final_report
TeamD_final_report
 
User manual
User manualUser manual
User manual
 
An Object-Oriented Database Model Approach For The Logical Design Of A Custom...
An Object-Oriented Database Model Approach For The Logical Design Of A Custom...An Object-Oriented Database Model Approach For The Logical Design Of A Custom...
An Object-Oriented Database Model Approach For The Logical Design Of A Custom...
 
Phrasalverb
PhrasalverbPhrasalverb
Phrasalverb
 
Accounting Monthly closing check list.pdf
Accounting Monthly closing check list.pdfAccounting Monthly closing check list.pdf
Accounting Monthly closing check list.pdf
 
The glorious quran
The glorious quranThe glorious quran
The glorious quran
 
Daether Jackson Seminar slide cast
Daether Jackson Seminar slide castDaether Jackson Seminar slide cast
Daether Jackson Seminar slide cast
 
Manual smart ideas 5
Manual smart ideas 5Manual smart ideas 5
Manual smart ideas 5
 
cops-p317-pub_Ferguson
cops-p317-pub_Fergusoncops-p317-pub_Ferguson
cops-p317-pub_Ferguson
 
Tu dien tranh tau thuy
Tu dien tranh tau thuyTu dien tranh tau thuy
Tu dien tranh tau thuy
 
In The Shade Of The Qur’an Volume 14 surahs_33-39
In The Shade Of The Qur’an Volume 14 surahs_33-39In The Shade Of The Qur’an Volume 14 surahs_33-39
In The Shade Of The Qur’an Volume 14 surahs_33-39
 
Herbarium Technique.pdf
Herbarium Technique.pdfHerbarium Technique.pdf
Herbarium Technique.pdf
 
Diveintogreasemonkey
DiveintogreasemonkeyDiveintogreasemonkey
Diveintogreasemonkey
 

Patterns of Reading Impairments in Cases of Anomia - Dr Christopher Williams

  • 1. i TABLE OF CONTENTS List of Figures .............................................................................................................. iv List of Tables................................................................................................................. v Acknowledgements ...................................................................................................... vii Abstract........................................................................................................................ ix Chapter 1. General introduction..................................................................................1:1 Cognitive models of language processing ...................................................................1:1 A simple model to explore language abilities: The ‘basic model’ ................................1:2 Two lexicons or four? ............................................................................................. 1:8 Different accounts of reading aloud ........................................................................ 1:9 The relationship between reading aloud and oral picture naming........................... 1:11 Research Aims...................................................................................................... 1:14 Chapter 2. Method .....................................................................................................2:17 Participants..............................................................................................................2:17 Recruitment of aphasic participants ...................................................................... 2:17 Recruitment of unimpaired controls ...................................................................... 2:18 Materials..................................................................................................................2:19 Procedures ...............................................................................................................2:23 Scoring.....................................................................................................................2:25 Analyses ...................................................................................................................2:25 Chapter 3. Control group – results and discussion ...................................................3:31 Regularity effects of unpublished tests ......................................................................3:31 Oral naming versus written naming..........................................................................3:32 Methodological issues ..............................................................................................3:33 Chapter 4. A simple case to explain? .........................................................................4:37 Case description.......................................................................................................4:37 Results......................................................................................................................4:37 Input processes ..................................................................................................... 4:38 Reading and repetition of words and nonwords..................................................... 4:38 The semantic system............................................................................................. 4:38 Picture naming...................................................................................................... 4:39 Discussion................................................................................................................4:40
  • 2. Chapter 5. Three cases of phonological dyslexia ...................................................... 5:45 ii Case 1 – RPD........................................................................................................... 5:45 Results for RPD........................................................................................................ 5:45 Input processes..................................................................................................... 5:46 Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 5:46 The semantic system ............................................................................................ 5:47 Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 5:48 Discussion – RPD .................................................................................................... 5:49 Case 2 – DHT .......................................................................................................... 5:53 Results for DHT ....................................................................................................... 5:53 Input processes..................................................................................................... 5:53 Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 5:54 The semantic system ............................................................................................ 5:55 Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 5:56 Item consistency and comparisons........................................................................ 5:57 Discussion – DHT.................................................................................................... 5:58 Case 3 – DPC .......................................................................................................... 5:62 Results for DPC ....................................................................................................... 5:62 Input processes..................................................................................................... 5:63 Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 5:63 The semantic system ............................................................................................ 5:64 Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 5:65 Discussion – DPC.................................................................................................... 5:67 Phonological dyslexia – general discussion.............................................................. 5:70 Chapter 6. Interpreting results for a bilingual aphasic ............................................ 6:73 Case description....................................................................................................... 6:73 Control M2........................................................................................................... 6:74 Results ..................................................................................................................... 6:74 Input processes..................................................................................................... 6:74 Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 6:75 The semantic system ............................................................................................ 6:76 Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 6:77 Discussion................................................................................................................ 6:80 Chapter 7. A case of deep dyslexia ............................................................................ 7:87 Deep dyslexia........................................................................................................... 7:87 Case description....................................................................................................... 7:88 Results ..................................................................................................................... 7:89 Input processes..................................................................................................... 7:89 Reading and repetition of words and nonwords .................................................... 7:90 The semantic system ............................................................................................ 7:92 Picture naming ..................................................................................................... 7:92 Item consistency................................................................................................... 7:95 Discussion................................................................................................................ 7:96
  • 3. Chapter 8. Collective results for aphasic participants ............................................ 8:101 Collective results .................................................................................................... 8:101 Severity of aphasia and dissociations...................................................................... 8:105 Severity .............................................................................................................. 8:105 Dissociations and double dissociations................................................................ 8:106 Chapter 9. General discussion ................................................................................. 9:109 The basic model - conclusions ................................................................................ 9:109 Reading aloud..................................................................................................... 9:109 Semantic errors on oral naming........................................................................... 9:110 Comments on methodological issues....................................................................... 9:113 References.................................................................................................................... 117 Appendices................................................................................................................... 123 Appendix 1. Materials................................................................................................ 124 Appendix 2. Analyses................................................................................................. 132 Appendix 3. Control group results ............................................................................. 133 Appendix 4. Nonword reading ................................................................................... 135 Appendix 5. Error analysis for aphasic participants .................................................. 136 iii
  • 4. List of Figures Figure 1:1. The ‘basic model’ of language processing. ............................................... 1:3 Figure 1:2. The three reading routes of the basic model: ........................................... 1:5 Figure 1:3. The central components of any four-lexicon model. ................................ 1:8 Figure 1:4. The hypothesis described by Orpwood and Warrington (1995). .......... 1:12 Figure 2:1. Example Item from the comprehension test: ......................................... 2:21 Figure 3:1. Control group performance on repetition tasks. ................................... 3:36 Figure 4:1. The basic model, showing MWN’s proposed lesion site. ....................... 4:41 Figure 5:1. The basic model as it applies to RPD...................................................... 5:49 Figure 5:2. The basic model as it applies to DHT. .................................................... 5:59 Figure 5:3. The basic model as it applies to DPC...................................................... 5:68 Figure 6:1. Sample of written naming responses for JWS. ...................................... 6:79 Figure 6:2. Attempted alphabet by JWS................................................................... 6:80 Figure 6:3. The basic model as it applies to JWS...................................................... 6:82 Figure 7:1. The basic model as it applies to SJS. ...................................................... 7:97 iv
  • 5. v List of Tables Table 2:1. Descriptive data for the aphasic participants. .........................................2:18 Table 2:2. List and order of tests in each session. .....................................................2:24 Table 3:1. Summary of control results on published tests. .......................................3:31 Table 3:2. Summary of control group results on unpublished tests. ........................3:32 Table 3:3. Most frequently incorrect items on PPT for controls. .............................3:34 Table 4:1. MWN’s performance on tests of input processes.....................................4:38 Table 4:2. MWN’s performance on reading and repetition tests. ............................4:38 Table 4:3. MWN’s performance on semantic tests. ..................................................4:39 Table 4:4. MWN’s performance on the oral naming test. ........................................4:39 Table 4:5. MWN’s performance on the written naming test. ...................................4:40 Table 5:1. RPD’s performance on tests of input processes. ......................................5:46 Table 5:2. RPD’s performance on reading and repetition tests................................5:47 Table 5:3. RPD’s performance on semantic tests. .....................................................5:47 Table 5:4. RPD’s performance on the oral naming test............................................5:48 Table 5:5. RPD’s performance on the written naming test. .....................................5:48 Table 5:6. DHT’s performance on tests of input processes.......................................5:54 Table 5:7. DHT’s performance on reading and repetition tests. ..............................5:54 Table 5:8. DHT’s performance on semantic tests. ....................................................5:55 Table 5:9. DHT’s performance on the oral naming test. ..........................................5:56 Table 5:10. DHT’s performance on the written naming test. ...................................5:57 Table 5:11. Item consistency between tests of verbal output for DHT. ....................5:58 Table 5:12. DPC’s performance on tests of input processes. ....................................5:63 Table 5:13. DPC’s performance on reading and repetition tests..............................5:64 Table 5:14. DPC’s performance on semantic tests. ...................................................5:65 Table 5:15. DPC’s performance on the oral naming test. .........................................5:65 Table 5:16. DPC’s performance on the written naming test.....................................5:66 Table 5:17. Item consistency between oral naming and other tasks for DPC. .........5:67 Table 6:1.JWS’ performance on tests of input processes..........................................6:75 Table 6:2. JWS’ performance on reading and repetition tests. ................................6:76 Table 6:3. JWS’ performance on semantic tests. ......................................................6:77 Table 6:4. JWS’ results on the oral naming test........................................................6:78 Table 6:5. JWS’ performance on the written naming test. .......................................6:79 Table 7:1. SJS’ performance on tests of input processes. .........................................7:90
  • 6. Table 7:2. SJS’ performance on reading and repetition tests. ................................. 7:90 Table 7:3. Reading errors for SJS. ............................................................................ 7:91 Table 7:4. SJS’ performance on semantic tests......................................................... 7:92 Table 7:5. SJS’ performance on the oral naming test............................................... 7:93 Table 7:6. Examples of oral naming errors for SJS.................................................. 7:93 Table 7:7. SJS’ performance on the written naming test. ........................................ 7:94 Table 7:8. Examples of written naming errors for SJS. ........................................... 7:95 Table 7:9. Item consistency between comprehension and naming for SJS. ............. 7:95 Table 7:10. Item consistency between several tests for SJS...................................... 7:96 Table 8:1. Performance of aphasic participants on tests of input processes. ......... 8:101 Table 8:2. Performance of aphasic participants on reading and repetition tests. . 8:102 Table 8:3. Performance of aphasic participants on semantic tests......................... 8:103 Table 8:4. Performance of aphasic participants on the oral naming tests. ............ 8:104 Table 8:5. Performance of aphasic participants on the written naming test ......... 8:105 Table 8:6. Comparison of the regular and exception word groups........................ 8:105 vi
  • 7. Acknowledgements First and foremost, I would like to thank all of the wonderful people who participated in this project, without whom none of this would have been possible. For most of these individuals, the assessment procedure involved several hours of testing, and I am eternally grateful for the time and effort that you all devoted to the project. I must also thank two participants, TB and FME, whose results were not included in the final report but who gave there time nevertheless. Second, I would like to acknowledge the professional assistance I received from various people. In particular, my supervisors, Professor Max Coltheart and Associate Professor Lindsey Nickels, who gave their time and effort over a period of many years, and who never lost faith that I would eventually submit. For your time, advice, and understanding, I cannot thank you enough. I am also grateful to the speech pathologists as St Joseph’s Hospital and the Royal Rehabilitation Centre Sydney for their assistance in referring patients and for being extremely accommodating in providing me with their time and other resources. I am also indebted to many other academics and support staff of the Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science and the Psychology Department of Macquarie University for their professional advice and assistance with resources. Third, I would like to thank the many amazing people in my life who I am lucky enough to have as family and friends. I am especially grateful to my parents, who not only provided me with the love and support that they always have, but who also went out of their way to help me with finding control participants. To all of my friends, including student peers, team mates, work colleagues, flat mates, and long-term friends, I cannot express how grateful I am for your professional support (including assistance with proof reading, material preparation and other advice) and, more importantly, your moral support – I would not have attained this feat without your compassion, reassurance, and understanding. vii
  • 9. ix Abstract Over recent decades, research-based cognitive models of language have become increasingly sophisticated. However, with increasing sophistication has come an equivalent increase in complexity, to the extent that it is now more difficult than ever for clinicians to utilise the model for testing hypotheses about patients and devise appropriate therapeutic interventions. A series of six cases is presented to explore the capacity of the ‘basic model’ to account for various aphasic profiles, with a particular focus on hypotheses about reading pathways. To this end, a series of experiments was designed using a single set of picture-word items, with a focus on the balance between words with and without regular spelling-sound correspondence. Various theoretical positions are discussed including the lexical non-semantic route, the summation hypothesis, and the hypothesis that reading aloud and oral naming are subserved by different phonological output lexicons (e.g. Orpwood & Warrington, 1995). Most of the aphasic participants presented with ‘output’ anomia, but for some this was in the context of mild semantic deficits that may have contributed to their poor oral naming. One of the participants was also completely unable to read nonwords, yet his reading of real words, although impaired, did not contain semantic errors. This is an uncommon finding and one that is incongruent with the summation hypothesis. Other participants demonstrated intact reading of exception words despite being impaired on the oral naming task, which further supports the inclusion of the lexical non-semantic route. Another of the aphasic participants was considered in the context of being a late-acquired bilingual speaker. He was compared not only to the main control group, but also to an unimpaired, late-acquired bilingual speaker with the same language background. The basic model was unable to account for his pattern of deficits, but it was determined that most cognitive models, no matter how intricate, are inadequate to account for aphasic syndromes in bilingual speakers. The final case examines the profile of a participant with deep dyslexia. Although the basic model is able to account for this participant’s profile, consideration is given to the right-hemisphere hypothesis and to the notion that, due to wide ranging and as yet unknown variables, standard cognitive models of language processing may again be inappropriate for use with these cases.
  • 10. It was concluded that the evidence supported the potential of the basic model and the assumptions associated with it, including the lexical non-semantic route and the depiction of only two lexicons, one each for spoken and written lexical entries. Additionally, several methodological issues are discussed including poor sensitivity of several tests. x
  • 11. 1:1 Chapter 1. General introduction Anomia is usually characterised as general word-finding difficulties. It can exist as a syndrome in itself or, more often, as a feature of a more general aphasic disorder (Garman, 1990). Almost every aphasic individual experiences some degree of impaired word retrieval (e.g. Garrett, 1992; Weigel-Crump & Koenigsknecht, 1973), which is made evident by the fact that the most common finding of aphasic research is the inability to name pictures correctly (Goodglass, 1983). Analysis of the various causes of naming failure, and the myriad of other lexical deficits associated with it, can reveal a great deal about the cognitive architecture of language processing. This chapter introduces and briefly discusses a range of issues surrounding cognitive models of language processing. In the chapters that follow, some of these issues will be explored through a case series involving six individuals with various anomic syndromes and degrees of impairment. In particular, the potential for a ‘basic’ model of language processing to account for the deficits of these individuals will be examined, and it will be argued that this relatively uncomplicated model is sufficient to explain and understand acquired language deficits at a clinical level. Cognitive models of language processing In any cognitive model of lexical processing, the ability to perform normal linguistic functions is explained by an array of processing modules linked to each other by a network of pathways. These models do not aim to account for neural processing centres and connections, rather, they are attempts to explain the processes involved in normal lexical functioning, and are often constructed around hypotheses that are generated from case studies of individuals with language impairments. Such hypotheses are generally based on dissociations (i.e. when a certain process is impaired while another is intact) and, more importantly, double dissociations (i.e. when two separate processes can be differentially impaired) – for example, there are cases of impaired written naming with intact oral naming and vice versa, indicating a double dissociation between the process involved in each form of naming. Whilst there are many ways in which the various models differ, by their very nature there are many aspects that they must have in common. Specifically, all lexical models must be able to explain the different processes involved in understanding and producing language, at least at the level of single words. Therefore, all models must account for orthographic processing (the way we process written words), phonological processing (spoken words),
  • 12. recognition of 2- and 3-dimensional objects, and semantic processing (comprehension of words and objects). The full range of everyday skills encompassed by a model should include: Confrontation naming (naming of pictures and objects), both oral and written; spontaneous speech and writing; recognition and comprehension of pictures, written words and spoken words; reading aloud; written ‘copying’ and verbal repetition; and writing to dictation. Also, models must account not only for our ability to process words that are known to us, but also words that are novel or made up. A simple model to explore language abilities: The ‘basic model’ The primary objective of this report is to show that a simple cognitive model of language is sufficient to account for most aphasic individuals. Being able to precisely identify a patient’s deficit within the context of a cognitive model can have significant implications for the design of therapeutic intervention. However, due to their complex nature, the practical application of the more sophisticated research-based models are often difficult for clinicians to apply and interpret. Therefore, simplifying models to a degree that they can be easily applied to the majority of cases could have significant implications for clinical practice. Keeping in mind the language abilities of normal speakers, in addition to the most commonly reported and generally agreed upon aspects, the simplest model that could be considered for clinical application is presented in Figure 1:1 (e.g. Allport, 1984; Allport & Funnell, 1981; Jackson & Coltheart, 2001). The most peripheral, non-language features such as initial acoustic processing and motor output are omitted, and internal processing of modules is not defined. At the centre of the basic model in Figure 1:1 is the semantic system, which stores and processes conceptual information about the meanings of words and objects; it represents an intricate network of semantic features (i.e. all the characteristics of the things that an individual knows). To either side of the semantic system are the phonological lexicon and orthographic lexicon, stores of all the spoken and written words (respectively) that an individual knows. 1:2
  • 13. 1:3 Figure 1:1. The ‘basic model’ of language processing. Input to the model can be auditory or visual. Auditory information first reaches the phonological input buffer, which temporarily stores and processes phonemes (small units of sound) before forwarding the information on to the phonological lexicon for activation of the appropriate word forms, and to the phonological output buffer, where phonological information is reorganised as speech. The pathway between the phonological input and output buffers is the sublexical repetition route, and allows auditory input to be re-processed as speech output – this is the mechanism that allows us to quickly repeat verbal information (both real words and nonwords). Repetition of known words can also occur via the phonological lexicon. Information from the phonological lexicon is also forwarded to the semantic system where relevant semantic nodes are activated, enabling comprehension of spoken words. Visual input to the system can take two forms. Firstly, 2- and 3-dimensional objects are identified and processed by the object recognition system, which then activates relevant nodes in the semantic system. Naming of these objects is then made possible via the phonological lexicon and phonological output buffer (for oral naming) or the orthographic lexicon and orthographic output buffer (for written naming). Secondly, written input is
  • 14. processed initially by a stage of letter identification, which associates the almost infinite array of forms that each letter of the alphabet can take with the single letter that they represent (i.e. no matter how the letter a is written – e.g. a, a, A, or A – it is usually recognisable). According to the basic model, reading aloud is made possible by three different routes, all beginning at the stage of letter identification. The first, called the semantic route (Figure 1:2a), proceeds to the orthographic lexicon, through the semantic system, and on to the phonological lexicon and phonological output buffer. The second (Figure 1:2b) is called the lexical non-semantic route, and also proceeds to the orthographic lexicon. At this point however, information is sent directly to the phonological lexicon, bypassing the semantic system, before being forwarded on to the phonological output buffer. This pathway allows for written words to be read aloud without necessarily activating semantic representations, and is discussed in greater detail later in the chapter. The third route (Figure 1:2c) is a direct connection from letter identification to the phonological output buffer via grapheme-phoneme 1:4 conversion. This pathway, also known as the sublexical route, allows for the processing of strings of graphemes (a grapheme is a letter or group of letters that represent a single phoneme) that do not have entries in the lexicons – that is, unfamiliar words, foreign words and nonwords (i.e. plausible made-up words such as ploon and chup). a.
  • 15. 1:5 b. c. Figure 1:2. The three reading routes of the basic model: (a) the semantic route; (b) the lexical non-semantic route; and (c) grapheme-phoneme conversion. From the perspective of the basic model, reading aloud is made possible by these three pathways. Words with regular spelling (i.e. those that have predictable grapheme-phoneme correspondence and therefore sound the way they are spelled, such as dog and arm) can be read via any of the three routes. In contrast, exception words (words that do not sound the way they are spelled, such as bowl and yacht) cannot be read via grapheme-phoneme conversion – since grapheme-phoneme conversion only allows for direct translation of graphemes into phonemes, this would cause regularisation errors (e.g. bowl would be read
  • 16. as ‘bowel’ and yacht would be read as ‘yatched’ or ‘yacked’). However, exception words can be read using either the semantic or lexical non-semantic route, since all words are represented in the lexicons and simply need to be activated, first in the orthographic lexicon, then in the phonological lexicon. Finally, novel words and nonwords can only be read via grapheme-phoneme conversion, since these letter strings are not represented in the lexicons. Damage to grapheme-phoneme conversion impairs the individual’s ability to read nonwords, which will often (but not always) be read as lexicalisations (e.g. ploon might be read as ‘plume’ or ‘prune,’ while chup might be read as ‘chap’). Finally, the model needs to include components that can process novel words not only in their written form, but also via auditory input. Repetition of novel words is achieved by the sublexical repetition route, which connects the phonological input and output buffers. Written dictation of novel words is achieved via phoneme-grapheme conversion, which is responsible for converting sequences of phonemes into graphemes, thus allowing a person to write novel strings of sounds that are heard. This process is not examined in the case series, but is shown in the model because its existence is well supported by evidence in the literature (e.g. Alario, Schiller, Domoto-Reilly, & Caramazza, 2003; Miceli, Capasso, & Caramazza, 1999). Damage to the model will result in a variety of deficits, depending on which component or components are damaged, and the degree to which the components are still able to function (see Allport, 1984; Allport & Funnell, 1981; Jackson & Coltheart, 2001). In broad terms, there are two ways that lesions might affect the functioning of the core components of the language system (i.e. the semantic system, phonological lexicon and orthographic lexicon) – damage to the representations within the process, or reduced activation of those representations. Generally, damage to the representations should lead to consistency of errors. In other words, if the actual representations are damaged, then the same errors will appear repeatedly, and for all tasks that rely on that module. On the other hand, reduced activation, which is generally conceptualised as damage to the connections between modules, is less likely to result in error consistency. Damage to individual components will obviously lead to a particular set of impairments. If the semantic system is damaged, comprehension will be impaired regardless of the method of input (i.e. the individual will have difficulty understanding the meanings of pictures, spoken words and written words). However, the most distinctive feature of ‘semantic 1:6
  • 17. anomia’ is bimodal naming failure to all forms of input (Rothi, Raymer, Maher, Greenwald, & Morris, 1991). That is, an impaired ability to name words both orally and in writing, whether the stimuli are presented as pictures, written definitions or auditory definitions. Semantic errors (meaning-related errors e.g. naming a car as a truck) should be common because damage to particular semantic representations increases the likelihood of lexical entries that are related by meaning being activated in the relevant lexicon (Miceli, Amitrano, Capasso, & Caramazza, 1996). Damage to a lexicon will lead to the inability to activate representations within that lexicon. From the perspective of the basic model, this will lead to: a) reduced ability to name pictures in that modality, with a range of error types including semantic and phonological errors; b) difficulties with lexical decision (i.e. distinguishing between real and made-up words) in that modality; and c) difficulties with comprehension of words input from that modality. Other abilities might be partially affected. In particular, reading aloud of exception words should lead to regularisation errors if either lexicon is damaged, and if grapheme-phoneme conversion is intact. Likewise, writing of exception words to dictation should be affected by damage to the phonological lexicon. However, nonword reading, repetition and writing to dictation should all be possible, even if both lexicons are damaged. In contrast, reduced activation of the lexicons from the semantic system should lead to impaired picture naming of that modality, without affecting any other language skill. Errors should be similar in nature to those seen for lexicon damage, including semantic errors, but with less consistency predicted. Post-lexical damage should also have similarities to lexical damage. In particular, damage to the connection between the phonological lexicon and phonological output buffer should impact on oral naming and reading of exception words. For naming, semantic errors would not be expected since the lexical entry has already been selected. On the other hand, auditory lexical decision should still be possible, as should repetition of words (via the sublexical repetition route). Damage to the connection between the orthographic lexicon and orthographic output buffer should mirror this pattern for writing. Finally, damage to the input or output buffers should affect all input or output for that modality, while damage to the object recognition process should affect all tasks that involve some aspect of interpreting pictures or objects. 1:7
  • 18. Two lexicons or four? Perhaps the most audacious argument presented in the basic model is that only two lexicons are defined, one each for spoken and written words. In contrast, the majority of mainstream models describe separate lexicons for input and for output for each modality, as depicted in Figure 1:3 below. Only the central components are shown, with peripheral features omitted (e.g. input and output buffers, grapheme-phoneme conversion and direct links between the lexicons), as are any hypothesised feedback mechanisms and connections between the lexicons. This is because of the diverse range of configurations that the various models hypothesise. On the other hand, the central features that are pictured are common to most cognitive models of language processing (e.g. Hillis & Caramazza, 1991; Martin & Saffran, 2002; Miceli et al., 1996; Nickels, 2000; Southwood & Chatterjee, 2001). Figure 1:3. The central components of any four-lexicon model. A considerable number of debates surround this issue (refer to Howard, 1995, for an extensive discussion on the topic; also see Martin & Saffran, 2002). However, despite the general consensus of four lexicons, the objective of this report is to show that a simple model is sufficient to account for the language of people with aphasia in a clinical setting. Therefore, judgments as to whether or not aspects of particular models are fundamental should not be restricted to peripheral components and pathways; determining the relevance of core components, in particular the number of lexicons, is just as crucial. A demonstration that two lexicons are sufficient would considerably decrease the complexity of cognitive models of language. The majority of evidence that favours the position of a distinction between input and output lexicons is not based on cases for which 1:8
  • 19. representations are clearly lost in one but preserved in the other. Rather, the arguments are based primarily on findings from intricate research methodology such as ‘dual-task decrement’ (Shallice, McLeod, & Lewis, 1985) and research findings as they relate to certain theoretical assumptions (see Howard & Franklin, 1988). However, at a clinical level there is often a lack of distinction between input and output modules when identifying deficits. Therefore, for the sake of simplicity the lexicons were not divided into input and output processes, in accordance with previous advocates of this approach (e.g. Allport & Funnell, 1981; Funnell, 1983; Jackson & Coltheart, 2001) 1:9 Different accounts of reading aloud At first glance, the lexical non-semantic reading route might appear to be redundant. Indeed, it is not entirely clear what purpose it serves for normal language, and is not included in many models, such as the summation hypothesis (e.g. Hillis & Caramazza, 1991) and Plaut’s computational model (Plaut, McClelland, Seidenberg, & Patterson, 1996). However, omission of the lexical non-semantic route leads to certain predictions concerning word reading for individuals with damage to the semantic reading route. First of all, if grapheme-phoneme conversion is impaired, then reading aloud of real words should include frequent semantic intrusions or omissions (Miceli et al., 1996). Indeed, this is the profile observed for deep dyslexia. However, not all individuals with impaired semantic processing and non-functional grapheme-phoneme conversion produce semantic errors on word reading. For example, WB (Funnell, 1983) performed poorly on tests of semantic processing and was completely unable to read nonwords, to the extent that he was unable to generate a response for more than half of the items. Nevertheless, he performed reading tasks with very few semantic errors or omissions. The second prediction is that even if reading aloud is impaired (and includes semantic errors) in addition to non-functional grapheme-phoneme conversion, as is generally the case in deep dyslexia, then this function should be just as severely compromised as oral naming, since the absence of grapheme-phoneme conversion should lead to a complete reliance on the semantic reading route. However, reading aloud is consistently reported to be superior to oral naming provided orthographic input is intact, even for individuals with deep dyslexia. This phenomenon is “only consistent with the partial operation of (the lexical non-semantic route)” (Howard, 1985, p403).
  • 20. A third prediction is that if grapheme-phoneme conversion is intact or at least partially active, then regularisation errors should occur on reading of exception words. In other words, surface dyslexia should be evident (Patterson, Marshall, & Coltheart, 1985). However, Weekes and Robinson (1997) report BP, whose performance on semantic tasks such as word-picture and picture-picture matching was impaired. Furthermore, he was able to name barely more than half of the picture items in the Snodgrass and Vanderwart corpus, and nearly half of his errors were semantic errors. His nonword reading was also impaired, though he successfully read approximately half of the items on a nonword reading task. Nevertheless, on a set of 40 exception words, BP made only one error (reading thumb as thump, most likely a visual error). This is considered by Weekes as strong evidence that BP is reading via a lexical pathway that does not involve semantic processing. This prediction also applies to post-semantic naming impairments – MRF was considered to have an intact semantic system but was impaired on oral naming, with partially active grapheme-phoneme conversion, yet there was no effect of regularity observed (Orpwood & Warrington, 1995). Although the predictions made by most cognitive models of language processing with only two reading pathways are not supported by the literature, the summation hypothesis (e.g. Hillis & Caramazza, 1991; Hillis, Rapp, & Caramazza, 1999; Miceli et al., 1996; Miceli, Capasso, & Caramazza, 1994; Miceli, Giustolisi, & Caramazza, 1991), considers reading aloud of real words to be achieved by the ‘summation’ of lexical and sublexical processes. If the semantic reading route is only partially operational but the sublexical process is also providing full or partial activation, reading of words, both regular and irregular, is still possible. Partial semantic activation means that semantically appropriate representations in the phonological output lexicon, including the target, are partially activated (e.g. the word yacht will activate representations such as boat, mast, sail, and of course yacht). At the same time, the sublexical process activates all phonologically appropriate representations in the phonological lexicon (so yacht might activate representations for words such as yet, yurt and, again, yacht). Therefore, the only node in the phonological output lexicon that will be activated above threshold is the target word, yacht. All other representations that are activated will fail to reach threshold. However, an important assumption of the summation hypothesis is that a complete lack of input from the sublexical process, in conjunction with a lesion at some stage of the semantic reading route, should lead to frequent semantic errors in word reading (i.e. deep 1:10
  • 21. dyslexia). On the other hand, partial activation from the sublexical process should all but eliminate semantic errors (and reduce total errors), as seen in phonological dyslexia. As mentioned, however, evidence from the phonological dyslexic WB (Funnell, 1983) suggests that this distinction between phonological and deep dyslexia does not always hold true. This debate is examined further in the case of DHT, who is presented in Chapter 5. The relationship between reading aloud and oral picture naming While there are many different interpretations of how word reading can be achieved through lexical, sublexical and lexical non-semantic processes, language researchers agree almost universally that the phonological process of reading words aloud overlaps with the phonological process of oral picture naming. However, a challenge to this principle was the suggestion that reading and oral naming have distinct phonological stores that can each be selectively damaged. The first clear presentation of this hypothesis appeared in a 1995 article by Orpwood and Warrington. They described MRF, an individual with poor oral naming of pictures and poor naming to definition, with frequent semantic errors. As demonstrated by his poor nonword reading, MRF had only partial access to grapheme-phoneme conversion. MRF was able to read real words, with no difference between regular and exception items, and his repetition of nonwords was intact; therefore he must have had a lesion affecting the grapheme-phoneme conversion process. Comprehension was also intact, suggesting a lesion of the phonological output lexicon. However, from the perspective of most serial models, this should also impair word reading. As can be seen in Figure 1:4, the authors propose that the lesion affecting oral naming is located at a phonological output lexicon that is unique for oral naming tasks (lesion a). Grapheme-phoneme conversion is also impaired (lesion b), but all other processes are intact, including an additional phonological output store for word reading; the presence of semantic errors in oral naming, and complete absence of them in reading, is considered further justification for their position. The authors reject the summation hypothesis as a plausible account on the basis that his grapheme-phoneme conversion is too severely impaired to adequately contribute to reading. Support for this hypothesis was provided by an apparent double dissociation between reading and oral naming. BF was described by Goldblum (1985), and was remarkable in that he was described as having intact oral naming yet impaired word reading (despite intact comprehension for words). According to Breen and Warrington (1995), BF contrasts with the many individuals reported for whom reading is intact while oral naming is 1:11
  • 22. impaired, thus representing a double dissociation between these two abilities. In the context of most mainstream models, there is no way to account for this phenomenon. The solution, according to the authors, is independent stores for each task. Extending this hypothesis, the authors conducted a series of priming experiments with participant NOR. They found that priming by first reading the word had very little effect on NOR’s oral naming unless the delay was extremely short. Although they concede that very little is known about the specific effects of priming at the level of the phonological lexicon, they consider this finding to represent a possible dissociation between the phonological processes involved in reading aloud and oral naming. Figure 1:4. The hypothesis described by Orpwood and Warrington (1995). Green boxes and arrows indicate intact processing; red boxes and arrows represent the hypothesised lesions. Only relevant processes are shown. The most significant feature of the hypothesis of independent phonological stores is the claim of a double dissociation between oral naming and reading aloud. However, this position is challenged by Lambon Ralph, Cippoloti and Patterson (1999), who argue that BF’s naming was not necessarily superior to oral reading, as purported by Goldblum (1985). Three reason are given for this challenge: First, BF’s profile represented a complex pattern of various dyslexic syndromes, rather than a single syndrome that could be accounted for by an isolated lesion of output phonology; second, and most significantly, 1:12
  • 23. reading and oral naming were not compared for the same set of words; finally, Goldblum considered BF’s naming to be less impaired than reading partly because the majority of his errors were almost always corrected – Lambon Ralph and colleagues (1999) argue that this is far from a clear demonstration of normal functioning. The claim that the summation hypothesis is unable to account for NOR and MRF is also disputed by Lambon Ralph and colleagues. Rather, the presence of semantic errors in naming but not in reading can be attributed to direct input from sublexical processes because only minimal orthographic information is needed to block semantic errors. For their participant MOS, who also performed poorly on oral naming and well on reading aloud, they suggest that the phonological output lexicon itself is preserved, as is the semantic system. Instead, it is the connection between these systems that is severed, with reading aided by the sublexical process. In accordance with the summation hypothesis, Lambon Ralph and colleagues (1999) go further by suggesting that the reason why oral naming is frequently found to be impaired in the context of intact reading is that oral naming is simply more vulnerable. There are two factors that contribute to this vulnerability. First, there is no direct correspondence between conceptual knowledge about an object and the phonological representation of that object’s name, while reading is largely aided by the ‘quasi-regular’ mapping between orthography and phonology. Second, only one source of phonological activation is available to oral naming, while reading has at least two. In support of this claim is evidence that oral naming in anomic participants can be improved by an additional source of phonological activation such as phonemic cueing, making it as robust as reading aloud with its two sources of phonological activation (Lambon Ralph, 1998; Lambon Ralph et al., 1999). In a third article aimed at supporting the notion of multiple phonological output stores, Crutch and Warrington (2001) present VYG, whose spontaneous speech was intact, but whose oral naming and reading aloud were both impaired. Oral naming responses consisted mostly of circumlocutions, with few phonological errors, while reading errors were all phonological. Since VYG was able to comprehend words that he was unable to read aloud, the authors concluded that the site of damage must be at the level of a phonological output store, or perhaps access to the output store from semantics. Despite the fact that VYG’s naming was more severely impaired than his reading, the authors claim that damage to the ‘stronger’ reading process should affect naming in the same way 1:13
  • 24. – the high number of phonological errors in reading, and almost complete absence of them in naming, is therefore considered evidence for a double dissociation between the tasks. However, there are several flaws in the logic of the articles discussed above. Firstly, if reading aloud and oral naming are enabled by separate phonological stores, then the double dissociation between them should not be restricted to differences in error patterns. There should be individuals reported in the literature for whom reading is worse than oral naming for the same items, a phenomenon which has not yet been described. Secondly, Crutch and Warrington (2001) claim that VYG’s comprehension of words that he is unable to read aloud indicates that his semantic system is unaffected. They fail to observe the principle that receptive tasks such as word-picture matching place considerably less strain on the semantic system than do expressive tasks (e.g. Howard, 1985; Laine, Kujala, Niemi, & Uusipaikka, 1992; Lambon Ralph, Sage, & Roberts, 2000). If VYG does have a mild semantic deficit, this could have a noticeable impact on oral naming, including generation of semantic errors, with less of an impact on reading, which is assisted by partially intact grapheme-phoneme conversion – thus leading to more phonological errors. Thirdly, many authors argue that discrepancies of error types should actually be expected for the same reason that reading aloud is considered to be less vulnerable to impairment than oral naming (e.g. Newcombe & Marshall, 1980; Southwood & Chatterjee, 2000, 2001). If additional phonological input constrains the responses, then more phonological errors, and less semantic/circumlocutory errors should be evident. Research Aims The general aim of this report is to demonstrate that the basic model of language processing, as described in this chapter, could be a useful clinical tool to aid the understanding of aphasic patients. To this end, the following predictions were made: 1) The basic model will be sufficient to account for each individual’s profile, or at least as capable as any existing model. 2) Two lexicons, one each for phonological and orthographic representations, are sufficient to explain the majority of aphasic participants. 3) The lexical non-semantic route is an essential component of serial models. Therefore: a) Participants with significantly impaired oral naming (that is not caused by pre-semantic 1:14 damage) but with intact reading are best accounted for by the existence of this pathway.
  • 25. b) If grapheme-phoneme conversion is completely abolished for an individual with damage to the semantic reading route, deep dyslexia will only result if the lexical non-semantic route is also damaged. c) Reading impairments exhibited by anomic participants will conform to models of language retrieval that assume a shared phonological process for reading aloud and for oral naming (i.e. Orpwood and Warrington’s (1995) hypothesis of distinct phonological stores will not be supported). The critical motivation for this study was the paucity of literature in which a single set of stimuli is used for a variety of language tasks. By developing a range of tests with a single set of words-picture items, aphasic participants could be assessed in such a way that intact and defective functions could be determined with much greater confidence than if a variety of different tests had been used, thus providing insight into what aspects of cognitive architecture are required to account for the participants. Furthermore, by carefully balancing the group of items so that half would have word names with regular spelling and the other half irregular, it was expected that a great deal more might be revealed about the process of reading aloud. The next chapter describes the processes involved in material preparation, recruitment of suitable participants, assessment procedures and analysis of results. 1:15
  • 26. 1:16
  • 27. 2:17 Chapter 2. Method As was described in the previous section, one aim of this project was to assess the validity of claims made initially by Orpwood and Warrington (1995) that reading aloud and oral naming are subserved by distinct phonological stores. This chapter describes the recruitment of participants and the tests used, including the development of the five unpublished tests that were designed to investigate the Orpwood and Warrington (1995) hypothesis. It also describes the procedures that were followed for administration, scoring and analysis of the battery of tests. As will be made clear, the lack of evidence for or against this hypothesis did little to diminish the value of the results. Participants Recruitment of aphasic participants Aphasic participants were recruited with the assistance of Speech Pathologists at the Royal Rehabilitation Centre Sydney and St Joseph’s Hospital, through the Macquarie University Psychology Clinic, and researchers at the Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science. The criteria for recruitment were adults with aphasia sustained at least 6 months prior to the assessment, who presented primarily with anomia, without excessive interference from complicating factors such as impaired hearing or vision, global cognitive dysfunction or prominent motor-speech deficits, including dysarthria or verbal dyspraxia. Individuals with mild complicating deficits were still requested with the understanding that they would be excluded if necessary, though none were excluded on this basis. Individuals were also excluded if they were identified as having recent psychiatric risk factors such as suicidal ideation, depression or heightened anxiety. A total of 12 potential participants were recruited. Of these, 7 were considered appropriate based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria. One was excluded due to a near-ceiling performance on most tests, two had recovered to the point that they were speaking fluently in conversational speech, and two individuals who showed interest were excluded on the basis of psychiatric conditions as it was considered unethical to risk placing them into a potentially stressful situation. One participant, FME, was described in a separate report in relation to her diagnosis of herpes simplex encephalitis, and is not discussed any further in this dissertation. Descriptive data for the six remaining participants appear in Table 2:1 below. Each participant is described in detail in the following chapters.
  • 28. Chapter Partic-ipant 2:18 Age Education Sex Description of injury/illness Months Since Injury Acute deficits (immediately post-onset) Relevant Medical History Vision/ Glasses 4 MWN 76 10 F LMCA ischaemic with minor cortical atrophy 8 Broca's aphasia; dysarthria; mild verbal dyspraxia; mild right arm weakness AMI 1990; mitral valve repair; TIA; hypercholesterole mia Bifocals 5 RPD 65 10 M LMCA infarct 29 Unknown Right meningioma and debulking surgery; CABG; high cholesterol Glasses (short and reading) 5 DPC 51 11 F LMCA haemorrhagic 56 Confusion; aphasia Type II DM; migraines; anxiety disorder Reading 5 DHT 62 9 M LMCA cerebral embolic infarct 35 Right hemiplegia; non-fluent aphasia, agrammatism Infective endocarditis; CABG Bifocals 6 JWS 69 9 M LMCA ischaemic 24 Right hemiparesis; hemisensory loss; global aphasia Unknown Reading 7 SJS 43 10 M LMCA haemorrhagic with bifurcation aneurism 83 Severe frontal headache; vomiting; global aphasia/dysphonia Hypertension Glasses (short) Table 2:1. Descriptive data for the aphasic participants. Education = total years of formal education; LMCA = left middle cerebral artery; CABG = coronary artery bypass graft; AMI = acute myocardial infarction; TIA = transient ischaemic attack; DM = diabetes mellitus. Recruitment of unimpaired controls Unimpaired controls were recruited through personal contacts, and were seen in two groups. The first group took part in the validation stage, and consisted of 10 age appropriate controls (M = 59.63, SD = 4.35) with appropriate anticipated years of education (M = 11.7, SD = 2.63). These participants were selected on the basis of expected age and education levels of the ABI participants, who had not yet been identified. For the second control group, 16 unimpaired participants were initially recruited, of which two had also been involved in the validation stage. One participant, M2, emigrated from the Netherlands at the age of 21. Because English is his second language, he was excluded from the main control group. However, his data are presented in Chapter 6 as a comparison for JWS, an aphasic participant with a similar background.
  • 29. The remaining 15 individuals, 8 females and 7 males, were included in the main group. Independent t-tests revealed no significant difference between the seven original aphasics and the control group for either age (aphasics M = 61.00, SD = 12.08; controls M = 60.20, SD = 6.35; t(19) = 0.20, p = 0.84) or years of formal education (aphasics M = 9.83, SD = 0.75; controls M = 10.07, SD = 1.16; t(19) = 0.45, p = 0.66). Many of the control participants wore glasses, and several had mild visual impairments (e.g. cataracts) though testing did not reveal any obvious visual difficulties (i.e. they did not perform any worse than other controls on tests that might be sensitive to visual impairment). Also, four members of the main control group (three males, one female) reported mild hearing difficulties, which were not identified until nonword repetition was attempted. The justification for including these individuals is that such mild hearing loss and visual difficulties are clearly common in this population, and difficult to identify. Therefore, similar difficulties cannot be eliminated as a cause of poor performance for some of the aphasic participants; the effect of mild hearing loss on repetition tasks is discussed in Chapter 3. Two participants emigrated from England about 25 years ago, and the results of these individuals are also examined more closely in Chapter 3. 2:19 Materials One of the key predictions made by the Orpwood and Warrington (1995) hypothesis is that if oral naming is impaired, and the cause of this impairment can be localised to the phonological output lexicon, then words with regular spelling should be less affected on a reading task than words with irregular spelling, assuming that grapheme-phoneme conversion is still involved. Determining the effects of regularity on reading performance is also tantamount to hypotheses relating to the lexical non-semantic route. Therefore, the primary objective when preparing the materials was to focus on this contrast between regular and exception words by gathering two word lists that differed only in this respect. That is, the word items needed to be matched on criteria such as frequency and linguistic complexity. In order to further limit potential differences in linguistic complexity, only monosyllabic words were chosen. Since the items also needed to be named, only words that could be easily elicited by their pictures were appropriate, which considerably limited the number of appropriate items. For example, a picture of a yacht will just as often be named as a boat; pictures of a buoy and a raft proved to be difficult to identify for many people.
  • 30. After an extensive period of item selection and refinement, including informal testing and discussion with peers, 104 items were selected from the list of monosyllabic words in the CELEX lexical database (Baayen, Piepenbrock, & Van Rijn, 1993). The pictures were obtained primarily from Hemera Photo Objects (Hemera, 1997-2000), with gaps filled by non-copyright pictures obtained from the internet. Alterations were made where necessary to exclude distracting aspects of the images or to highlight the relevant part of the picture. The regular and exception word sets were matched for spoken and written frequency (Baayen et al., 1993), number of phonemes, number of letters, the number of plural words (only one item in each set (shorts/blinds) was a plural word), and whether the item was animate or inanimate. Since many nouns also act as verbs (e.g. axe, bowl, or comb), which can have a considerable impact on frequency effects, only items that were deemed to be used most often as nouns were selected. Comparisons were analysed using t-test and Fisher’s exact calculations, with the results presented in Appendix 1. Following the validation phase of the research (see the Procedures section that follows) the final word/picture set included 40 items with regular spelling and 40 exception items, with classification determined by the set of grapheme to phoneme correspondence rules listed by Rastle and Coltheart (1999). These 80 items were used for four simple tests of language ability: Oral naming, written naming, reading aloud and repetition. As it was anticipated that some participants might have considerable difficulties with written picture naming and that they would be unable to complete the test, the first 20 items on this test were also matched as per the criteria listed above. Again, comparisons were by way of t-tests and Fisher’s exact, with the results appearing in Appendix 1. Presentation order of items in each test was pseudorandom – items were selected at random but relocated to ensure that no more than three consecutive items were related by regularity, semantic field or phonological similarity. Additionally, a word-picture matching task was designed to determine whether or not participants had intact access to the semantic representations of the test items from the written word. A multiple-choice format was used. For each item, the target word appeared in the middle, with four pictures around the word. The pictures were equated in size as much as possible, but often needed to be slightly different to remain size appropriate (e.g. a picture of a cat needs to be larger than a picture of a mouse). An example item from the 2:20
  • 31. 2:21 comprehension test appears in Figure 2:1. For each written word item, the pictures included: a) The target picture; b) A semantic distractor – the regular and exception word groups were matched for degree of semantic relatedness between the distractor and the target based on figures sourced from Maki, McKinley and Thompson (2004) as well as the type of semantic relationship (each pair was broadly classified as either related by association, such as bowl and spoon, or simply being members of the same category, such as an axe and a saw); c) A phonological/orthographic distractor – the two groups were matched for degree of phonological relatedness; and d) An unrelated distractor. bowl Figure 2:1. Example Item from the comprehension test: The given word item (bowl), the target picture, the semantic distractor (spoon), the phonological distractor (bell), and the unrelated distractor (tricycle). Most pictures appeared more than once throughout the test, though none appeared more than three times in total (including once as the target, for many of the pictures). The full list of items for the comprehension test appears in Appendix 1, along with relatedness figures and classifications, and statistical calculations. Other tests: Aphasic participants were also assessed on several published tests in order to assess the integrity of other aspects of the lexical system. The following tests were administered: • Tests from the Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA, Kay, Lesser, & Coltheart, 1992):
  • 32. 2:22 o Visual lexical decision (subtest 25) – spelling-sound regularity (distinguishing real words (regular and exception) from nonwords (pseudohomophones and non-homophonic nonwords)). This test was used to assess the integrity of the orthographic lexicon and input to it. Chance is 50% on this test. o Homophone decision (subtest 28) – judging whether or not pairs of words (with regular and irregular spelling) or nonwords sound the same. This test relies on the integrity of multiple components of lexical processing, including the orthographic lexicon, phonological lexicon, grapheme-phoneme conversion and the phonological output buffer. The error pattern of this task, in particular the contrast between real word and nonword pairs, is often more important than the total score. Chance is 50% for this test also. o Nonword reading and repetition (subtest 36) – grapheme-phoneme conversion and the sublexical repetition route can potentially play an important role in processing of words, particularly when other abilities are impaired. Therefore, assessment of nonword reading and repetition was vital. To enable relevant comparisons, it was also crucial that the nonword items be comparable to items used for the unpublished tests (i.e. the 80 regular and exception words discussed previously). Indeed, two-tailed independent t-tests revealed no significant difference between the 80 test items and the 24 nonwords used in PALPA for either number of letters (for real words M = 4.30, SD = 0.79; for nonwords M = 4.50, SD = 1.14, t (102) = 0.98, p = 0.33) or number of phonemes (for real words M = 3.30, SD = 0.80; for nonwords M = 3.42, SD = 0.72, t (102) = 0.64, p = 0.52). o Cross-case matching (subtest 19) and, for participants who made errors on this test, mirror reversal (subtest 18). These tests were intended to eliminate an impairment of letter identification as the cause of a participant’s difficulties with processing written words. • Pyramids and Palm Trees test (PPT, Howard & Patterson, 1992) – this test requires the participant to match the stimulus item (picture, written word or spoken word) to an associated item from a choice of two semantically related pictures. Three versions were utilised in order to assess the integrity of the semantic system and input to it: o 3 pictures version – poor performance relative to the other versions might suggest reduced input from object recognition.
  • 33. o 2 pictures + 1 written word version – relatively poor performance suggests 2:23 reduced input to the semantic system from the orthographic lexicon. o 2 pictures + 1 spoken word version – relatively poor performance suggests reduced input to the semantic system from the phonological lexicon. Equal difficulty with all three versions is indicative of damage to representations within the semantic system. • From the Birmingham Object Recognition Battery (BORB, Riddoch & Humphreys, 1993): o Subtest A (hard). This subtest is comprised of 32 black and white drawings of which half are real and half are made up from two different objects (e.g. the body of a cow with the head of a horse). This tests the integrity of the object recognition process. Procedures Validation phase: The original 104 pictures were shown to the validation group of controls on the screen of a 17” laptop computer using Microsoft PowerPoint. In cases where the target was provided in conjunction with an appropriate non-target word (e.g. ‘crow, bird’ for the desired target of crow), the target was considered to have been achieved (on testing, aphasic participants and members of the second control group were prompted to provide another response if they answered with an appropriate non-target word). Likewise, if the target response was included as part of a larger, similarly appropriate response (e.g. steak ‘t-bone steak’; plane ‘aeroplane’), the item was considered appropriate for inclusion, and hence correct if produced by the aphasic participants and members of the second control group. Items were only included if the target word was achieved by nine out of ten controls in the validation group, and the two word groups (regular/exception words) were matched for the number of participants who named each word correctly (mean number correct out of 10 for the regular group was 9.85 (SD = 0.33) and for the exception word group 9.75 (SD = 0.44), t(78) = 0.42; p = 0.68). Experimental phase: The items for four tests were shown to all participants on a 17” laptop screen using Microsoft PowerPoint – the items for the repetition task were read by the examiner. For picture naming (oral and written) and reading, five seconds was allowed for the response, with the timing controlled by the computer (a further 5 seconds was allowed if the participant was prompted to provide a different response, as described for the validation study). For written naming, the time limit only applied to the commencement of
  • 34. writing a name to allow for any motor difficulties (i.e. extra time was allowed for slow writing, within reason). For repetition, the 80 items were read to the participant, with 5 seconds allowed for each response. Ten seconds was allowed for each item on the comprehension test. A five second gap (a blank screen) separated each item on all tests except for repetition, for which one to two seconds separated each response from the following item. Participants were permitted to move through the computerised tests faster by pressing an appropriate key on the keyboard. The assessments with all participants were conducted over four sessions, with each session a week apart (or within 2 days). The tests administered in each session are listed in Table 2:2 below. The unpublished tests were spread out over the sessions to reduce the effects of priming. The exception was the last session, during which written naming was followed soon after by repetition; it was considered too impractical and burdensome on the participants to extend testing beyond four sessions. Controls were assessed on all tests except for cross-case matching, on which unimpaired individuals are assumed to be 100% accurate. 2:24 Session 1 Interview Comprehension test PPT (3 pictures) Session 2 Oral naming test PPT (2 pictures, 1 written word) Visual lexical decision – regularity (PALPA: 25) Object decision (BORB: Subtest A – Hard) Homophone decision (PALPA: 28) Nonword reading (PALPA: 36) Session 3 Reading test PPT (2 pictures, 1 spoken word) Nonword repetition (PALPA: 36) Cross-case matching (PALPA: 19) Session 4 Written naming test Repetition test Table 2:2. List and order of tests in each session. Italics indicate unpublished tests. The structure of testing was not varied between participants; all aphasic and unimpaired participants completed the tests in the same order. This was to ensure consistency with, and therefore enable accurate interpretation of, practice effects and priming.
  • 35. 2:25 Scoring For the unpublished tests, clarification of certain error types is needed: • Phonological error was scored when at least half of the target phonemes were produced in the correct position. • Spelling error was scored for written naming if at least half of the target letters were produced in the correct position (e.g. chefchark). • Mixed errors were considered unrelated unless there was an obvious connection with the target item (e.g. bone dag (presumably dog) in written naming was considered a semantic error). • Errors that were self-corrected within the time limit were considered correct without further consideration. • Morphological errors were primarily inflectional errors (mostly addition or deletion of the plural –s). • Based on the responses of controls, plural variation in picture naming was considered acceptable for two items, blind/s, for which both variants are common, and gate/s (which was generally named as the singular, but since the picture was of a two-part gate this could not be considered an error). Also, the pronunciation of vase varied (either pronounced /vaz/ or /veIs/). • No response errors included items for which some effort was made but nothing meaningful (i.e. only one phoneme or letter) was generated. Finally, although errors on the comprehension test appear fairly straightforward, there is at least two ways that the actual error types could reflect problems such as reduced visual acuity or scanning. First, the phonological distractors more often than not had names that were visually similar to the target (e.g. ball/bell; nose/hose) – therefore, many phonological errors could actually be visual or orthographic errors. Second, many of the semantic distractors were not only visually similar to the target, but in some cases were actually more prominent (especially when the distractor picture, but not the target, had the background removed) – therefore, some semantic errors could actually reflect failure to adequately scan all components of the item, which might account for the rare control errors. Analyses Measures of impairment: To ascertain whether or not an aphasic participant performed significantly worse than the control group, the Bayesian methodology of Crawford and
  • 36. Garthwaite (2007) was employed (using the software for simple difference, cited in the same article). This was the primary calculation used for determining whether or not a participant had performed significantly worse than the control group on a particular task. Because the regular and exception groups were so well matched in terms of control performance, Fisher’s exact test (an unstardardised method of comparing independent groups) was used to determine differences, rather than Crawford and Garthwaite’s standardised calculation, which was influenced by ceiling effects. For certain participants, the discrepancy between two unpublished tests was measured with McNemar’s Test, with the obvious caveat that the tests differ slightly in their levels of difficulty, meaning a certain level of subjective interpretation was unavoidable. The Crawford and Garthwaite method (2007) proved to be inappropriate for judging these discrepancies and dissociations due to the differing influences of ceiling effects on the different tests. Item consistency: An important consideration for error analysis is item consistency, or the comparison between two tasks for a particular set of items. Since language based entries are conceptualised as representations stored within the semantic system and each of the lexicons, damage to particular representations should lead to errors on the relevant items regardless of the task, assuming that the same processing module is necessary for each of the tasks being compared. For example, damage to representations in the semantic system might lead to item consistency for oral naming, written naming and word-picture matching for particular items, but not necessarily for repetition or reading; damage to representations in the orthographic lexicon might lead to consistency for reading, word-picture matching and written naming, but not repetition or oral naming. On the other hand, a lesion that causes reduced activation of a processing module, rather than damage to the representations in the module, would not be expected to result in such consistency. Therefore, item consistency can, in certain conditions, provide an indication of the extent to which two deficits might be related by a single lesion. However, there are several aspects of item consistency that warrant caution when interpreting the results. First of all, not all tasks have the same ‘degree of difficulty’ – even for unimpaired individuals, written naming is usually performed less well than oral naming, at least for English in which written naming entails not only naming the picture, but also retrieving details about complex spelling rules and a large number of memorised 2:26
  • 37. word spellings that do not abide by rules or even a consistent exception to the rule (for example, it would not be unusual for some unimpaired individuals to be unable to spell words such as yacht). Furthermore, impaired participants could easily have multiple lesion sites affecting particular abilities, yet it is still relevant to investigate the possibility that one of the lesions is at least partially responsible for two or more of the deficits. Therefore, calculation of item consistency between different tasks should include an element of maximum consistency or ‘maximum overlap,’ which is discussed shortly. The second caution relating to item consistency is that a certain level of similarity is often expected between two tasks even if the difficulties on the tasks are not the result of a single lesion. This argument relates most prominently to the relationship between oral and written naming, and arose from observations that certain participants with post-semantic naming impairments would demonstrate statistical consistency between the two tasks, suggesting to many that there could be an additional process after the semantic system but before the lexicons (e.g. Levelt et al., 1991; Raymer et al., 1997; Raymer, Maher, Foundas, Rothi, Heilman, 2000). However, several authors have questioned the need for this additional process in accounting for item consistency. For example, Miceli and colleagues (1991) consider a certain level of consistency to simply represent deficits resulting from co-occurring 2:27 lesions affected by the same linguistic factors such as word frequency, imageability and linguistic complexity. That is, for any particular set of words, it is likely that the least frequent and most complex words will be the most vulnerable. This can lead to consistency between any tasks that happen to share the same common pressures. Furthermore, the particular common pressures are different for different pairs of tasks. For example, for oral and written naming, word frequency and imageability are likely to play a role, while for reading aloud and written naming, word frequency and grapheme-phoneme regularity might lead to consistency, and the effect of imageability is perhaps less predictable. Therefore, it is important to keep in mind that a certain level of consistency between tasks, even beyond what would be predicted from mathematical chance, could simply be the result of the factors that affect both tasks. Despite these cautions about interpreting item consistency, the benefit of being able to judge the relationship between two deficits makes this form of analysis extremely worthwhile. There are numerous methodologies for calculating and interpreting consistency (see Howard, 1995, for a statistical procedure that attempts to negate the effects of some of the variables that affect word retrieval). Although it is theoretically
  • 38. possible to use or devise a procedure for determining statistically significant consistency that takes into account frequency, visual complexity, phonological and orthographic complexity and so on, the nature of comparing the results of two different tests is so complex that it is not reasonable to consider such a method to be entirely accurate. Furthermore, attempting to compare five different tests with such a methodology would mean calculating the effects of the various factors for up to ten comparisons, each with different common factors with varying degrees of impact for each. For these reasons, a straightforward method was used to allow qualitative judgement of item consistency between tests. Simply put, the actual overlap (of correct plus incorrect responses) is compared to the maximum overlap and the chance overlap. The maximum overlap is the greatest that the overlap between two tests can be, given the difference in test scores, and is found by adding the number of errors of the more accurate test to the number of correct responses on the less accurate test. For example, if the score on oral naming is 60/80 and the score on written naming is 30/80, then the maximum overlap is 50 (20 errors on oral naming plus 30 correct on written naming). In other words, the overlap between the two tests, given the difference in performance, cannot be higher than 50. The closer the scores are for two tests, the higher the maximum overlap. The chance overlap, which is derived from Cohen’s Kappa, is the overlap that would be predicted by chance alone, given the difference in scores (assuming complete independence). This figure is found by multiplying the number of errors on test a by the percentage of errors on test b, added to the number correct on test a multiplied by the percentage correct on test b. Since the figure does not attempt to incorporate item frequency or complexity, there is no illusion that the comparison can render a statistically sound comparison. Rather, it simply allows an estimate that can be used for all of the comparison regardless of the common pressures that would be expected. This allows for a more honest comparison by allowing a much greater depth of interpretation and debate, instead of relying on a statistical procedure that may or may not encompass all of the relevant factors. Calculation of overlap is explained further in Appendix 2. By considering the actual overlap as it compares with the chance overlap and maximum overlap, a qualitative judgement can be made about the relationship between two tasks: An overlap closer to chance than to the maximum suggests little or no relationship; a score midway between chance and the maximum suggests a possible relationship, with possible 2:28
  • 39. involvement from common pressures; finally, an overlap that is close to the maximum is a good indication of a relationship between the tasks, provided the maximum is reasonably high (if tests differ too greatly in score, the maximum overlap can be too low to allow a meaningful interpretation). Nonword reading and repetition: In addition to presenting the results of the two nonword tasks in terms of number of items correct, an additional calculation was performed to assess the level of accuracy of the individual phonemes produced. This phoneme overlap is a simple method of displaying a participant’s accuracy when their total score on the test is below normal levels. For each item, the number of phonemes in the target response is compared with the number of correct phonemes in the actual response. The lesser of the two is then divided by the greater to achieve a figure that represents the percentage of correct phonemes that were achieved for that item. For example, if ploon is read as ‘foon,’ two of the 4 target phonemes have been achieved, or a 50% overlap. The mean overlap for all 24 items can then be calculated. The number of lexicalisations is also recorded. These are items that are generated as real words that are similar to the target (usually within a single phoneme or grapheme of the target). A relatively high number of lexicalisations for a particular test suggests that the lexicons are being employed to process novel grapheme or phoneme sequences, rather than grapheme-phoneme conversion (for nonword reading) and the sublexical repetition route (for nonword repetition). A high number of lexicalisations also reduces the relevance of phoneme overlap; for example, DHT (Chapter 4) had an overlap of 31% on nonword reading, but it came almost entirely from his lexicalisations suggesting that nonwords were being read via lexical processes and not at all by grapheme-phoneme conversion. On the other hand, a reasonable overlap with a lower number of lexicalisations would suggest at least partial access to grapheme-phoneme conversion. The following chapter presents and discusses the results of the 15 unimpaired participants whose data allowed enabled effective analysis of the aphasic participants. 2:29
  • 40. 2:30
  • 41. 3:31 Chapter 3. Control group – results and discussion Before reporting and discussing each of the aphasic participants, several issues arose from the control group data that are worth discussing. Summary data of testing with control participants appear in Tables 3:1 and 3:2. Full results are reported in Appendix 3. BORB PALPA PPT Object decision Lexical decision Homophone decision Nonword reading Nonword repetition Mean 3P 2P1W 2P1S n 32 60 60 24 24 52 52 52 52 Mean 25.93 58.33 55.65 22.94 21.73 50.44 50.07 50.73 50.53 StDev 2.66 2.38 3.46 1.18 3.16 1.42 1.94 1.33 1.30 Lowest 20 53 50 21 13 46.33 45 47 47 Table 3:1. Summary of control results on published tests. Mean, standard deviation (StDev) and lowest score for each of the published tests. For PPT, Mean = mean of all 3 versions; 3P = 3-picture version; 2P1W = 2-picture/1-written word version; 2P1S = 2- picture/1-spoken word version. Regularity effects of unpublished tests Since one of the aims of the research focused on the issue of regularity effects, the first comparison was between regular and exception words for the unpublished tests. Although there is a slight discrepancy between word groups for each test, efforts to match the groups were fairly successful. The only meaningful difference was for written naming, presumably due to less predictable spelling of some words. This was a significant discrepancy (t(14) = 2.69, p = 0.02). Since the balance of imageability between the two groups was based only on the oral naming performance of the validation control group, it was not surprising that differences were revealed for the written naming test, for which regularity should, theoretically, play a much greater role. Having said that, the effect of regularity should only be evident in spelling errors. While there mean of spelling errors was higher for the exception words (0.93 to 0.53), so too was the mean of semantic errors (0.67 to 0.20). The only other significant discrepancy was for the comprehension test (t(14) = 2.26, p = 0.04), though the actual difference was minimal (summed across the 15 participants, there were 4 errors on regular words and none on exception). Means and standard deviations for these tests, as well as the discrepancy between means of the regular and exception word groups, appear in Table 3:1.
  • 42. 3:32 Compre-hension Oral Naming Reading Written Naming Repetition n 80 80 80 80 80 Mean 79.73 79.00 79.87 77.07 79.20 Standard Deviation 0.46 1.00 0.35 2.63 1.08 Regular - exception -0.27 0.33 0.13 1.07 0.40 Table 3:2. Summary of control group results on unpublished tests. Descriptive data for each test and for the discrepancies between regular and exception word groups on each test (mean of total scores for each participant on regular words minus mean for exception words). Oral naming versus written naming It was not unexpected that some aphasic participants would have greater difficulty with written naming than with oral. Therefore, it is important to be able to judge whether this discrepancy is the result of differing effects of lesions, or the effects of a single lesion affecting both output modalities, with the written naming score lower simply because this task is more difficult. Indeed, control data suggest that written naming is significantly more difficult, with the range of scores much lower (lowest control score 72 for written naming, 77 for oral). Therefore, a small difference between scores might simply relate to the degree of difficulty on each task, and this needs to be taken into account when comparing participants’ scores for each task. On the other hand, a significant difference in the opposite direction is a strong indication that oral naming is defective, and more so than written naming if both are impaired. Written naming was significantly more difficult than oral naming for the control group t(14) = 3.08, p 0.01. Strangely, however, it was not just spelling errors that distinguished the two naming tasks (a mean of 1.47 compared with 0 phonological errors on oral naming) – the mean number of semantic errors also increased from oral naming (0.47) to written naming (0.87). Other error types were fairly consistent between the two tasks. In terms of item consistency across participants, one item on written naming (scroll) was scored incorrectly by three participants, while 10 other items were incorrect for two different participants. Written naming was also significantly more difficult than comprehension (t(14) = 3.73, p 0.01), reading (t(14) = 2.85, p = 0.01), and repetition (t(14) = 4.05, p 0.01), though on each of these tasks the mean number of errors was less than one, as can be seen in Table 3:1.
  • 43. 3:33 Methodological issues Several methodological issues became apparent when the results of the control group were analysed: 1. Written naming: One problem with the added difficulty of written naming is a reduction of sensitivity for the test. Some people are simply ‘bad spellers,’ which lowers the mean and range of the control scores. Unfortunately, this erodes the test’s ability to detect mild deficits of written naming amongst aphasic participants who were premorbidly ‘good spellers.’ If an individual who would have premorbidly scored close to 100% on written naming then sustains an injury that reduces their performance to the level of unimpaired ‘bad spellers,’ any assertion of a deficit is much less conclusive. This problem can be addressed to some extent with error patterns: Errors of unimpaired individuals with low scores should be predominantly spelling mistakes, with ‘appropriate’ misspellings (e.g. waspwosp). A high number of semantic errors (no control made more than 2) or very unusual spellings (e.g. beeeeb; kitekert) might indicate that a lesion is affecting this process. 2. PPT: Two members of the control group, F6 and M6, emigrated from England about 25 years ago. Despite performing at similar levels to the rest of the control group on most tests, there was an obvious advantage for F6 and M6 on PPT, achieving 100% accuracy on all three versions. Only one other control, F5, achieved full scores on all three version – predictably, F5 was also educated in England. Only one Australian educated control achieved a perfect score, and only on one version. The difference between controls educated in England and those educated in Australia was significant (t (13) = 2.47, p = 0.03), Nevertheless, merging the scores of all 15 control participants had only a small impact on group scores – the mean for the 12 Australian-educated controls across all three versions was 50.06 (SD = 1.32) compared with 50.44 (SD = 1.42) when the three England-educated participants are included. For most of the aphasic participants, the inclusion of the England-educated controls made little difference. Nevertheless, the apparent cultural bias in PPT suggests that there should be concern as to the appropriateness of using English norms for a clinical population educated in Australia. Howard and Patterson (1992) report a mean score of 98-99% (less than one error) for the 3-picture and 3-word versions, with no participant making more than three errors. In contrast, three of the twelve unimpaired, Australian-educated
  • 44. participants made more than three errors on at least one occasion, and the mean for the 3- picture version was considerably lower than that given by the authors of the test (95%, or about 2 errors more on average). This finding should serve as a warning to clinicians who use the PPT in practice that the results of the PPT, although very useful, need to be interpreted within the context of this possible cultural bias. On the other hand, lowering the cut-off score for PPT reduces its sensitivity, so it is important to balance these considerations. The items that were most unreliable for the Australian educated members of the control group are listed below. While most items were unreliable on just a few occasions and for only two to three participants, the most noticeable difficulty was on the acorn item, for which Australian-educated controls consistently chose the distractor. Percent accuracy 3:34 Item number Given item Target Distractor 14 40 acorn pig donkey 86 16 windmill tulip daffodil 89 4 thimble needle cotton 92 31 puddle cloud sun 92 32 rocket moon star 94 12 pyramid palm tree pine tree 94 14 ticket bus car 94 26 nun church house Table 3:3. Most frequently incorrect items on PPT for controls. Only items that were incorrect for at least two different control participants are included. 3. Object decision: Perhaps the most concerning test result was for the object recognition task from BORB (Riddoch Humphreys, 1993). The performance of the control group on this task varied so greatly that it effectively had very little capacity for detecting impairments, with the worst-performing control scoring just 20/32 (chance is 16/32). The mean of 25.93 was also noticeably lower than that of the original normative sample (M = 27.0, SD = 2.2). Fortunately, the lack of sensitivity of this test did not matter for the aphasic participants, for whom the lowest score was 26/32. Although an English advantage might again be predicted, given that many of the animals represented in the test might be unfamiliar to people raised and educated in Australia, this was certainly not the case – three of the Australian-educated controls outperformed their England-educated peers. However, the clearest outcome of error analysis was the obvious response bias towards real objects (M = 95%, SD = 5%; for unreal objects, M = 67%, SD = 13%). This lack of ability for unimpaired controls to reliably identify made-up pictures suggests possible problems with the materials. Feedback from the controls who found the
  • 45. test difficult indicated that the drawings were not clear enough, and it is important to note that the lowest scoring control did not report any diagnosed visual problems (aside from the need to wear glasses). A qualitative observation was that participants appeared to improve as the test proceeded, suggesting that practice items or coaching might improve reliability, validity and sensitivity. 4. Nonword repetition: Lastly, it was noted in Chapter 2 that several control participants had minor hearing difficulties which were considered age appropriate and were not obvious in conversation. These difficulties seemed irrelevant on all of the tasks with the exception of nonword repetition. As can be seen in Figure 3:1, those with minor hearing loss performed considerably worse on this task (for the 4 hearing impaired controls, out of 24, M = 16.75, SD = 2.87, for the 11 unimpaired controls M = 23.55, SD = 0.69). This is in contrast to word repetition, on which the score out of a possible 80 differed only slightly (hearing impaired M = 78.25, SD = 1.26; for unimpaired M = 79.55, SD = 0.82). A two-way analysis of variance revealed a significant effect of hearing on the tests (F(1, 13) = 3:35 55.67, p 0.01). The inclusion of these participants was justified on the grounds that they were considered representative of the general population. The advantage of this decision is that allowances can be made for aphasic participants with mild hearing difficulties (to the extent that it seems relatively ‘normal’ for this age group). The disadvantage is the lack of sensitivity in detecting impairments of nonword repetition in participants with good hearing. Although the outcome of nonword repetition is therefore somewhat less transparent, the results are nevertheless useful. Obviously, a good score on this task (22/24 or higher) is indicative of intact abilities (within the confines of the basic model, which means intact auditory input, phoneme input and output buffers, and speech/motor output). Scores below this point were interpreted within the context of the basic model (which in this instance complies with most mainstream language models), and are examined more closely in the relevant chapters.
  • 46. 3:36 100 80 60 40 20 0 Words Nonwords Words Nonwords Hearing impaired Normal hearing Percent correct Figure 3:1. Control group performance on repetition tasks. Mean scores in percent for word and nonword repetition for the 4 members of the control group with minor hearing loss and the 11 without.