SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 5
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
Social Learning in Humans and Nonhuman Animals:
Theoretical and Empirical Dissections
Mark Nielsen
University of Queensland
Francys Subiaul
The George Washington University
Andrew Whiten
University of St. Andrews
Bennett Galef
McMaster University
Thomas Zentall
University of Kentucky
The past decade has seen a resurgent, concerted interest in social
learning research comparing human and nonhuman animals. Such
research has involved not only examination of the basic mecha-
nisms and processes of social learning but also determinations of
which species and age classes engage in these various sorts of
social learning. It has considered the costs and benefits of social
learning and the circumstances that favor reliance on social rather
than asocial learning. As a result of such investigations, the study
of social learning has become one of the most important and
intensely researched fields of comparative psychology. This re-
naissance, fostered by multidisciplinary collaborations, has re-
sulted in significant methodological and conceptual innovations.
In this special issue, we present a synthesis of work that con-
solidates what is currently known and provides a platform for
future research. Consequently, we include both new empirical
studies and novel theoretical proposals describing work with both
human children and adults and a range of nonhuman animals. In
this introduction, we describe the background of this special issue
and provide a context for each of the eight articles it contains. We
hope such introduction will not only help the reader synthesize the
interdisciplinary views that characterize this broad field, but also
stimulate development of new methods, concepts, and data.
The survival of any animal, human or nonhuman, depends on its
acquiring a range of skills, among others how to extract food from
defended sources, how or where to hide to avoid predators (or be
successful as one), and how to increase access to mates. Some of
these skills might be genetically canalized and some might be
acquired through individual, trial-and-error learning. However,
successful development of many such skills would not occur in the
absence of interaction with knowledgeable others. Consequently,
the study of social learning has become a major focus of research
in those sciences interested in the behavior of both humans and
nonhumans, and the effort devoted to such research has increased
greatly in recent years.
Figure 1 provides a measure of that increase, presenting the
number of peer-reviewed papers published since 1982 and listed in
PsycINFO that include social learning or imitation as keywords.
For comparison, we have also provided similar data for publica-
tions listing as a keyword theory of mind (another active area of
contemporary research in social cognition). Of note here are the
distinct trajectories taken by theory of mind compared with social
learning and imitation. Whereas since 1981 papers with theory of
mind as a keyword have increased in frequency almost linearly,
those listing social learning, imitation, or both dip toward the end
of 2001 and then climb rapidly (it is worth noting that of the 166
papers that include both social learning and imitation as keywords,
103—or 62%—were published since 2001).
There are many possible reasons for this climb since 2001 in the
number of papers with social learning and imitation as keywords.
One is the attention drawn in the social psychology literature to the
role that copying plays in smoothing interactions and increasing
liking between social partners (e.g., Chartrand & Bargh, 1999,
with current citations exceeding 600). Another is investigation of
the neural underpinnings of imitative behavior, including the pos-
sible implication of mirror neurons (e.g., Iacoboni et al., 1999,
with current citations exceeding 950) and the linking of these to
deficits associated with autism (Williams, Whiten, Suddendorf, &
Perrett, 2001, with current citations exceeding 250). Yet another
plausible explanation lies in the growing body of evidence that
humans are not the only animals for whom cultural transmission
plays a significant role in the development of behavioral reper-
toires (Whiten et al., 1999, with current citations exceeding 650).
A rising tide of scientific studies of such cultural phenomena in
both human and nonhuman species has focused attention on the
evolution of culture, its development and transmission from gen-
eration to generation by processes of social learning.
The link between the macro perspective on culture (an inher-
ently population-level phenomenon) and the micro perspective
focused on underlying social learning mechanisms was explicit in
the pairing of a 2010 Royal Society Discussion Meeting—Culture
Evolves (http://www.cultureevolves.org)—with a satellite meet-
ing—Social Learning in Humans and Nonhuman Animals: Theo-
Mark Nielsen, ●●●, University of Queensland, ●●●; Francys Subiaul,
●●●, The George Washington University; Andrew Whiten, ●●●, Univer-
sity of St. Andrews, ●●●; Bennett Galef, ●●●, McMaster University, ●●●;
Thomas Zentall, ●●●, University of Kentucky.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Francys
Subiaul, ●●●. E-mail: subiaul@gwu.edu
Journal of Comparative Psychology © 2012 American Psychological Association
2012, Vol. ●●, No. ●, 000–000 0735-7036/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027758
1
AQ: 1
AQ: 3
F1
tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124
retical and Empirical Dissections—held at the Kavli Royal Society
International Centre at Chichley Hall that followed immediately
after. The satellite meeting brought together an international com-
munity of scientists active in developing taxonomies, theories, and
conceptual schemes for the dissection of different kinds of social
learning, and empirical studies aimed at implementing such dis-
sections. The presentations provided frameworks for synthesizing
the interdisciplinary views that characterize the broad field of
social learning while stimulating the development of new methods,
concepts, and data. It is from these presentations that the articles
included in this special issue have emerged. Papers presented at the
Culture Evolves Discussion Meeting have already been published
in a special issue of Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Society B (Volume 366, whole issue 1567, pp. 935–1187; see
Whiten, Hinde, Laland, & Stringer, 2011).
Commensurate with the increasing focus on issues relating to
social learning evident in Figure 1 has been an effort by contem-
porary authors to identify and define the multiple mechanisms that
underpin such learning. Consider the oft-cited example of Imo, the
then-1.5-year-old female Japanese macaque who discovered that a
sand-covered sweet potato could be washed by taking it into a
nearby stream, thereby making it palatable. The practice subse-
quently spread throughout Imo’s troop along clearly defined social
networks. At one time, this was suggested to be “probably the
single most impressive case of imitation on record” (Premack,
1984, p. 17). The same behavior if reported today would be
assigned to one of numerous alternative categories of social learn-
ing, including various forms of imitation (action, motor, contex-
tual, procedural, automatic, vocal, cognitive, and program level),
as well as goal and result emulation, affordance learning, object
movement reenactment, observational conditioning, or contagion
(Byrne, 2002; Byrne & Russon, 1998; Call & Carpenter, 2002;
Galef, 1988; Heyes, 1994, 2011; Hoppitt & Laland, 2008; Subiaul,
2007, 2010; Want & Harris, 2002; Whiten & Ham, 1992; Whiten,
Horner, Litchfield, & Marshall-Pescini, 2004; Zentall, 2001, 2006,
2011). As a result, for many, the ascription of imitation to the
impressive case of sweet potato washing is no longer justified
(Galef, 2004; Kendal, Kendal, & Laland, 2007). At the heart of a
number of the articles we present are the application and devel-
opment of these taxonomies of social learning and the associated
dissection of empirical cases.
Here, Zentall’s (2012) concise yet comprehensive review of the
main branches of this social learning literature emphasizes that any
attempt to understand social learning will be incomplete unless it
both ranges across the animal kingdom (e.g., the data on avian
imitation is compelling, rendering as suspect any claims that the
capacity for imitation belongs to humans only) and strives to
distinguish a host of alternative mechanisms of social learning that
might underlie specific cases. Zentall’s review draws attention to
the fact that despite the increased attention focused on social
learning research in the past few decades, we remain a long way
from understanding the underlying cognitive and neural processes
that enable animals to match their behavior to that of a demon-
strator.
Burkart, Kupferberg, Glasauer, and van Schaik (2012) present
empirical work suggesting that even when engaged in basic forms
of social learning such as local enhancement, marmoset monkeys
(Callithrix jacchus) use the perceived intentions of a model to
guide their behavior. Adapting Woodward’s (1998) highly influ-
ential habituation–dishabituation paradigm, Burkart and col-
leagues presented monkeys with an agent who repeatedly ap-
proached and interacted with one of two objects. The marmosets
perceived the agent’s behavior as goal-directed whether the agent
was a conspecific or a robot, but not if it was a black box. Social
facilitation and enhancement learning were strongest when the
agent was a conspecific and weakest when the agent was a black
box. It has been suggested that social learning guided by intention
understanding is necessary for cumulative culture in humans, but
the existence of sensitivity to intention in marmosets indicates that
such sensitivity is not sufficient for cumulative culture to emerge.
Delineating processes of social learning is a prominent feature
of the articles by Zentall and by Burkart and colleagues, an
endeavor with a long pedigree in the comparative and nonhuman
animal literature. In contrast, as Want and Harris (2002) lament,
research with humans has tended to lag in assimilating the taxon-
omies outlined previously as standard practice. Even now, many
Figure 1. Number of peer-reviewed publications listed in PsycINFO since 1982 that have only social learning,
only imitation, both social learning and imitation, and theory of mind as keywords.
2 NIELSEN, SUBIAUL, WHITEN, GALEF, AND ZENTALL
tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124
papers continue to be published that use imitation as a blanket term
for any form of copying. Here, Huang’s (2012) contribution rep-
resents a clear exception to this rule. Huang presents three novel
experiments identifying the conditions under which infants engage
in emulation learning. In these experiments, infants are presented
with varying degrees of information about the outcomes (or end
states) associated with a series of novel objects. Huang’s study
shows that emulation (learning from the results of others’ actions)
is relatively efficient when (a) the target items involve outcomes
that are self-contained and are produced with a single-step action
but not when the items comprise two separate parts that can be
combined into a novel end configuration; (b) that seeing the items
in both the starting and end states facilitates emulation more than
seeing the items in their end states alone, as does seeing an adult
model in static postures consistent with the target actions; and (c)
that infants’ encoding of bodily cues from static body postures
may be susceptible to the concurrence of the spatiotemporal causal
relations of objects. Huang’s article makes an important contribu-
tion in showing how young children can produce specific object-
directed outcomes in the absence of a full demonstration.
Given that very young children appear capable of identifying
how to bring about object-related outcomes by observing aspects
of their beginning and end states, one could expect children to be
increasingly unconstrained by the specifics of what is modeled
before them. Yet, the results of an increasing number of recent
studies suggest that children may replicate even those actions of a
model that have no apparent purpose and even after the causal
irrelevance of those actions has been made explicit (e.g., Kenward,
Karlsson, & Persson, 2011; Lyons, Damrosch, Lin, Macris, &
Keil, 2011; Nielsen & Blank, 2011; Nielsen & Tomaselli, 2010).
This phenomenon, known as overimitation, features in varying
degrees in many of the articles included in this special issue, and
it is a core focus of the contribution of McGuigan (2012).
McGuigan shows that, unlike young children (Flynn, 2008; Flynn
& Whiten, 2008; McGuigan & Graham, 2010), adults will over-
imitate to the extent of transmitting irrelevant actions down diffu-
sion chains of several individuals, simulating cultural transmis-
sion. More important, however, McGuigan demonstrates that such
overimitation is influenced by the transparency of the actions; the
more obvious the irrelevance of the actions, the less likely they are
to be passed on. For McGuigan, overimitation springs from a
conformity bias, a human propensity for acquiring culture that
becomes increasingly strong with age (Whiten, in press).
Caldwell, Schillinger, Evans, and Hopper (2012), building on
previous research with adults, show that full demonstration by a
live model is not necessary for overimitation or the transmission of
information with high fidelity. In their study, participants were
shown either a photograph or live model of two different spaghetti
towers (model towers built of uncooked spaghetti with modeling
clay junctions). Participants were then instructed not to copy the
demonstrated model but to build the tallest tower possible. But
copying is exactly what they did; rather than attempt to build the
tallest tower, participants simply replicated the demonstrated
model. Thus, despite not seeing a full demonstration (i.e., observ-
ing a model build a tower) and relying only on a model of a
completed tower (i.e., end state), participants reproduced models
with high fidelity. For Caldwell and colleagues, these results cast
doubt on the hypothesis that imitation is necessary for the high-
fidelity transmission of information.
These results lead us back to the link between imitation and
culture that runs through this special issue. The contribution by
Nielsen (2012) is explicitly focused on this link. He notes (as do
others in our special issue) how the high-fidelity copying charac-
teristic of overimitation facilitates rapid transmission of informa-
tion while simultaneously enabling nonfunctional traditions to be
similarly passed on. However, he raises questions regarding the
capacity of overimitation to support innovation and speculates that
something else may be needed to drive the vast technological
advances characteristic of the Homo lineage. The answer, he
suggests, may be found in the pretend play children engage in—a
possibility that presupposes the existence of childhood in the first
place, which Nielsen argues may be a relatively recently evolved
trait of our species. What Nielsen does not dispute is that the
evolution of overimitation as a copying strategy is likely to have
been pivotal in the evolution of human cumulative culture. But
why might it have emerged in the first place? The answer might lie
in a species-specific increase in the use of copying to satisfy social
motivations.
Directly copying others can be a particularly powerful form of
learning that permits the rapid acquisition of new skills. However,
humans also copy for socially oriented reasons: We will imitate to
communicate, to be like others, to be liked by others, and to show
others that we are alike (Dijksterhuis, 2005; Nadel, Gue´rini, Peze´,
& Rivet, 1999). Here, Over and Carpenter (2012) argue that these
social factors have been largely ignored in many accounts of
imitation. Their article fully articulates how social motivations
function to drive imitation across varying circumstances and ex-
perimental arrangements, and using this approach, they explain
apparent paradoxes in the literature (notably that children will both
overimitate and selectively imitate). For Over and Carpenter, chil-
dren’s choices about what to copy are influenced by three primary
factors: (a) the relationship between personally acquired and so-
cially acquired information, (b) their identification with the model
and the social group in general, and (c) the social pressures they
experience to imitate in particular ways. For Over and Carpenter,
failing to acknowledge the strong pull in humans to do as others do
will result in an incomplete picture of imitation that, they con-
clude, is a profoundly social process.
This highly social view of imitation provides an interesting
complement to the argument provided by Heyes (2012), who raises
questions as to whether there is anything inherently social about
social learning and asks whether the constituent mechanisms of
social learning are distinct in any way from those supporting
asocial learning. Heyes’s answer is that there are no fundamental
differences; rather, both social and asocial learning depend on a
common set of associative learning processes that operate in
similar ways across animal species. What, then, of arguments that
humans are uniquely imitative (e.g., Meltzoff, 1988)? How does
this fit with the view outlined by Over and Carpenter? For Heyes,
the answer is relatively straightforward. What makes human learn-
ing different is that the effects of social living have affected the
input mechanisms for learning but not the actual learning mecha-
nisms per se. In humans, doing as others do is so strong because
from early in infancy such copying behavior is highly rewarded.
The nature of the input sets us apart from other animals, not the
cognitive processes we use in acquiring behavior.
This special issue includes a wide range of perspectives on
social learning incorporating new empirical work on human chil-
3SOCIAL LEARNING IN HUMANS AND NONHUMAN ANIMALS
tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124
dren (Huang), human adults (McGuigan; Caldwell et al.), and
monkeys (Burkart et al.) together with contrasting and comple-
mentary theoretical views and reviews that outline what is unique
and shared among phylogenetically distant species (Heyes; Over &
Carpenter; Nielsen; Zentall). This reflects the unprecedented in-
terest in social learning that has emerged in the past decade, and
the questions raised by the contributors to this issue will help
ensure that this interest continues to grow. We have greatly deep-
ened our understanding of social learning, but as Zentall reminds
us, we still have a lot to learn.
References
Burkart, J., Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & van Schaik, C. (2012). Even
simple forms of social learning rely on intention attribution in marmoset
monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●,
●●●–●●●.
Byrne, R. W. (2002). Imitation of novel complex actions: What does the
evidence from animals mean? Advances in the Study of Behavior, 31,
77–105. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(02)80006-7
Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierar-
chical approach. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 21, 667–684. doi:
10.1017/S0140525X98001745
Caldwell, C. A., Schillinger, K., Evans, C. L., & Hopper, L. M. (2012). End
state copying by humans (Homo sapiens): Implications for a compara-
tive perspective on cumulative culture. Journal of Comparative Psychol-
ogy, ●●●, ●●●–●●●.
Call, J., & Carpenter, M. (2002). Three sources of information in social
learning. In K. Dautenhahn & C. L. Nehaniv (Eds.), Imitation in animals
and artifacts (pp. 211–228). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The
perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893
Dijksterhuis, A. (2005). Why we are social animals: The high road to
imitation as social glue. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on
imitation: From neuroscience to social science (Vol. 2: Imitation, hu-
man development, and culture, pp. 207–220). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Flynn, E. (2008). Investigating children as cultural magnets: Do young
children transmit redundant information along diffusion chains? Philo-
sophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 363, 3541–
3551. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0136
Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2008). Cultural transmission of tool use in young
children: A diffusion chain study. Social Development, 17, 699–718.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00453.x
Galef, B. G. (1988). Imitation in animals: History, definition and interpre-
tation of data from the psychological laboratory. In T. R. Zentall & B. G.
Galef (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives
(pp. 3–28). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Galef, B. G. (2004). Approaches to the study of traditional behaviors of
free-living animals. Learning & Behavior, 32, 53–61. doi:10.3758/
BF03196006
Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and mecha-
nisms. Biological Reviews, 69, 207–231. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X
.1994.tb01506.x
Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation [Review]. Psychological Bulletin,
137, 463–483. doi:10.1037/a0022288
Heyes, C. (2012). What’s social about social learning? Journal of Com-
parative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●.
Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2008). Social processes influencing learning
in animals: A review of the evidence. Advances in the Study of Behavior,
38, 105–165. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)00003-X
Huang, C. (2012). Outcome-based observational learning in human infants.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●.
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., &
Giacomo, R. (1999, December 24). Cortical mechanisms of human
imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528. doi:10.1126/science.286.5449.2526
Kendal, J. R., Kendal, R. L., & Laland, K. N. (2007). Quantifying and
modelling social learning processes in monkey populations. Interna-
tional Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 7, 123–138.
Kenward, B., Karlsson, M., & Persson, J. (2011). Over-imitation is better
explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proceed-
ings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 1239–1246.
doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1399
Lyons, D. E., Damrosch, D. H., Lin, J. K., Macris, D. M., & Keil, F. C.
(2011). The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of
artefact culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366,
1158–1167. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0335
McGuigan, N. (2012). The role of transmission biases in the cultural
diffusion of irrelevant actions. Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●,
●●●–●●●.
McGuigan, N., & Graham, M. (2010). Cultural transmission of irrelevant
tool actions in diffusion chains of 3- and 5-year-old children. European
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 561–577. doi:10.1080/
17405620902858125
Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). The human infant as “homo imitans.” In T. R.
Zentall & B. G. J. Galef (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and
biological perspectives (pp. 319–341). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nadel, J., Gue´rini, C., Peze´, A., & Rivet, C. (1999). The evolving nature of
imitation as a format for communication. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth
(Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 209–234). Cambridge, England: Cam-
bridge University Press.
Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play and childhood: Essential ele-
ments in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative
Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●.
Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children: When who
gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental
Psychology, 47, 1050–1053. doi:10.1037/a0023866
Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Over-imitation in Kalahari Bushman
children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological
Science, 21, 729–736. doi:10.1177/0956797610368808
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning:
Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●.
Premack, D. (1984). Pedagogy and aesthetics as sources of culture. In
M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive neuroscience (pp. 15–35).
New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Subiaul, F. (2007). The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its
features, distribution and evolution. Journal of Anthropological Sci-
ences, 85, 35–62.
Subiaul, F. (2010). Dissecting the imitation faculty: The multiple imitation
mechanisms (MIM) hypothesis. Behavioural Processes, 83, 222–234.
doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2009.12.002
Want, S. C., & Harris, P. L. (2002). How do children ape? Applying
concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental
study of “imitation” in human children. Developmental Science, 5, 1–14.
doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00194
Whiten, A. (in press). Social cognition: Making us smart, or sometimes
making us dumb? Overimitation, conformity, non-conformity and the
transmission of culture in ape and child. In M. Banaji & S. Gelman
(Eds.), The development of social cognition. Oxford, England: Oxford
University Press.
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V.,
Sugiyama, Y., . . . Boesch, C. (1999, June 17). Cultures in chimpanzees.
Nature, 399, 682–685. doi:10.1038/21415
Whiten, A., & Ham, R. (1992). On the nature and evolution of imitation in
the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. In P. J. B.
Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, G. Beer, & M. Milinski (Eds.), Advances in the
4 NIELSEN, SUBIAUL, WHITEN, GALEF, AND ZENTALL
AQ: 2
tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124
study of behaviour (Vol. 21, pp. 239–283). New York, NY: Academic
Press.
Whiten, A., Hinde, R. A., Laland, K. N., & Stringer, C. B. (2011). Culture
evolves. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 938–
948. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0372
Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. A., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004).
How do apes ape? Learning & Behavior, 32, 36–52. doi:10.3758/
BF03196005
Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001).
Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral
Reviews, 25, 287–295. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00014-8
Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an
actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
Zentall, T. R. (2001). Imitation in animals: Evidence, function and mech-
anisms. Cybernetics and Systems, 32, 53–96. doi:10.1080/
019697201300001812
Zentall, T. R. (2006). Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms.
Animal Cognition, 9, 335–353. doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0039-2
Zentall, T. R. (2011). Social learning mechanisms: Implications for a
cognitive theory of imitation. Interaction Studies: Social Behaviour and
Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems, 12, 233–261. doi:
10.1075/is.12.2.03zen
Zentall, T. R. (2012). Perspectives on observational learning in animals.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●.
Received February 9, 2012
Revision received February 9, 2012
Accepted February 10, 2012 Ⅲ
5SOCIAL LEARNING IN HUMANS AND NONHUMAN ANIMALS
tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

Racism and imperialism in the child development discourse (chapter) (2010)
Racism and imperialism in the child development discourse (chapter) (2010)Racism and imperialism in the child development discourse (chapter) (2010)
Racism and imperialism in the child development discourse (chapter) (2010)sadafsh
 
Marres & lezaun materials and devices of the public
Marres & lezaun   materials and devices of the publicMarres & lezaun   materials and devices of the public
Marres & lezaun materials and devices of the publicJorge Pacheco
 
Passion and Love (P/L) with Knowledge and Understanding (K/U): Coherence, Con...
Passion and Love (P/L) with Knowledge and Understanding (K/U): Coherence, Con...Passion and Love (P/L) with Knowledge and Understanding (K/U): Coherence, Con...
Passion and Love (P/L) with Knowledge and Understanding (K/U): Coherence, Con...SyedVAhamed
 
Class 9 Cbse Science Syllabus 2011-12
Class 9 Cbse Science Syllabus 2011-12Class 9 Cbse Science Syllabus 2011-12
Class 9 Cbse Science Syllabus 2011-12Sunaina Rawat
 
Cbse Class 9 Science Syllabus 2012-13
Cbse Class 9 Science Syllabus 2012-13Cbse Class 9 Science Syllabus 2012-13
Cbse Class 9 Science Syllabus 2012-13Sunaina Rawat
 
Research as situated cultural practice
Research as situated cultural practiceResearch as situated cultural practice
Research as situated cultural practiceAlfredo Artiles
 
Schoolandinstruction Aretheyrevolutionarilyimprovable 091201141313 Phpapp01
Schoolandinstruction Aretheyrevolutionarilyimprovable 091201141313 Phpapp01Schoolandinstruction Aretheyrevolutionarilyimprovable 091201141313 Phpapp01
Schoolandinstruction Aretheyrevolutionarilyimprovable 091201141313 Phpapp01Pekka Ihanainen
 

Was ist angesagt? (9)

Racism and imperialism in the child development discourse (chapter) (2010)
Racism and imperialism in the child development discourse (chapter) (2010)Racism and imperialism in the child development discourse (chapter) (2010)
Racism and imperialism in the child development discourse (chapter) (2010)
 
Marres & lezaun materials and devices of the public
Marres & lezaun   materials and devices of the publicMarres & lezaun   materials and devices of the public
Marres & lezaun materials and devices of the public
 
MP0605 (R)
MP0605 (R)MP0605 (R)
MP0605 (R)
 
Passion and Love (P/L) with Knowledge and Understanding (K/U): Coherence, Con...
Passion and Love (P/L) with Knowledge and Understanding (K/U): Coherence, Con...Passion and Love (P/L) with Knowledge and Understanding (K/U): Coherence, Con...
Passion and Love (P/L) with Knowledge and Understanding (K/U): Coherence, Con...
 
Class 9 Cbse Science Syllabus 2011-12
Class 9 Cbse Science Syllabus 2011-12Class 9 Cbse Science Syllabus 2011-12
Class 9 Cbse Science Syllabus 2011-12
 
Cbse Class 9 Science Syllabus 2012-13
Cbse Class 9 Science Syllabus 2012-13Cbse Class 9 Science Syllabus 2012-13
Cbse Class 9 Science Syllabus 2012-13
 
Research as situated cultural practice
Research as situated cultural practiceResearch as situated cultural practice
Research as situated cultural practice
 
Parenthood by art
Parenthood by artParenthood by art
Parenthood by art
 
Schoolandinstruction Aretheyrevolutionarilyimprovable 091201141313 Phpapp01
Schoolandinstruction Aretheyrevolutionarilyimprovable 091201141313 Phpapp01Schoolandinstruction Aretheyrevolutionarilyimprovable 091201141313 Phpapp01
Schoolandinstruction Aretheyrevolutionarilyimprovable 091201141313 Phpapp01
 

Andere mochten auch

Working Memory Constraints on Imitation and Emulation
Working Memory Constraints on Imitation and EmulationWorking Memory Constraints on Imitation and Emulation
Working Memory Constraints on Imitation and EmulationFrancys Subiaul
 
Funny cat images
Funny cat imagesFunny cat images
Funny cat imagesdavidcammer
 
multi_rights_ott_certificate
multi_rights_ott_certificatemulti_rights_ott_certificate
multi_rights_ott_certificateSandor Fulop
 
La docencia en Educación a Distancia: resistencias, compromisos, competencias
La docencia en Educación a Distancia: resistencias, compromisos, competenciasLa docencia en Educación a Distancia: resistencias, compromisos, competencias
La docencia en Educación a Distancia: resistencias, compromisos, competenciasCarina Perretti
 
IFTIKHAR AHMED CV.2015
IFTIKHAR  AHMED  CV.2015IFTIKHAR  AHMED  CV.2015
IFTIKHAR AHMED CV.2015Iftikhar Ahmed
 
Tatawin midoun presentation v16
Tatawin midoun presentation v16Tatawin midoun presentation v16
Tatawin midoun presentation v16ELYAS90
 
VOLAR SOBRE EL PANTANO
VOLAR SOBRE EL PANTANOVOLAR SOBRE EL PANTANO
VOLAR SOBRE EL PANTANOJorge Llosa
 
TAG2015: ArchiteKultur – wie bekommen wir Architekturarbeit in den Alltag rei...
TAG2015: ArchiteKultur – wie bekommen wir Architekturarbeit in den Alltag rei...TAG2015: ArchiteKultur – wie bekommen wir Architekturarbeit in den Alltag rei...
TAG2015: ArchiteKultur – wie bekommen wir Architekturarbeit in den Alltag rei...Matthias Bohlen
 
Googdocs2pp Test
Googdocs2pp TestGoogdocs2pp Test
Googdocs2pp Testasdf893hf
 
These Web Semantheque
These Web SemanthequeThese Web Semantheque
These Web Semanthequesahar
 

Andere mochten auch (18)

Cheryl's Resume
Cheryl's ResumeCheryl's Resume
Cheryl's Resume
 
Working Memory Constraints on Imitation and Emulation
Working Memory Constraints on Imitation and EmulationWorking Memory Constraints on Imitation and Emulation
Working Memory Constraints on Imitation and Emulation
 
Grcka
GrckaGrcka
Grcka
 
Funny cat images
Funny cat imagesFunny cat images
Funny cat images
 
multi_rights_ott_certificate
multi_rights_ott_certificatemulti_rights_ott_certificate
multi_rights_ott_certificate
 
La docencia en Educación a Distancia: resistencias, compromisos, competencias
La docencia en Educación a Distancia: resistencias, compromisos, competenciasLa docencia en Educación a Distancia: resistencias, compromisos, competencias
La docencia en Educación a Distancia: resistencias, compromisos, competencias
 
IFTIKHAR AHMED CV.2015
IFTIKHAR  AHMED  CV.2015IFTIKHAR  AHMED  CV.2015
IFTIKHAR AHMED CV.2015
 
Vogelmensch
VogelmenschVogelmensch
Vogelmensch
 
RÍO OKAVANGO
RÍO OKAVANGORÍO OKAVANGO
RÍO OKAVANGO
 
Bellas fotos
Bellas fotosBellas fotos
Bellas fotos
 
Tatawin midoun presentation v16
Tatawin midoun presentation v16Tatawin midoun presentation v16
Tatawin midoun presentation v16
 
CUANDO ME AME
CUANDO ME AMECUANDO ME AME
CUANDO ME AME
 
VOLAR SOBRE EL PANTANO
VOLAR SOBRE EL PANTANOVOLAR SOBRE EL PANTANO
VOLAR SOBRE EL PANTANO
 
TAG2015: ArchiteKultur – wie bekommen wir Architekturarbeit in den Alltag rei...
TAG2015: ArchiteKultur – wie bekommen wir Architekturarbeit in den Alltag rei...TAG2015: ArchiteKultur – wie bekommen wir Architekturarbeit in den Alltag rei...
TAG2015: ArchiteKultur – wie bekommen wir Architekturarbeit in den Alltag rei...
 
Googdocs2pp Test
Googdocs2pp TestGoogdocs2pp Test
Googdocs2pp Test
 
Carlos
CarlosCarlos
Carlos
 
These Web Semantheque
These Web SemanthequeThese Web Semantheque
These Web Semantheque
 
O calibre ou pe de rei
O calibre ou pe de reiO calibre ou pe de rei
O calibre ou pe de rei
 

Ähnlich wie Social Learning in Humans and Nonhuman Animals: Theoretical and Empirical Dissections

A Unified Theory of Development A Dialectic Integration of Na.docx
A Unified Theory of Development A Dialectic Integration of Na.docxA Unified Theory of Development A Dialectic Integration of Na.docx
A Unified Theory of Development A Dialectic Integration of Na.docxdaniahendric
 
SPECIAL FEATURE PERSPECTIVEUnraveling the evolution of un.docx
SPECIAL FEATURE PERSPECTIVEUnraveling the evolution of un.docxSPECIAL FEATURE PERSPECTIVEUnraveling the evolution of un.docx
SPECIAL FEATURE PERSPECTIVEUnraveling the evolution of un.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and e...
The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and e...The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and e...
The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and e...Francys Subiaul
 
A Conceptual Framework for Examining Adolescent Identity,Med.docx
A Conceptual Framework for Examining Adolescent Identity,Med.docxA Conceptual Framework for Examining Adolescent Identity,Med.docx
A Conceptual Framework for Examining Adolescent Identity,Med.docxsleeperharwell
 
Optimising for Cultural Learning - Velocity EU 2013
Optimising for Cultural Learning - Velocity EU 2013Optimising for Cultural Learning - Velocity EU 2013
Optimising for Cultural Learning - Velocity EU 2013Christopher Read
 
Unit 2 Theoretical and Methodological IssuesSubunit 1 Concep.docx
Unit 2 Theoretical and Methodological IssuesSubunit 1 Concep.docxUnit 2 Theoretical and Methodological IssuesSubunit 1 Concep.docx
Unit 2 Theoretical and Methodological IssuesSubunit 1 Concep.docxouldparis
 
An Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
An Emerging Theory Of Human RelatednessAn Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
An Emerging Theory Of Human RelatednessJim Webb
 
CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docx
CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docxCURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docx
CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docxdorishigh
 
Social Work in Public Health, 26471–481, 2011Copyright © .docx
Social Work in Public Health, 26471–481, 2011Copyright © .docxSocial Work in Public Health, 26471–481, 2011Copyright © .docx
Social Work in Public Health, 26471–481, 2011Copyright © .docxrosemariebrayshaw
 
Learning_evolution_through_SSI.pdf
Learning_evolution_through_SSI.pdfLearning_evolution_through_SSI.pdf
Learning_evolution_through_SSI.pdfJoão Soares
 
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docxLinking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docxjesssueann
 
Disadvantages Of Applied Ethnomethodology
Disadvantages Of Applied EthnomethodologyDisadvantages Of Applied Ethnomethodology
Disadvantages Of Applied EthnomethodologyAshley Fisher
 
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research MethodsStrengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research MethodsSandra Arveseth
 
Feygina & Henry (2015, Oxford) Culture and Prosocial Behavior
Feygina & Henry (2015, Oxford) Culture and Prosocial BehaviorFeygina & Henry (2015, Oxford) Culture and Prosocial Behavior
Feygina & Henry (2015, Oxford) Culture and Prosocial BehaviorIrina Feygina, Ph.D.
 
Annotated bibliography1- Kulesza, J. (January 01, 2014). Due Dil.docx
Annotated bibliography1- Kulesza, J. (January 01, 2014). Due Dil.docxAnnotated bibliography1- Kulesza, J. (January 01, 2014). Due Dil.docx
Annotated bibliography1- Kulesza, J. (January 01, 2014). Due Dil.docxrossskuddershamus
 
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their LivesBullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their LivesTrading Game Pty Ltd
 
Module-13-Ethnomethodology.pdf
Module-13-Ethnomethodology.pdfModule-13-Ethnomethodology.pdf
Module-13-Ethnomethodology.pdfVikramjit Singh
 
Running head TECHNOLOGY IN HUMAN EDUCATION 1 Te.docx
Running head TECHNOLOGY IN HUMAN EDUCATION 1 Te.docxRunning head TECHNOLOGY IN HUMAN EDUCATION 1 Te.docx
Running head TECHNOLOGY IN HUMAN EDUCATION 1 Te.docxagnesdcarey33086
 

Ähnlich wie Social Learning in Humans and Nonhuman Animals: Theoretical and Empirical Dissections (20)

A Unified Theory of Development A Dialectic Integration of Na.docx
A Unified Theory of Development A Dialectic Integration of Na.docxA Unified Theory of Development A Dialectic Integration of Na.docx
A Unified Theory of Development A Dialectic Integration of Na.docx
 
SPECIAL FEATURE PERSPECTIVEUnraveling the evolution of un.docx
SPECIAL FEATURE PERSPECTIVEUnraveling the evolution of un.docxSPECIAL FEATURE PERSPECTIVEUnraveling the evolution of un.docx
SPECIAL FEATURE PERSPECTIVEUnraveling the evolution of un.docx
 
JohnsonKarinDArcy2006
JohnsonKarinDArcy2006JohnsonKarinDArcy2006
JohnsonKarinDArcy2006
 
The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and e...
The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and e...The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and e...
The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and e...
 
A Conceptual Framework for Examining Adolescent Identity,Med.docx
A Conceptual Framework for Examining Adolescent Identity,Med.docxA Conceptual Framework for Examining Adolescent Identity,Med.docx
A Conceptual Framework for Examining Adolescent Identity,Med.docx
 
Optimising for Cultural Learning - Velocity EU 2013
Optimising for Cultural Learning - Velocity EU 2013Optimising for Cultural Learning - Velocity EU 2013
Optimising for Cultural Learning - Velocity EU 2013
 
Unit 2 Theoretical and Methodological IssuesSubunit 1 Concep.docx
Unit 2 Theoretical and Methodological IssuesSubunit 1 Concep.docxUnit 2 Theoretical and Methodological IssuesSubunit 1 Concep.docx
Unit 2 Theoretical and Methodological IssuesSubunit 1 Concep.docx
 
An Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
An Emerging Theory Of Human RelatednessAn Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
An Emerging Theory Of Human Relatedness
 
lesson 1.pptx
lesson 1.pptxlesson 1.pptx
lesson 1.pptx
 
CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docx
CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docxCURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docx
CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docx
 
Social Work in Public Health, 26471–481, 2011Copyright © .docx
Social Work in Public Health, 26471–481, 2011Copyright © .docxSocial Work in Public Health, 26471–481, 2011Copyright © .docx
Social Work in Public Health, 26471–481, 2011Copyright © .docx
 
Learning_evolution_through_SSI.pdf
Learning_evolution_through_SSI.pdfLearning_evolution_through_SSI.pdf
Learning_evolution_through_SSI.pdf
 
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docxLinking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
Linking Social Change and Developmental ChangeShifting Path.docx
 
Disadvantages Of Applied Ethnomethodology
Disadvantages Of Applied EthnomethodologyDisadvantages Of Applied Ethnomethodology
Disadvantages Of Applied Ethnomethodology
 
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research MethodsStrengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
Strengths And Weaknesses Of Social Research Methods
 
Feygina & Henry (2015, Oxford) Culture and Prosocial Behavior
Feygina & Henry (2015, Oxford) Culture and Prosocial BehaviorFeygina & Henry (2015, Oxford) Culture and Prosocial Behavior
Feygina & Henry (2015, Oxford) Culture and Prosocial Behavior
 
Annotated bibliography1- Kulesza, J. (January 01, 2014). Due Dil.docx
Annotated bibliography1- Kulesza, J. (January 01, 2014). Due Dil.docxAnnotated bibliography1- Kulesza, J. (January 01, 2014). Due Dil.docx
Annotated bibliography1- Kulesza, J. (January 01, 2014). Due Dil.docx
 
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their LivesBullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
Bullshiters - Who Are They And What Do We Know About Their Lives
 
Module-13-Ethnomethodology.pdf
Module-13-Ethnomethodology.pdfModule-13-Ethnomethodology.pdf
Module-13-Ethnomethodology.pdf
 
Running head TECHNOLOGY IN HUMAN EDUCATION 1 Te.docx
Running head TECHNOLOGY IN HUMAN EDUCATION 1 Te.docxRunning head TECHNOLOGY IN HUMAN EDUCATION 1 Te.docx
Running head TECHNOLOGY IN HUMAN EDUCATION 1 Te.docx
 

Mehr von Francys Subiaul

Mosaic Cognitive Evolution: The case of imitation learning
Mosaic Cognitive Evolution: The case of imitation learningMosaic Cognitive Evolution: The case of imitation learning
Mosaic Cognitive Evolution: The case of imitation learningFrancys Subiaul
 
Gorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory tests
Gorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory testsGorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory tests
Gorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory testsFrancys Subiaul
 
Multiple imitation mechanisms in children
Multiple imitation mechanisms in childrenMultiple imitation mechanisms in children
Multiple imitation mechanisms in childrenFrancys Subiaul
 
Curious monkeys have increased gray matter density in the precuneus
Curious monkeys have increased gray matter density in the precuneusCurious monkeys have increased gray matter density in the precuneus
Curious monkeys have increased gray matter density in the precuneusFrancys Subiaul
 
The Ghosts in the Computer: The Role of Agency and Animacy Attributions in “...
The Ghosts in the Computer:  The Role of Agency and Animacy Attributions in “...The Ghosts in the Computer:  The Role of Agency and Animacy Attributions in “...
The Ghosts in the Computer: The Role of Agency and Animacy Attributions in “...Francys Subiaul
 
Dissecting the Imitation Faculty: The Multiple Imitation Mechanisms (MIM) Hyp...
Dissecting the Imitation Faculty: The Multiple Imitation Mechanisms (MIM) Hyp...Dissecting the Imitation Faculty: The Multiple Imitation Mechanisms (MIM) Hyp...
Dissecting the Imitation Faculty: The Multiple Imitation Mechanisms (MIM) Hyp...Francys Subiaul
 
Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...
Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...
Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...Francys Subiaul
 
A natural history of the human mind
A natural history of the human mindA natural history of the human mind
A natural history of the human mindFrancys Subiaul
 
Do Chimpanzees Learn Reputation by Observation? Evidence from Direct and Ind...
Do Chimpanzees Learn Reputation by Observation?  Evidence from Direct and Ind...Do Chimpanzees Learn Reputation by Observation?  Evidence from Direct and Ind...
Do Chimpanzees Learn Reputation by Observation? Evidence from Direct and Ind...Francys Subiaul
 
Cognitive Imitation in 2-Year Old Children (Homo sapiens): A comparison with...
Cognitive Imitation in 2-Year Old Children (Homo sapiens):  A comparison with...Cognitive Imitation in 2-Year Old Children (Homo sapiens):  A comparison with...
Cognitive Imitation in 2-Year Old Children (Homo sapiens): A comparison with...Francys Subiaul
 
Human cognitive specilizations
Human cognitive specilizationsHuman cognitive specilizations
Human cognitive specilizationsFrancys Subiaul
 
Cognitive imitation in rhesus monkeys
Cognitive imitation in rhesus monkeysCognitive imitation in rhesus monkeys
Cognitive imitation in rhesus monkeysFrancys Subiaul
 
Becoming a high-fidelity--Super--Imitator: What are the contributions of soci...
Becoming a high-fidelity--Super--Imitator: What are the contributions of soci...Becoming a high-fidelity--Super--Imitator: What are the contributions of soci...
Becoming a high-fidelity--Super--Imitator: What are the contributions of soci...Francys Subiaul
 
The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years.
The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years. The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years.
The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years. Francys Subiaul
 

Mehr von Francys Subiaul (14)

Mosaic Cognitive Evolution: The case of imitation learning
Mosaic Cognitive Evolution: The case of imitation learningMosaic Cognitive Evolution: The case of imitation learning
Mosaic Cognitive Evolution: The case of imitation learning
 
Gorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory tests
Gorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory testsGorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory tests
Gorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory tests
 
Multiple imitation mechanisms in children
Multiple imitation mechanisms in childrenMultiple imitation mechanisms in children
Multiple imitation mechanisms in children
 
Curious monkeys have increased gray matter density in the precuneus
Curious monkeys have increased gray matter density in the precuneusCurious monkeys have increased gray matter density in the precuneus
Curious monkeys have increased gray matter density in the precuneus
 
The Ghosts in the Computer: The Role of Agency and Animacy Attributions in “...
The Ghosts in the Computer:  The Role of Agency and Animacy Attributions in “...The Ghosts in the Computer:  The Role of Agency and Animacy Attributions in “...
The Ghosts in the Computer: The Role of Agency and Animacy Attributions in “...
 
Dissecting the Imitation Faculty: The Multiple Imitation Mechanisms (MIM) Hyp...
Dissecting the Imitation Faculty: The Multiple Imitation Mechanisms (MIM) Hyp...Dissecting the Imitation Faculty: The Multiple Imitation Mechanisms (MIM) Hyp...
Dissecting the Imitation Faculty: The Multiple Imitation Mechanisms (MIM) Hyp...
 
Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...
Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...
Carryover effects of joint attention to repeated events in chimpanzees and yo...
 
A natural history of the human mind
A natural history of the human mindA natural history of the human mind
A natural history of the human mind
 
Do Chimpanzees Learn Reputation by Observation? Evidence from Direct and Ind...
Do Chimpanzees Learn Reputation by Observation?  Evidence from Direct and Ind...Do Chimpanzees Learn Reputation by Observation?  Evidence from Direct and Ind...
Do Chimpanzees Learn Reputation by Observation? Evidence from Direct and Ind...
 
Cognitive Imitation in 2-Year Old Children (Homo sapiens): A comparison with...
Cognitive Imitation in 2-Year Old Children (Homo sapiens):  A comparison with...Cognitive Imitation in 2-Year Old Children (Homo sapiens):  A comparison with...
Cognitive Imitation in 2-Year Old Children (Homo sapiens): A comparison with...
 
Human cognitive specilizations
Human cognitive specilizationsHuman cognitive specilizations
Human cognitive specilizations
 
Cognitive imitation in rhesus monkeys
Cognitive imitation in rhesus monkeysCognitive imitation in rhesus monkeys
Cognitive imitation in rhesus monkeys
 
Becoming a high-fidelity--Super--Imitator: What are the contributions of soci...
Becoming a high-fidelity--Super--Imitator: What are the contributions of soci...Becoming a high-fidelity--Super--Imitator: What are the contributions of soci...
Becoming a high-fidelity--Super--Imitator: What are the contributions of soci...
 
The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years.
The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years. The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years.
The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years.
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

Base editing, prime editing, Cas13 & RNA editing and organelle base editing
Base editing, prime editing, Cas13 & RNA editing and organelle base editingBase editing, prime editing, Cas13 & RNA editing and organelle base editing
Base editing, prime editing, Cas13 & RNA editing and organelle base editingNetHelix
 
User Guide: Pulsar™ Weather Station (Columbia Weather Systems)
User Guide: Pulsar™ Weather Station (Columbia Weather Systems)User Guide: Pulsar™ Weather Station (Columbia Weather Systems)
User Guide: Pulsar™ Weather Station (Columbia Weather Systems)Columbia Weather Systems
 
Oxo-Acids of Halogens and their Salts.pptx
Oxo-Acids of Halogens and their Salts.pptxOxo-Acids of Halogens and their Salts.pptx
Oxo-Acids of Halogens and their Salts.pptxfarhanvvdk
 
Q4-Mod-1c-Quiz-Projectile-333344444.pptx
Q4-Mod-1c-Quiz-Projectile-333344444.pptxQ4-Mod-1c-Quiz-Projectile-333344444.pptx
Q4-Mod-1c-Quiz-Projectile-333344444.pptxtuking87
 
User Guide: Magellan MX™ Weather Station
User Guide: Magellan MX™ Weather StationUser Guide: Magellan MX™ Weather Station
User Guide: Magellan MX™ Weather StationColumbia Weather Systems
 
linear Regression, multiple Regression and Annova
linear Regression, multiple Regression and Annovalinear Regression, multiple Regression and Annova
linear Regression, multiple Regression and AnnovaMansi Rastogi
 
User Guide: Capricorn FLX™ Weather Station
User Guide: Capricorn FLX™ Weather StationUser Guide: Capricorn FLX™ Weather Station
User Guide: Capricorn FLX™ Weather StationColumbia Weather Systems
 
Replisome-Cohesin Interfacing A Molecular Perspective.pdf
Replisome-Cohesin Interfacing A Molecular Perspective.pdfReplisome-Cohesin Interfacing A Molecular Perspective.pdf
Replisome-Cohesin Interfacing A Molecular Perspective.pdfAtiaGohar1
 
Pests of soyabean_Binomics_IdentificationDr.UPR.pdf
Pests of soyabean_Binomics_IdentificationDr.UPR.pdfPests of soyabean_Binomics_IdentificationDr.UPR.pdf
Pests of soyabean_Binomics_IdentificationDr.UPR.pdfPirithiRaju
 
The Sensory Organs, Anatomy and Function
The Sensory Organs, Anatomy and FunctionThe Sensory Organs, Anatomy and Function
The Sensory Organs, Anatomy and FunctionJadeNovelo1
 
final waves properties grade 7 - third quarter
final waves properties grade 7 - third quarterfinal waves properties grade 7 - third quarter
final waves properties grade 7 - third quarterHanHyoKim
 
6.2 Pests of Sesame_Identification_Binomics_Dr.UPR
6.2 Pests of Sesame_Identification_Binomics_Dr.UPR6.2 Pests of Sesame_Identification_Binomics_Dr.UPR
6.2 Pests of Sesame_Identification_Binomics_Dr.UPRPirithiRaju
 
GENERAL PHYSICS 2 REFRACTION OF LIGHT SENIOR HIGH SCHOOL GENPHYS2.pptx
GENERAL PHYSICS 2 REFRACTION OF LIGHT SENIOR HIGH SCHOOL GENPHYS2.pptxGENERAL PHYSICS 2 REFRACTION OF LIGHT SENIOR HIGH SCHOOL GENPHYS2.pptx
GENERAL PHYSICS 2 REFRACTION OF LIGHT SENIOR HIGH SCHOOL GENPHYS2.pptxRitchAndruAgustin
 
Thermodynamics ,types of system,formulae ,gibbs free energy .pptx
Thermodynamics ,types of system,formulae ,gibbs free energy .pptxThermodynamics ,types of system,formulae ,gibbs free energy .pptx
Thermodynamics ,types of system,formulae ,gibbs free energy .pptxuniversity
 
LESSON PLAN IN SCIENCE GRADE 4 WEEK 1 DAY 2
LESSON PLAN IN SCIENCE GRADE 4 WEEK 1 DAY 2LESSON PLAN IN SCIENCE GRADE 4 WEEK 1 DAY 2
LESSON PLAN IN SCIENCE GRADE 4 WEEK 1 DAY 2AuEnriquezLontok
 
whole genome sequencing new and its types including shortgun and clone by clone
whole genome sequencing new  and its types including shortgun and clone by clonewhole genome sequencing new  and its types including shortgun and clone by clone
whole genome sequencing new and its types including shortgun and clone by clonechaudhary charan shingh university
 
Introduction of Human Body & Structure of cell.pptx
Introduction of Human Body & Structure of cell.pptxIntroduction of Human Body & Structure of cell.pptx
Introduction of Human Body & Structure of cell.pptxMedical College
 
Pests of Sunflower_Binomics_Identification_Dr.UPR
Pests of Sunflower_Binomics_Identification_Dr.UPRPests of Sunflower_Binomics_Identification_Dr.UPR
Pests of Sunflower_Binomics_Identification_Dr.UPRPirithiRaju
 
trihybrid cross , test cross chi squares
trihybrid cross , test cross chi squarestrihybrid cross , test cross chi squares
trihybrid cross , test cross chi squaresusmanzain586
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

Base editing, prime editing, Cas13 & RNA editing and organelle base editing
Base editing, prime editing, Cas13 & RNA editing and organelle base editingBase editing, prime editing, Cas13 & RNA editing and organelle base editing
Base editing, prime editing, Cas13 & RNA editing and organelle base editing
 
User Guide: Pulsar™ Weather Station (Columbia Weather Systems)
User Guide: Pulsar™ Weather Station (Columbia Weather Systems)User Guide: Pulsar™ Weather Station (Columbia Weather Systems)
User Guide: Pulsar™ Weather Station (Columbia Weather Systems)
 
Oxo-Acids of Halogens and their Salts.pptx
Oxo-Acids of Halogens and their Salts.pptxOxo-Acids of Halogens and their Salts.pptx
Oxo-Acids of Halogens and their Salts.pptx
 
Q4-Mod-1c-Quiz-Projectile-333344444.pptx
Q4-Mod-1c-Quiz-Projectile-333344444.pptxQ4-Mod-1c-Quiz-Projectile-333344444.pptx
Q4-Mod-1c-Quiz-Projectile-333344444.pptx
 
User Guide: Magellan MX™ Weather Station
User Guide: Magellan MX™ Weather StationUser Guide: Magellan MX™ Weather Station
User Guide: Magellan MX™ Weather Station
 
linear Regression, multiple Regression and Annova
linear Regression, multiple Regression and Annovalinear Regression, multiple Regression and Annova
linear Regression, multiple Regression and Annova
 
User Guide: Capricorn FLX™ Weather Station
User Guide: Capricorn FLX™ Weather StationUser Guide: Capricorn FLX™ Weather Station
User Guide: Capricorn FLX™ Weather Station
 
Replisome-Cohesin Interfacing A Molecular Perspective.pdf
Replisome-Cohesin Interfacing A Molecular Perspective.pdfReplisome-Cohesin Interfacing A Molecular Perspective.pdf
Replisome-Cohesin Interfacing A Molecular Perspective.pdf
 
Pests of soyabean_Binomics_IdentificationDr.UPR.pdf
Pests of soyabean_Binomics_IdentificationDr.UPR.pdfPests of soyabean_Binomics_IdentificationDr.UPR.pdf
Pests of soyabean_Binomics_IdentificationDr.UPR.pdf
 
The Sensory Organs, Anatomy and Function
The Sensory Organs, Anatomy and FunctionThe Sensory Organs, Anatomy and Function
The Sensory Organs, Anatomy and Function
 
final waves properties grade 7 - third quarter
final waves properties grade 7 - third quarterfinal waves properties grade 7 - third quarter
final waves properties grade 7 - third quarter
 
6.2 Pests of Sesame_Identification_Binomics_Dr.UPR
6.2 Pests of Sesame_Identification_Binomics_Dr.UPR6.2 Pests of Sesame_Identification_Binomics_Dr.UPR
6.2 Pests of Sesame_Identification_Binomics_Dr.UPR
 
GENERAL PHYSICS 2 REFRACTION OF LIGHT SENIOR HIGH SCHOOL GENPHYS2.pptx
GENERAL PHYSICS 2 REFRACTION OF LIGHT SENIOR HIGH SCHOOL GENPHYS2.pptxGENERAL PHYSICS 2 REFRACTION OF LIGHT SENIOR HIGH SCHOOL GENPHYS2.pptx
GENERAL PHYSICS 2 REFRACTION OF LIGHT SENIOR HIGH SCHOOL GENPHYS2.pptx
 
Thermodynamics ,types of system,formulae ,gibbs free energy .pptx
Thermodynamics ,types of system,formulae ,gibbs free energy .pptxThermodynamics ,types of system,formulae ,gibbs free energy .pptx
Thermodynamics ,types of system,formulae ,gibbs free energy .pptx
 
LESSON PLAN IN SCIENCE GRADE 4 WEEK 1 DAY 2
LESSON PLAN IN SCIENCE GRADE 4 WEEK 1 DAY 2LESSON PLAN IN SCIENCE GRADE 4 WEEK 1 DAY 2
LESSON PLAN IN SCIENCE GRADE 4 WEEK 1 DAY 2
 
whole genome sequencing new and its types including shortgun and clone by clone
whole genome sequencing new  and its types including shortgun and clone by clonewhole genome sequencing new  and its types including shortgun and clone by clone
whole genome sequencing new and its types including shortgun and clone by clone
 
Introduction of Human Body & Structure of cell.pptx
Introduction of Human Body & Structure of cell.pptxIntroduction of Human Body & Structure of cell.pptx
Introduction of Human Body & Structure of cell.pptx
 
Pests of Sunflower_Binomics_Identification_Dr.UPR
Pests of Sunflower_Binomics_Identification_Dr.UPRPests of Sunflower_Binomics_Identification_Dr.UPR
Pests of Sunflower_Binomics_Identification_Dr.UPR
 
Let’s Say Someone Did Drop the Bomb. Then What?
Let’s Say Someone Did Drop the Bomb. Then What?Let’s Say Someone Did Drop the Bomb. Then What?
Let’s Say Someone Did Drop the Bomb. Then What?
 
trihybrid cross , test cross chi squares
trihybrid cross , test cross chi squarestrihybrid cross , test cross chi squares
trihybrid cross , test cross chi squares
 

Social Learning in Humans and Nonhuman Animals: Theoretical and Empirical Dissections

  • 1. Social Learning in Humans and Nonhuman Animals: Theoretical and Empirical Dissections Mark Nielsen University of Queensland Francys Subiaul The George Washington University Andrew Whiten University of St. Andrews Bennett Galef McMaster University Thomas Zentall University of Kentucky The past decade has seen a resurgent, concerted interest in social learning research comparing human and nonhuman animals. Such research has involved not only examination of the basic mecha- nisms and processes of social learning but also determinations of which species and age classes engage in these various sorts of social learning. It has considered the costs and benefits of social learning and the circumstances that favor reliance on social rather than asocial learning. As a result of such investigations, the study of social learning has become one of the most important and intensely researched fields of comparative psychology. This re- naissance, fostered by multidisciplinary collaborations, has re- sulted in significant methodological and conceptual innovations. In this special issue, we present a synthesis of work that con- solidates what is currently known and provides a platform for future research. Consequently, we include both new empirical studies and novel theoretical proposals describing work with both human children and adults and a range of nonhuman animals. In this introduction, we describe the background of this special issue and provide a context for each of the eight articles it contains. We hope such introduction will not only help the reader synthesize the interdisciplinary views that characterize this broad field, but also stimulate development of new methods, concepts, and data. The survival of any animal, human or nonhuman, depends on its acquiring a range of skills, among others how to extract food from defended sources, how or where to hide to avoid predators (or be successful as one), and how to increase access to mates. Some of these skills might be genetically canalized and some might be acquired through individual, trial-and-error learning. However, successful development of many such skills would not occur in the absence of interaction with knowledgeable others. Consequently, the study of social learning has become a major focus of research in those sciences interested in the behavior of both humans and nonhumans, and the effort devoted to such research has increased greatly in recent years. Figure 1 provides a measure of that increase, presenting the number of peer-reviewed papers published since 1982 and listed in PsycINFO that include social learning or imitation as keywords. For comparison, we have also provided similar data for publica- tions listing as a keyword theory of mind (another active area of contemporary research in social cognition). Of note here are the distinct trajectories taken by theory of mind compared with social learning and imitation. Whereas since 1981 papers with theory of mind as a keyword have increased in frequency almost linearly, those listing social learning, imitation, or both dip toward the end of 2001 and then climb rapidly (it is worth noting that of the 166 papers that include both social learning and imitation as keywords, 103—or 62%—were published since 2001). There are many possible reasons for this climb since 2001 in the number of papers with social learning and imitation as keywords. One is the attention drawn in the social psychology literature to the role that copying plays in smoothing interactions and increasing liking between social partners (e.g., Chartrand & Bargh, 1999, with current citations exceeding 600). Another is investigation of the neural underpinnings of imitative behavior, including the pos- sible implication of mirror neurons (e.g., Iacoboni et al., 1999, with current citations exceeding 950) and the linking of these to deficits associated with autism (Williams, Whiten, Suddendorf, & Perrett, 2001, with current citations exceeding 250). Yet another plausible explanation lies in the growing body of evidence that humans are not the only animals for whom cultural transmission plays a significant role in the development of behavioral reper- toires (Whiten et al., 1999, with current citations exceeding 650). A rising tide of scientific studies of such cultural phenomena in both human and nonhuman species has focused attention on the evolution of culture, its development and transmission from gen- eration to generation by processes of social learning. The link between the macro perspective on culture (an inher- ently population-level phenomenon) and the micro perspective focused on underlying social learning mechanisms was explicit in the pairing of a 2010 Royal Society Discussion Meeting—Culture Evolves (http://www.cultureevolves.org)—with a satellite meet- ing—Social Learning in Humans and Nonhuman Animals: Theo- Mark Nielsen, ●●●, University of Queensland, ●●●; Francys Subiaul, ●●●, The George Washington University; Andrew Whiten, ●●●, Univer- sity of St. Andrews, ●●●; Bennett Galef, ●●●, McMaster University, ●●●; Thomas Zentall, ●●●, University of Kentucky. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Francys Subiaul, ●●●. E-mail: subiaul@gwu.edu Journal of Comparative Psychology © 2012 American Psychological Association 2012, Vol. ●●, No. ●, 000–000 0735-7036/12/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0027758 1 AQ: 1 AQ: 3 F1 tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124
  • 2. retical and Empirical Dissections—held at the Kavli Royal Society International Centre at Chichley Hall that followed immediately after. The satellite meeting brought together an international com- munity of scientists active in developing taxonomies, theories, and conceptual schemes for the dissection of different kinds of social learning, and empirical studies aimed at implementing such dis- sections. The presentations provided frameworks for synthesizing the interdisciplinary views that characterize the broad field of social learning while stimulating the development of new methods, concepts, and data. It is from these presentations that the articles included in this special issue have emerged. Papers presented at the Culture Evolves Discussion Meeting have already been published in a special issue of Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B (Volume 366, whole issue 1567, pp. 935–1187; see Whiten, Hinde, Laland, & Stringer, 2011). Commensurate with the increasing focus on issues relating to social learning evident in Figure 1 has been an effort by contem- porary authors to identify and define the multiple mechanisms that underpin such learning. Consider the oft-cited example of Imo, the then-1.5-year-old female Japanese macaque who discovered that a sand-covered sweet potato could be washed by taking it into a nearby stream, thereby making it palatable. The practice subse- quently spread throughout Imo’s troop along clearly defined social networks. At one time, this was suggested to be “probably the single most impressive case of imitation on record” (Premack, 1984, p. 17). The same behavior if reported today would be assigned to one of numerous alternative categories of social learn- ing, including various forms of imitation (action, motor, contex- tual, procedural, automatic, vocal, cognitive, and program level), as well as goal and result emulation, affordance learning, object movement reenactment, observational conditioning, or contagion (Byrne, 2002; Byrne & Russon, 1998; Call & Carpenter, 2002; Galef, 1988; Heyes, 1994, 2011; Hoppitt & Laland, 2008; Subiaul, 2007, 2010; Want & Harris, 2002; Whiten & Ham, 1992; Whiten, Horner, Litchfield, & Marshall-Pescini, 2004; Zentall, 2001, 2006, 2011). As a result, for many, the ascription of imitation to the impressive case of sweet potato washing is no longer justified (Galef, 2004; Kendal, Kendal, & Laland, 2007). At the heart of a number of the articles we present are the application and devel- opment of these taxonomies of social learning and the associated dissection of empirical cases. Here, Zentall’s (2012) concise yet comprehensive review of the main branches of this social learning literature emphasizes that any attempt to understand social learning will be incomplete unless it both ranges across the animal kingdom (e.g., the data on avian imitation is compelling, rendering as suspect any claims that the capacity for imitation belongs to humans only) and strives to distinguish a host of alternative mechanisms of social learning that might underlie specific cases. Zentall’s review draws attention to the fact that despite the increased attention focused on social learning research in the past few decades, we remain a long way from understanding the underlying cognitive and neural processes that enable animals to match their behavior to that of a demon- strator. Burkart, Kupferberg, Glasauer, and van Schaik (2012) present empirical work suggesting that even when engaged in basic forms of social learning such as local enhancement, marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) use the perceived intentions of a model to guide their behavior. Adapting Woodward’s (1998) highly influ- ential habituation–dishabituation paradigm, Burkart and col- leagues presented monkeys with an agent who repeatedly ap- proached and interacted with one of two objects. The marmosets perceived the agent’s behavior as goal-directed whether the agent was a conspecific or a robot, but not if it was a black box. Social facilitation and enhancement learning were strongest when the agent was a conspecific and weakest when the agent was a black box. It has been suggested that social learning guided by intention understanding is necessary for cumulative culture in humans, but the existence of sensitivity to intention in marmosets indicates that such sensitivity is not sufficient for cumulative culture to emerge. Delineating processes of social learning is a prominent feature of the articles by Zentall and by Burkart and colleagues, an endeavor with a long pedigree in the comparative and nonhuman animal literature. In contrast, as Want and Harris (2002) lament, research with humans has tended to lag in assimilating the taxon- omies outlined previously as standard practice. Even now, many Figure 1. Number of peer-reviewed publications listed in PsycINFO since 1982 that have only social learning, only imitation, both social learning and imitation, and theory of mind as keywords. 2 NIELSEN, SUBIAUL, WHITEN, GALEF, AND ZENTALL tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124
  • 3. papers continue to be published that use imitation as a blanket term for any form of copying. Here, Huang’s (2012) contribution rep- resents a clear exception to this rule. Huang presents three novel experiments identifying the conditions under which infants engage in emulation learning. In these experiments, infants are presented with varying degrees of information about the outcomes (or end states) associated with a series of novel objects. Huang’s study shows that emulation (learning from the results of others’ actions) is relatively efficient when (a) the target items involve outcomes that are self-contained and are produced with a single-step action but not when the items comprise two separate parts that can be combined into a novel end configuration; (b) that seeing the items in both the starting and end states facilitates emulation more than seeing the items in their end states alone, as does seeing an adult model in static postures consistent with the target actions; and (c) that infants’ encoding of bodily cues from static body postures may be susceptible to the concurrence of the spatiotemporal causal relations of objects. Huang’s article makes an important contribu- tion in showing how young children can produce specific object- directed outcomes in the absence of a full demonstration. Given that very young children appear capable of identifying how to bring about object-related outcomes by observing aspects of their beginning and end states, one could expect children to be increasingly unconstrained by the specifics of what is modeled before them. Yet, the results of an increasing number of recent studies suggest that children may replicate even those actions of a model that have no apparent purpose and even after the causal irrelevance of those actions has been made explicit (e.g., Kenward, Karlsson, & Persson, 2011; Lyons, Damrosch, Lin, Macris, & Keil, 2011; Nielsen & Blank, 2011; Nielsen & Tomaselli, 2010). This phenomenon, known as overimitation, features in varying degrees in many of the articles included in this special issue, and it is a core focus of the contribution of McGuigan (2012). McGuigan shows that, unlike young children (Flynn, 2008; Flynn & Whiten, 2008; McGuigan & Graham, 2010), adults will over- imitate to the extent of transmitting irrelevant actions down diffu- sion chains of several individuals, simulating cultural transmis- sion. More important, however, McGuigan demonstrates that such overimitation is influenced by the transparency of the actions; the more obvious the irrelevance of the actions, the less likely they are to be passed on. For McGuigan, overimitation springs from a conformity bias, a human propensity for acquiring culture that becomes increasingly strong with age (Whiten, in press). Caldwell, Schillinger, Evans, and Hopper (2012), building on previous research with adults, show that full demonstration by a live model is not necessary for overimitation or the transmission of information with high fidelity. In their study, participants were shown either a photograph or live model of two different spaghetti towers (model towers built of uncooked spaghetti with modeling clay junctions). Participants were then instructed not to copy the demonstrated model but to build the tallest tower possible. But copying is exactly what they did; rather than attempt to build the tallest tower, participants simply replicated the demonstrated model. Thus, despite not seeing a full demonstration (i.e., observ- ing a model build a tower) and relying only on a model of a completed tower (i.e., end state), participants reproduced models with high fidelity. For Caldwell and colleagues, these results cast doubt on the hypothesis that imitation is necessary for the high- fidelity transmission of information. These results lead us back to the link between imitation and culture that runs through this special issue. The contribution by Nielsen (2012) is explicitly focused on this link. He notes (as do others in our special issue) how the high-fidelity copying charac- teristic of overimitation facilitates rapid transmission of informa- tion while simultaneously enabling nonfunctional traditions to be similarly passed on. However, he raises questions regarding the capacity of overimitation to support innovation and speculates that something else may be needed to drive the vast technological advances characteristic of the Homo lineage. The answer, he suggests, may be found in the pretend play children engage in—a possibility that presupposes the existence of childhood in the first place, which Nielsen argues may be a relatively recently evolved trait of our species. What Nielsen does not dispute is that the evolution of overimitation as a copying strategy is likely to have been pivotal in the evolution of human cumulative culture. But why might it have emerged in the first place? The answer might lie in a species-specific increase in the use of copying to satisfy social motivations. Directly copying others can be a particularly powerful form of learning that permits the rapid acquisition of new skills. However, humans also copy for socially oriented reasons: We will imitate to communicate, to be like others, to be liked by others, and to show others that we are alike (Dijksterhuis, 2005; Nadel, Gue´rini, Peze´, & Rivet, 1999). Here, Over and Carpenter (2012) argue that these social factors have been largely ignored in many accounts of imitation. Their article fully articulates how social motivations function to drive imitation across varying circumstances and ex- perimental arrangements, and using this approach, they explain apparent paradoxes in the literature (notably that children will both overimitate and selectively imitate). For Over and Carpenter, chil- dren’s choices about what to copy are influenced by three primary factors: (a) the relationship between personally acquired and so- cially acquired information, (b) their identification with the model and the social group in general, and (c) the social pressures they experience to imitate in particular ways. For Over and Carpenter, failing to acknowledge the strong pull in humans to do as others do will result in an incomplete picture of imitation that, they con- clude, is a profoundly social process. This highly social view of imitation provides an interesting complement to the argument provided by Heyes (2012), who raises questions as to whether there is anything inherently social about social learning and asks whether the constituent mechanisms of social learning are distinct in any way from those supporting asocial learning. Heyes’s answer is that there are no fundamental differences; rather, both social and asocial learning depend on a common set of associative learning processes that operate in similar ways across animal species. What, then, of arguments that humans are uniquely imitative (e.g., Meltzoff, 1988)? How does this fit with the view outlined by Over and Carpenter? For Heyes, the answer is relatively straightforward. What makes human learn- ing different is that the effects of social living have affected the input mechanisms for learning but not the actual learning mecha- nisms per se. In humans, doing as others do is so strong because from early in infancy such copying behavior is highly rewarded. The nature of the input sets us apart from other animals, not the cognitive processes we use in acquiring behavior. This special issue includes a wide range of perspectives on social learning incorporating new empirical work on human chil- 3SOCIAL LEARNING IN HUMANS AND NONHUMAN ANIMALS tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124
  • 4. dren (Huang), human adults (McGuigan; Caldwell et al.), and monkeys (Burkart et al.) together with contrasting and comple- mentary theoretical views and reviews that outline what is unique and shared among phylogenetically distant species (Heyes; Over & Carpenter; Nielsen; Zentall). This reflects the unprecedented in- terest in social learning that has emerged in the past decade, and the questions raised by the contributors to this issue will help ensure that this interest continues to grow. We have greatly deep- ened our understanding of social learning, but as Zentall reminds us, we still have a lot to learn. References Burkart, J., Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & van Schaik, C. (2012). Even simple forms of social learning rely on intention attribution in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●. Byrne, R. W. (2002). Imitation of novel complex actions: What does the evidence from animals mean? Advances in the Study of Behavior, 31, 77–105. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(02)80006-7 Byrne, R. W., & Russon, A. E. (1998). Learning by imitation: A hierar- chical approach. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 21, 667–684. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X98001745 Caldwell, C. A., Schillinger, K., Evans, C. L., & Hopper, L. M. (2012). End state copying by humans (Homo sapiens): Implications for a compara- tive perspective on cumulative culture. Journal of Comparative Psychol- ogy, ●●●, ●●●–●●●. Call, J., & Carpenter, M. (2002). Three sources of information in social learning. In K. Dautenhahn & C. L. Nehaniv (Eds.), Imitation in animals and artifacts (pp. 211–228). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893 Dijksterhuis, A. (2005). Why we are social animals: The high road to imitation as social glue. In S. Hurley & N. Chater (Eds.), Perspectives on imitation: From neuroscience to social science (Vol. 2: Imitation, hu- man development, and culture, pp. 207–220). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Flynn, E. (2008). Investigating children as cultural magnets: Do young children transmit redundant information along diffusion chains? Philo- sophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 363, 3541– 3551. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0136 Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2008). Cultural transmission of tool use in young children: A diffusion chain study. Social Development, 17, 699–718. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00453.x Galef, B. G. (1988). Imitation in animals: History, definition and interpre- tation of data from the psychological laboratory. In T. R. Zentall & B. G. Galef (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives (pp. 3–28). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Galef, B. G. (2004). Approaches to the study of traditional behaviors of free-living animals. Learning & Behavior, 32, 53–61. doi:10.3758/ BF03196006 Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and mecha- nisms. Biological Reviews, 69, 207–231. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X .1994.tb01506.x Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation [Review]. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 463–483. doi:10.1037/a0022288 Heyes, C. (2012). What’s social about social learning? Journal of Com- parative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●. Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2008). Social processes influencing learning in animals: A review of the evidence. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 38, 105–165. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)00003-X Huang, C. (2012). Outcome-based observational learning in human infants. Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●. Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Giacomo, R. (1999, December 24). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286, 2526–2528. doi:10.1126/science.286.5449.2526 Kendal, J. R., Kendal, R. L., & Laland, K. N. (2007). Quantifying and modelling social learning processes in monkey populations. Interna- tional Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 7, 123–138. Kenward, B., Karlsson, M., & Persson, J. (2011). Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proceed- ings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 1239–1246. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1399 Lyons, D. E., Damrosch, D. H., Lin, J. K., Macris, D. M., & Keil, F. C. (2011). The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 1158–1167. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0335 McGuigan, N. (2012). The role of transmission biases in the cultural diffusion of irrelevant actions. Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●. McGuigan, N., & Graham, M. (2010). Cultural transmission of irrelevant tool actions in diffusion chains of 3- and 5-year-old children. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 561–577. doi:10.1080/ 17405620902858125 Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). The human infant as “homo imitans.” In T. R. Zentall & B. G. J. Galef (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives (pp. 319–341). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Nadel, J., Gue´rini, C., Peze´, A., & Rivet, C. (1999). The evolving nature of imitation as a format for communication. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Imitation in infancy (pp. 209–234). Cambridge, England: Cam- bridge University Press. Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play and childhood: Essential ele- ments in the evolution of human culture? Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●. Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental Psychology, 47, 1050–1053. doi:10.1037/a0023866 Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Over-imitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21, 729–736. doi:10.1177/0956797610368808 Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2012). Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●. Premack, D. (1984). Pedagogy and aesthetics as sources of culture. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Handbook of cognitive neuroscience (pp. 15–35). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Subiaul, F. (2007). The imitation faculty in monkeys: Evaluating its features, distribution and evolution. Journal of Anthropological Sci- ences, 85, 35–62. Subiaul, F. (2010). Dissecting the imitation faculty: The multiple imitation mechanisms (MIM) hypothesis. Behavioural Processes, 83, 222–234. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2009.12.002 Want, S. C., & Harris, P. L. (2002). How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of “imitation” in human children. Developmental Science, 5, 1–14. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00194 Whiten, A. (in press). Social cognition: Making us smart, or sometimes making us dumb? Overimitation, conformity, non-conformity and the transmission of culture in ape and child. In M. Banaji & S. Gelman (Eds.), The development of social cognition. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., . . . Boesch, C. (1999, June 17). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685. doi:10.1038/21415 Whiten, A., & Ham, R. (1992). On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. In P. J. B. Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, G. Beer, & M. Milinski (Eds.), Advances in the 4 NIELSEN, SUBIAUL, WHITEN, GALEF, AND ZENTALL AQ: 2 tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124
  • 5. study of behaviour (Vol. 21, pp. 239–283). New York, NY: Academic Press. Whiten, A., Hinde, R. A., Laland, K. N., & Stringer, C. B. (2011). Culture evolves. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366, 938– 948. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0372 Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. A., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape? Learning & Behavior, 32, 36–52. doi:10.3758/ BF03196005 Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287–295. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00014-8 Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4 Zentall, T. R. (2001). Imitation in animals: Evidence, function and mech- anisms. Cybernetics and Systems, 32, 53–96. doi:10.1080/ 019697201300001812 Zentall, T. R. (2006). Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms. Animal Cognition, 9, 335–353. doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0039-2 Zentall, T. R. (2011). Social learning mechanisms: Implications for a cognitive theory of imitation. Interaction Studies: Social Behaviour and Communication in Biological and Artificial Systems, 12, 233–261. doi: 10.1075/is.12.2.03zen Zentall, T. R. (2012). Perspectives on observational learning in animals. Journal of Comparative Psychology, ●●●, ●●●–●●●. Received February 9, 2012 Revision received February 9, 2012 Accepted February 10, 2012 Ⅲ 5SOCIAL LEARNING IN HUMANS AND NONHUMAN ANIMALS tapraid5/zct-com/zct-com/zct00212/zct2305d12z xppws Sϭ1 2/29/12 5:07 Art: 2012-0124