SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 14
Downloaden Sie, um offline zu lesen
Contemporary Educational Psychology 25, 54–67 (2000)
doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1020, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations: Classic Definitions and
New Directions
Richard M. Ryan and Edward L. Deci
University of Rochester
Intrinsic and extrinsic types of motivation have been widely studied, and the
distinction between them has shed important light on both developmental and educa-
tional practices. In this review we revisit the classic definitions of intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation in light of contemporary research and theory. Intrinsic motiva-
tion remains an important construct, reflecting the natural human propensity to learn
and assimilate. However, extrinsic motivation is argued to vary considerably in its
relative autonomy and thus can either reflect external control or true self-regulation.
The relations of both classes of motives to basic human needs for autonomy, compe-
tence and relatedness are discussed. © 2000 Academic Press
To be motivated means to be moved to do something. A person who feels
no impetus or inspiration to act is thus characterized as unmotivated, whereas
someone who is energized or activated toward an end is considered moti-
vated. Most everyone who works or plays with others is, accordingly, con-
cerned with motivation, facing the question of how much motivation those
others, or oneself, has for a task, and practitioners of all types face the peren-
nial task of fostering more versus less motivation in those around them. Most
theories of motivation reflect these concerns by viewing motivation as a uni-
tary phenomenon, one that varies from very little motivation to act to a great
deal of it.
Yet, even brief reflection suggests that motivation is hardly a unitary phe-
nomenon. People have not only different amounts, but also different kinds
of motivation. That is, they vary not only in level of motivation (i.e., how
much motivation), but also in the orientation of that motivation (i.e., what
type of motivation). Orientation of motivation concerns the underlying atti-
tudes and goals that give rise to action—that is, it concerns the why of ac-
tions. As an example, a student can be highly motivated to do homework
out of curiosity and interest or, alternatively, because he or she wants to
procure the approval of a teacher or parent. A student could be motivated
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Richard Ryan, University of Rochester,
Meliora 492, Rochester, NY 14627.
54
0361-476X/00 $35.00
Copyright © 2000 by Academic Press
All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 55
to learn a new set of skills because he or she understands their potential
utility or value or because learning the skills will yield a good grade and
the privileges a good grade affords. In these examples the amount of motiva-
tion does not necessarily vary, but the nature and focus of the motivation
being evidenced certainly does.
In Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985) we distinguish
between different types of motivation based on the different reasons or goals
that give rise to an action. The most basic distinction is between intrinsic
motivation, which refers to doing something because it is inherently interest-
ing or enjoyable, and extrinsic motivation, which refers to doing something
because it leads to a separable outcome. Over three decades of research has
shown that the quality of experience and performance can be very different
when one is behaving for intrinsic versus extrinsic reasons. One purpose of
this review is to revisit this classic distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation and to summarize the functional differences of these two general
types of motivation.
Intrinsic motivation has emerged as an important phenomena for educa-
tors—a natural wellspring of learning and achievement that can be systemati-
cally catalyzed or undermined by parent and teacher practices (Ryan &
Stiller, 1991). Because intrinsic motivation results in high-quality learning
and creativity, it is especially important to detail the factors and forces that
engender versus undermine it.
However, equally important in the current review is the explication of
the very different types of motivation that fall into the category of extrinsic
motivation. In the classic literature, extrinsic motivation has typically been
characterized as a pale and impoverished (even if powerful) form of motiva-
tion that contrasts with intrinsic motivation (e.g., deCharms, 1968). How-
ever, SDT proposes that there are varied types of extrinsic motivation, some
of which do, indeed, represent impoverished forms of motivation and some
of which represent active, agentic states.
Students can perform extrinsically motivated actions with resentment, re-
sistance, and disinterest or, alternatively, with an attitude of willingness that
reflects an inner acceptance of the value or utility of a task. In the former
case—the classic case of extrinsic motivation—one feels externally pro-
pelled into action; in the later case, the extrinsic goal is self-endorsed and
thus adopted with a sense of volition. Understanding these different types
of extrinsic motivation, and what fosters each of them, is an important issue
for educators who cannot always rely on intrinsic motivation to foster learn-
ing. Frankly speaking, because many of the tasks that educators want their
students to perform are not inherently interesting or enjoyable, knowing how
to promote more active and volitional (versus passive and controlling) forms
of extrinsic motivation becomes an essential strategy for successful teaching.
56 RYAN AND DECI
We detail in this article not only the different types of motivational orienta-
tion that exist within the global extrinsic category, but moreover, their differ-
ential antecedents and consequences.
In sum, our aim in this article is to revisit the classic distinction between
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and detail the conditions that fosters each.
Second, we describe a model of differing types of extrinsic motivation. Our
concern here is with how teachers, parents and other socializers can lead
students to internalize the responsibility and sense of value for extrinsic goals
or, alternatively, how they can foster the more typically depicted ‘‘alienated’’
type of extrinsic motivation that is associated with low student persistence,
interest, and involvement.
INTRINSIC MOTIVATION
Intrinsic motivation is defined as the doing of an activity for its inherent
satisfactions rather than for some separable consequence. When intrinsically
motivated a person is moved to act for the fun or challenge entailed rather
than because of external prods, pressures, or rewards. The phenomenon of
intrinsic motivation was first acknowledged within experimental studies of
animal behavior, where it was discovered that many organisms engage in
exploratory, playful, and curiosity-driven behaviors even in the absence of
reinforcement or reward (White, 1959). These spontaneous behaviors, al-
though clearly bestowing adaptive benefits on the organism, appear not to be
done for any such instrumental reason, but rather for the positive experiences
associated with exercising and extending ones capacities.
In humans, intrinsic motivation is not the only form of motivation, or even
of volitional activity, but it is a pervasive and important one. From birth
onward, humans, in their healthiest states, are active, inquisitive, curious,
and playful creatures, displaying a ubiquitous readiness to learn and explore,
and they do not require extraneous incentives to do so. This natural motiva-
tional tendency is a critical element in cognitive, social, and physical devel-
opment because it is through acting on one’s inherent interests that one grows
in knowledge and skills. The inclinations to take interest in novelty, to ac-
tively assimilate, and to creatively apply our skills is not limited to childhood,
but is a significant feature of human nature that affects performance, persis-
tence, and well-being across life’s epochs (Ryan & LaGuardia, in press).
Although, in one sense, intrinsic motivation exists within individuals, in
another sense intrinsic motivation exists in the relation between individuals
and activities. People are intrinsically motivated for some activities and not
others, and not everyone is intrinsically motivated for any particular task.
Because intrinsic motivation exists in the nexus between a person and a
task, some authors have defined intrinsic motivation in terms of the task
being interesting while others have defined it in terms of the satisfactions a
person gains from intrinsically motivated task engagement. In part, these
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 57
different definitions derive from the fact that the concept of intrinsic motiva-
tion was proposed as a critical reaction to the two behavioral theories that
were dominant in empirical psychology from the 1940s to the 1960s.
Specifically, because operant theory (Skinner, 1953) maintained that all
behaviors are motivated by rewards (i.e., by separable consequence such as
food or money), intrinsically motivated activities were said to be ones for
which the reward was in the activity itself. Thus, researchers investigated
what task characteristics make an activity interesting. In contrast, because
learning theory (Hull, 1943) asserted that all behaviors are motivated by
physiological drives (and their derivatives), intrinsically motivated activities
were said to be ones that provided satisfaction of innate psychological needs.
Thus, researchers explored what basic needs are satisfied by intrinsically
motivated behaviors.
Our own approach focuses primarily on psychological needs—namely,
the innate needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness—but we of
course recognize that basic need satisfaction accrues in part from engaging
in interesting activities. Thus, we do sometimes speak of intrinsically inter-
esting activities, but when we do so we are really only talking about tasks
that, on average, many people find to be intrinsically interesting. There is
considerable practical utility in focusing on task properties and their potential
intrinsic interest, as it leads toward improved task design or selection to
enhance motivation.
Operational Definitions
Intrinsic motivation has been operationally defined in various ways, al-
though there have been two measures that have been most often used. Basic
experimental research (e.g., Deci, 1971) has rested primarily on a behavioral
measure of intrinsic motivation called the ‘‘free choice’’ measure. In experi-
ments using this measure participants are exposed to a task under varying
conditions (e.g., getting a reward or not). Following this period, the experi-
menter tells participants they will not be asked to work with the target task
any further, and they are then left alone in the experimental room with the
target task as well as various distractor activities. They thus have a period
of ‘‘free choice’’ about whether to return to the activity, and it is assumed
that, if there is no extrinsic reason to do the task (e.g., no reward and no
approval), then the more time they spend with the target task, the more intrin-
sically motivated they are for that task. This measure has been the mainstay
through which the dynamics of intrinsic motivation have been experimen-
tally studied.
The other common approach to the measurement of intrinsic motivation
is the use of self-reports of interest and enjoyment of the activity per se.
Experimental studies typically rely on task-specific measures (e.g. Ryan,
1982; Harackiewicz, 1979). Most field studies have instead used more gen-
58 RYAN AND DECI
eral, ‘‘domain’’ focused measures, such as one’s intrinsic motivation for
school (e.g., Harter, 1981).
Facilitating versus Undermining Intrinsic Motivation
Despite the observable evidence that humans are liberally endowed with
intrinsic motivational tendencies, this propensity appears to be expressed
only under specifiable conditions. Research into intrinsic motivation has thus
placed much emphasis on those conditions that elicit, sustain, and enhance
this special type of motivation versus those that subdue or diminish it. Self-
Determination Theory is specifically framed in terms of social and environ-
mental factors that facilitate versus undermine intrinsic motivation. This lan-
guage reflects the assumption that intrinsic motivation, being an inherent
organismic propensity, is catalyzed (rather than caused) when individuals
are in conditions that conduce toward its expression.
Cognitive Evaluation Theory (CET) was presented by Deci and Ryan
(1985) to specify the factors in social contexts that produce variability in
intrinsic motivation. CET, which is considered a subtheory of self-determina-
tion theory, argues that interpersonal events and structures (e.g., rewards,
communications, feedback) that conduce toward feelings of competence dur-
ing action can enhance intrinsic motivation for that action because they allow
satisfaction of the basic psychological need for competence. Accordingly,
for example, optimal challenges, effectance promoting feedback, and free-
dom from demeaning evaluations are all predicted to facilitate intrinsic moti-
vation.
CET further specifies that feelings of competence will not enhance intrin-
sic motivation unless they are accompanied by a sense of autonomy or, in
attributional terms, by an internal perceived locus of causality (IPLOC; de-
Charms, 1968). Thus, people must not only experience perceived compe-
tence (or self-efficacy), they must also experience their behavior to be self-
determined if intrinsic motivation is to be maintained or enhanced. Stated
differently, for a high level of intrinsic motivation people must experience
satisfaction of the needs both for competence and autonomy. Much of the
research has focused on the effects of immediate contextual conditions that
either support or thwart the needs for competence and autonomy, but some
has recognized that the supports can, to some extent, come from individuals’
abiding inner resources that support their ongoing feelings of competence
and autonomy.
The tenets of CET, with their primary focus on the needs for competence
and autonomy, were formulated to integrate a set of results from initial stud-
ies of the effects of rewards, feedback, and other external events on intrinsic
motivation. Subsequently, they have been confirmed in both laboratory ex-
periments and applied field studies, many of which have been done in class-
rooms.
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 59
Several early studies showed that positive performance feedback enhanced
intrinsic motivation (e.g., Deci, 1971; Harackiewicz, 1979), whereas nega-
tive performance feedback diminished it (e.g., Deci & Cascio, 1972). Others
(e.g., Vallerand & Reid, 1984) showed that perceived competence mediated
these effects, and still others supported the hypothesis that increases in per-
ceived competence must be accompanied by a sense of autonomy in order
for the enhanced feelings of competence to result in increased intrinsic moti-
vation (Ryan, 1982).
In fact, the majority of the research on the effects of environmental events
on intrinsic motivation has focused on the issue of autonomy versus control
rather than that of competence. And this issue has been considerably more
controversial. The research began with the demonstration that extrinsic re-
wards can undermine intrinsic motivation (Deci, 1971; Lepper, Greene, &
Nisbett, 1973), which we interpret in terms of the reward shifting people
from a more internal to external perceived locus of causality. Although the
issue of rewards has been hotly debated, a recent meta-analysis (Deci, Koes-
tner, & Ryan, in press) confirms that virtually every type of expected tangible
reward made contingent on task performance does, in fact, undermine intrin-
sic motivation. Furthermore, not only tangible rewards, but also threats
(Deci & Cascio, 1972), deadlines (Amabile, DeJong, & Lepper, 1976), direc-
tives (Koestner, Ryan, Bernieri, & Holt, 1984), and competition pressure
(Reeve & Deci, 1996) diminish intrinsic motivation because, according to
CET, people experience them as controllers of their behavior. On the other
hand, choice and the opportunity for self-direction (e.g., Zuckerman, Porac,
Lathin, Smith, & Deci, 1978) appear to enhance intrinsic motivation, as they
afford a greater sense of autonomy.
The significance of autonomy versus control for the maintenance of intrin-
sic motivation has been clearly observed in studies of classroom learning.
For example, several studies have shown that autonomy-supportive (in con-
trast to controlling) teachers catalyze in their students greater intrinsic moti-
vation, curiosity, and the desire for challenge (e.g., Deci, Nezlek, & Shein-
man, 1981; Ryan & Grolnick, 1986). Students who are overly controlled not
only lose initiative but also learn less well, especially when learning is com-
plex or requires conceptual, creative processing (Benware & Deci, 1984;
Grolnick & Ryan, 1987). Similarly, studies show children of parents who
are more autonomy supportive to be more mastery oriented—more likely to
spontaneously explore and extend themselves—than children of parents who
are more controlling (Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, 1997).
To summarize, the CET aspect of SDT suggests that classroom and home
environments can facilitate or forestall intrinsic motivation by supporting
versus thwarting the needs for autonomy and competence. However, it is
critical to remember that intrinsic motivation will occur only for activities
that hold intrinsic interest for an individual—those that have the appeal of
60 RYAN AND DECI
novelty, challenge, or aesthetic value for that individual. For activities that
do not hold such appeal, the principles of CET do not apply. To understand
the motivation for activities that are not experienced as inherently interesting,
we need to look more deeply into the nature and dynamics of extrinsic moti-
vation.
EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION
Although intrinsic motivation is clearly an important type of motivation,
most of the activities people do are not, strictly speaking, intrinsically moti-
vated. This is especially the case after early childhood, as the freedom to be
intrinsically motivated becomes increasingly curtailed by social demands
and roles that require individuals to assume responsibility for nonintrinsically
interesting tasks. In schools, for example, it appears that intrinsic motivation
becomes weaker with each advancing grade.
Extrinsic motivation is a construct that pertains whenever an activity is
done in order to attain some separable outcome. Extrinsic motivation thus
contrasts with intrinsic motivation, which refers to doing an activity simply
for the enjoyment of the activity itself, rather than its instrumental value.
However, unlike some perspectives that view extrinsically motivated behav-
ior as invariantly nonautonomous, SDT proposes that extrinsic motivation
can vary greatly in the degree to which it is autonomous. For example, a
student who does his homework only because he fears parental sanctions for
not doing it is extrinsically motivated because he is doing the work in order
to attain the separable outcome of avoiding sanctions. Similarly, a student
who does the work because she personally believes it is valuable for her
chosen career is also extrinsically motivated because she too is doing it for its
instrumental value rather than because she finds it interesting. Both examples
involve instrumentalities, yet the latter case entails personal endorsement
and a feeling of choice, whereas the former involves mere compliance with
an external control. Both represent intentional behavior, but the two types
of extrinsic motivation vary in their relative autonomy.
Given that many of the educational activities prescribed in schools are not
designed to be intrinsically interesting, a central question concerns how to
motivate students to value and self-regulate such activities, and without ex-
ternal pressure, to carry them out on their own. This problem is described
within SDT in terms of fostering the internalization and integration of values
and behavioral regulations (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Internalization is the pro-
cess of taking in a value or regulation, and integration is the process by
which individuals more fully transform the regulation into their own so that
it will emanate from their sense of self. Thought of as a continuum, the
concept of internalization describes how one’s motivation for behavior can
range from amotivation or unwillingness, to passive compliance, to active
personal commitment. With increasing internalization (and its associated
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 61
FIG. 1. A taxonomy of human motivation.
sense of personal commitment) come greater persistence, more positive self-
perceptions, and better quality of engagement.
Within SDT a second subtheory, referred to as Organismic Integration
Theory (OIT), was introduced to detail the different forms of extrinsic moti-
vation and the contextual factors that either promote or hinder internalization
and integration of the regulation for these behaviors (Deci & Ryan, 1985).
Figure 1 illustrates the OIT taxonomy of types of motivation, arranged from
left to right in terms of the extent to which the motivation for one’s behavior
emanates from one’s self.
At the far left is amotivation, which is the state of lacking an intention to
act. When amotivated, a person’s behavior lacks intentionality and a sense
of personal causation. Amotivation results from not valuing an activity
(Ryan, 1995), not feeling competent to do it (Deci, 1975), or not believing
it will yield a desired outcome (Seligman, 1975). Theorists who have treated
motivation as a unitary concept (e.g., Bandura, 1986) have been concerned
only with the distinction between what we call amotivation and motivation.
However, one can see from Fig. 1 that to the right of amotivation are various
types of motivation that we have organized to reflect their differing degrees
of autonomy or self-determination.
Just to the right of amotivation, is a category that represents the least au-
tonomous forms of extrinsic motivation, a category we label external regula-
tion. Such behaviors are performed to satisfy an external demand or obtain
an externally imposed reward contingency. Individuals typically experience
externally regulated behavior as controlled or alienated, and their actions
62 RYAN AND DECI
have an external perceived locus of causality (EPLOC; deCharms, 1968).
External regulation is the only kind of motivation recognized by operant
theorists (e.g., Skinner, 1953), and it is this type of extrinsic motivation that
was typically contrasted with intrinsic motivation in early lab studies and
discussions.
A second type of extrinsic motivation is introjected regulation. Introjec-
tion describes a type of internal regulation that is still quite controlling be-
cause people perform such actions with the feeling of pressure in order to
avoid guilt or anxiety or to attain ego-enhancements or pride. Put differently,
introjection represents regulation by contingent self-esteem. A classic form
of introjection is ego involvement (Nicholls, 1984; Ryan, 1982), in which a
person performs an act in order to enhance or maintain self-esteem and the
feeling of worth. Although the regulation is internal to the person, introjected
behaviors are not experienced as fully part of the self and thus still have an
EPLOC.
A more autonomous, or self-determined, form of extrinsic motivation is
regulation through identification. Here, the person has identified with the
personal importance of a behavior and has thus accepted its regulation as
his or her own. A boy who memorizes spelling lists because he sees it as
relevant to writing, which he values as a life goal, has identified with the
value of this learning activity.
Finally, the most autonomous form of extrinsic motivation is integrated
regulation. Integration occurs when identified regulations have been fully
assimilated to the self. This occurs through self-examination and bringing
new regulations into congruence with one’s other values and needs. The
more one internalizes the reasons for an action and assimilates them to the
self, the more one’s extrinsically motivated actions become self-determined.
Integrated forms of motivation share many qualities with intrinsic motiva-
tion, being both autonomous and unconflicted. However, they are still extrin-
sic because behavior motivated by integrated regulation is done for its pre-
sumed instrumental value with respect to some outcome that is separate from
the behavior, even though it is volitional and valued by the self.
At the far right hand end of the figure is intrinsic motivation. This place-
ment emphasizes that intrinsic motivation is a prototype of self-determined
activity. Yet, as implied above, this does not mean that as extrinsic regula-
tions become more internalized they are transformed into intrinsic motiva-
tion.
The process of internalization is developmentally important, as social val-
ues and regulations are continually being internalized over the life span. Still,
we do not suggest that the continuum underlying types of extrinsic motiva-
tion is a developmental continuum, per se. One does not have to progress
through each stage of internalization with respect to a particular regulation;
indeed, one can initially adopt a new behavioral regulation at any point along
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 63
this continuum depending upon prior experiences and situational factors
(Ryan, 1995). Some behaviors could begin as introjects, others as identifica-
tions. A person might originally get exposed to an activity because of an
external regulation (e.g., a reward), and (if the reward is not perceived as
too controlling) such exposure might allow the person to experience the ac-
tivity’s intrinsically interesting properties, resulting in an orientation shift.
Or a person who has identified with the value of an activity might lose that
sense of value under a controlling mentor and move ‘‘backward’’ into an
external regulatory mode. Thus, while there are predictable reasons for
movement between orientations, there is no necessary ‘‘sequence.’’ Devel-
opmental issues are, however, evident in two ways: (1) the types of behaviors
and values that can be assimilated to the self increase with growing cognitive
and ego capacities and (2) it appears that people’s general regulatory style
does, on average, tend to become more ‘‘internal’’ over time (e.g., Chan-
dler & Connell, 1987), in accord with the general organismic tendencies
toward autonomy and self-regulation (Ryan, 1995).
Ryan and Connell (1989) tested the formulation that these different types
of motivation do indeed lie along a continuum of relative autonomy. They
investigated achievement behaviors (e.g., doing homework) among elemen-
tary school children, assessing external, introjected, identified, and intrinsic
reasons for engaging in these behaviors. They found that the four types of
regulation were intercorrelated according to a quasi-simplex (ordered corre-
lation) pattern, thus providing evidence for an underlying continuum of au-
tonomy. Differences in attitudes and adjustment were also associated with
the different types of extrinsic motivation. For example, the more students
were externally regulated the less they showed interest, value, or effort, and
the more they indicated a tendency to blame others, such as the teacher, for
negative outcomes. Introjected regulation was positively related to ex-
pending effort, but was also related to more anxiety and to poorer coping
with failures. Identified regulation was associated with greater enjoyment of
school and more positive coping styles. And intrinsic motivation was corre-
lated with interest, enjoyment, felt competence, and positive coping.
Subsequent studies have extended these findings concerning types of ex-
trinsic motivation, showing for example that more autonomous extrinsic mo-
tivation is associated with greater engagement (Connell & Wellborn, 1990),
better performance (Miserandino, 1996), less dropping out (Vallerand & Bis-
sonnette, 1992), higher quality learning (Grolnick & Ryan, 1987), and
greater psychological well-being (Sheldon & Kasser, 1995), among other
outcomes. Additionally, there appears to be cross-cultural generalizability to
the model as presented in Fig. 1 (e.g., Hayamizu, 1997).
Greater internalization appears, then, to yield manifold adaptive advan-
tages (Ryan, Kuhl, & Deci, 1997), including more behavioral effectiveness
(due to lessened conflict and greater access to personal resources) and greater
64 RYAN AND DECI
experienced well-being. Given the clear significance of internalization for
both personal experience and behavioral and performance outcomes, the crit-
ical applied issue concerns how to promote the autonomous regulation of
extrinsically motivated behaviors.
Because extrinsically motivated behaviors are not inherently interesting
and thus must initially be externally prompted, the primary reason people
are likely to be willing to do the behaviors is that they are valued by signifi-
cant others to whom they feel (or would like to feel) connected, whether
that be a family, a peer group, or a society. This suggests that the groundwork
for facilitating internalization is providing a sense of belongingness and con-
nectedness to the persons, group, or culture disseminating a goal, or what
in SDT we call a sense of relatedness. In classrooms this means that students’
feeling respected and cared for by the teacher is essential for their willingness
to accept the proffered classroom values. In support of this, Ryan, Stiller,
and Lynch (1994) found that relatedness to teachers (and parents) was associ-
ated with greater internalization of school-related behavioral regulations.
A second issue concerns perceived competence. Adopting as one’s own
an extrinsic goal requires that one feel efficacious with respect to it. Students
will more likely adopt and internalize a goal if they understand it and have
the relevant skills to succeed at it. Thus, we theorize that supports for compe-
tence (e.g., offering optimal challenges and effectance-relevant feedback)
facilitate internalization.
According to the SDT approach, a regulation that has been internalized
may be only introjected, and that type of regulation could well leave people
feeling satisfaction of their needs for competence and relatedness. However,
to only introject a regulation and thus to be controlled by it will not leave
the people feeling self-determined. We therefore suggest that autonomy sup-
port also facilitates internalization; in fact, it is the critical element for a
regulation being integrated rather than just introjected. Controlling contexts
may yield introjected regulation if they support competence and relatedness,
but only autonomy supportive contexts will yield integrated self-regulation.
To fully internalize a regulation, and thus to become autonomous with re-
spect to it, people must inwardly grasp its meaning and worth. It is these
meanings that become internalized and integrated in environments that pro-
vide supports for the needs for competence, relatedness, and autonomy.
Again, research has supported this reasoning. Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, and
Leone (1994) experimentally demonstrated that providing a meaningful ra-
tionale for an uninteresting behavior, along with supports for autonomy and
relatedness, promoted internalization and integration. Controlling contexts
yielded less overall internalization, but even more interesting, the internaliza-
tion that did occur in controlling contexts tended to be only introjected. In
a study involving parent interviews, Grolnick and Ryan (1989) found higher
levels of internalization and integration of school-related values among chil-
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 65
dren whose parents were more supportive of autonomy and relatedness. Wil-
liams and Deci (1996) used a longitudinal design to show greater internaliza-
tion among medical students whose instructors were more autonomy and
competence supportive. These are a few of the findings in this area that sug-
gest how supports for relatedness and competence facilitate internalization
and how support for autonomy additionally facilitates the integration of be-
havioral regulations. When that occurs, people not only feel competent and
related, but also self-determined, as they carry out extrinsically valued activi-
ties.
CONCLUSIONS
We have briefly presented self-determination theory in order to make the
critical distinction between behaviors that are volitional and accompanied
by the experience of freedom and autonomy—those that emanate from one’s
sense of self—and those that are accompanied by the experience of pressure
and control and are not representative of one’s self. Intrinsically motivated
behaviors, which are performed out of interest and satisfy the innate psycho-
logical needs for competence and autonomy are the prototype of self-deter-
mined behavior. Extrinsically motivated behaviors—those that are executed
because they are instrumental to some separable consequence—can vary in
the extent to which they represent self-determination. Internalization and in-
tegration are the processes through which extrinsically motivated behaviors
become more self-determined.
We reviewed studies that have specified the social contextual conditions
that support intrinsic motivation and facilitate internalization and integration
of extrinsically motivated tasks. The studies have been interpreted in terms
of the basic psychological needs. That is, we saw that social contextual con-
ditions that support one’s feelings of competence, autonomy, and relatedness
are the basis for one maintaining intrinsic motivation and becoming more
self-determined with respect to extrinsic motivation. We pointed out that in
schools, the facilitation of more self-determined learning requires classroom
conditions that allow satisfaction of these three basic human needs—that is
that support the innate needs to feel connected, effective, and agentic as one
is exposed to new ideas and exercises new skills.
REFERENCES
Amabile, T. M., DeJong, W., & Lepper, M. R. (1976). Effects of externally imposed deadlines
on subsequent intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34,
92–98.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice–Hall.
Benware, C., & Deci, E. L. (1984). Quality of learning with an active versus passive motiva-
tional set. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 755–765.
66 RYAN AND DECI
Chandler, C. L., & Connell, J. P. (1987). Children’s intrinsic, extrinsic and internalized motiva-
tion: A developmental study of children’s reasons for liked and disliked behaviours. Brit-
ish Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 357–365.
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1990). Competence, autonomy and relatedness: A motiva-
tional analysis of self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), The
Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 22, (pp. 43–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press.
Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105–115.
Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., & Cascio, W. F. (1972, April). Changes in intrinsic motivation as a function of
negative feedback and threats. Presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological
Association, Boston.
Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994). Facilitating internalization:
The self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119–142.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1998). Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation:
Clear and reliable effects. Unpublished manuscript, University of Rochester.
Deci, E. L., Nezlek, J., & Sheinman, L. (1981). Characteristics of the rewarder and intrinsic
motivation of the rewardee. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 1–10.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human
behavior. New York: Plenum.
Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997). Internalization within the family: The
selfdetermination perspective. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and
children’s internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 135–161).
New York: Wiley.
Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy in children’s learning: An experimental
and individual difference investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
52, 890–898.
Harackiewicz, J. (1979). The effects of reward contingency and performance feedback on
intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1352–1363.
Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the class-
room: Motivational and informational components. Developmental Psychology, 17, 300–
312.
Hayamizu, T. (1997). Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for
academic study based on the theory of internalization. Japanese Psychological Research,
39, 98–108.
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts.
Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. (1984). Setting limits on children’s behav-
ior: The differential effects of controlling versus informational styles on intrinsic motiva-
tion and creativity. Journal of Personality, 52, 233–248.
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest
with extrinsic rewards: A test of the ‘‘overjustification’’ hypothesis. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.
Miserandino, M. (1996). Children who do well in school: Individual differences in perceived
competence and autonomy in above-average children. Journal of Educational Psychol-
ogy, 88, 203–214.
INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 67
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience,
task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328–346.
Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Elements of the competitive situation that affect intrinsic
motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 24–33.
Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of
cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450–461.
Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal
of Personality, 63, 397–427.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examin-
ing reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
57, 749–761.
Ryan, R. M., & Grolnick, W. S. (1986). Origins and pawns in the classroom: Self-report
and projective assessments of individual differences in children’s perceptions. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 550–558.
Ryan, R. M., Kuhl, J., & Deci, E. L. (1997). Nature and autonomy: Organizational view
of social and neurobiological aspects of self-regulation in behavior and development.
Development and Psychopathology, 9, 701–728.
Ryan, R. M., & Stiller, J. (1991). The social contexts of internalization: Parent and teacher
influences on autonomy, motivation and learning. In P. R. Pintrich & M. L. Maehr (Eds.),
Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 115–149). Greenwich, CT: JAI
Press.
Ryan, R. M., Stiller, J., & Lynch, J. H. (1994). Representations of relationships to teachers,
parents, and friends as predictors of academic motivation and self-esteem. Journal of
Early Adolescence,, 14, 226–249.
Seligman, M. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco:
W. H. Freeman.
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1995). Coherence and congruence: Two aspects of personality
integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 531–543.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.
Vallerand, R. J., & Bissonnette, R. (1992). Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational styles as
predictors of behavior: A prospective study. Journal of Personality, 60, 599–620.
Vallerand, R. J., & Reid, G. (1984). On the causal effects of perceived competence on intrinsic
motivation: A test of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 94–
102.
White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.
Williams, G. C., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Internalization of biopsychosocial values by medical
students: A test of self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 70, 767–779.
Zuckerman, M., Porac, J., Lathin, D., Smith, R., & Deci, E. L. (1978). On the importance of
self-determination for intrinsically motivated behavior. Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy Bulletin, 4, 443–446.

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivationCognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivationEl Sameeha
 
Self determination theory
Self determination theorySelf determination theory
Self determination theoryJeannie Maraya
 
Importance of motivation in teaching and learning
Importance of motivation in teaching and learningImportance of motivation in teaching and learning
Importance of motivation in teaching and learningDr. Amjad Ali Arain
 
Theories of Motivation
Theories of MotivationTheories of Motivation
Theories of MotivationAndualem192
 
Meaning and Types of Motivation
Meaning and Types of MotivationMeaning and Types of Motivation
Meaning and Types of MotivationMarvin Ramirez
 
philosophical sociological psychological of curriculum development
philosophical sociological psychological of curriculum developmentphilosophical sociological psychological of curriculum development
philosophical sociological psychological of curriculum developmentANALUZFUENTEBELLA
 
Determinants of Aspirations
Determinants of AspirationsDeterminants of Aspirations
Determinants of AspirationsRodie Akerman
 
Psychologicalfoundationofeducation 130417163850-phpapp01
Psychologicalfoundationofeducation 130417163850-phpapp01Psychologicalfoundationofeducation 130417163850-phpapp01
Psychologicalfoundationofeducation 130417163850-phpapp01Sajida Rehman
 
Motivation and It's Theories.
Motivation and It's Theories.Motivation and It's Theories.
Motivation and It's Theories.Subrata Dey
 
Psychological Foundations of Education
Psychological Foundations of EducationPsychological Foundations of Education
Psychological Foundations of EducationRamil Gallardo
 

Was ist angesagt? (20)

Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivationCognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
Cognitive, Humanistic approach on motivation
 
Behavioral view of motivation
Behavioral view of motivationBehavioral view of motivation
Behavioral view of motivation
 
Self determination theory
Self determination theorySelf determination theory
Self determination theory
 
Chen presentation
Chen presentationChen presentation
Chen presentation
 
Importance of motivation in teaching and learning
Importance of motivation in teaching and learningImportance of motivation in teaching and learning
Importance of motivation in teaching and learning
 
Mini Grant Proposal
Mini Grant ProposalMini Grant Proposal
Mini Grant Proposal
 
Motivation
MotivationMotivation
Motivation
 
Theories of Motivation
Theories of MotivationTheories of Motivation
Theories of Motivation
 
Responsible Self
Responsible SelfResponsible Self
Responsible Self
 
Psychological foundation of education
Psychological foundation of education Psychological foundation of education
Psychological foundation of education
 
Meaning and Types of Motivation
Meaning and Types of MotivationMeaning and Types of Motivation
Meaning and Types of Motivation
 
philosophical sociological psychological of curriculum development
philosophical sociological psychological of curriculum developmentphilosophical sociological psychological of curriculum development
philosophical sociological psychological of curriculum development
 
Determinants of Aspirations
Determinants of AspirationsDeterminants of Aspirations
Determinants of Aspirations
 
Psychologicalfoundationofeducation 130417163850-phpapp01
Psychologicalfoundationofeducation 130417163850-phpapp01Psychologicalfoundationofeducation 130417163850-phpapp01
Psychologicalfoundationofeducation 130417163850-phpapp01
 
Concept map outline
Concept map outlineConcept map outline
Concept map outline
 
Concept map
Concept mapConcept map
Concept map
 
foundation of education
foundation of educationfoundation of education
foundation of education
 
Motivation and It's Theories.
Motivation and It's Theories.Motivation and It's Theories.
Motivation and It's Theories.
 
Psychological Foundations of Education
Psychological Foundations of EducationPsychological Foundations of Education
Psychological Foundations of Education
 
Qw
QwQw
Qw
 

Ähnlich wie Extrinsec & intrinsec

cognition and social neuroscience (pp. 350-367). New York, ;.docx
cognition and social neuroscience (pp. 350-367). New York, ;.docxcognition and social neuroscience (pp. 350-367). New York, ;.docx
cognition and social neuroscience (pp. 350-367). New York, ;.docxmary772
 
The_effects_of_motivation_in_education.pdf
The_effects_of_motivation_in_education.pdfThe_effects_of_motivation_in_education.pdf
The_effects_of_motivation_in_education.pdfJamieMorrissey6
 
Assessing academic intrinsic motivation
Assessing academic intrinsic motivationAssessing academic intrinsic motivation
Assessing academic intrinsic motivationmizah16
 
Motivation[1] juju
Motivation[1] jujuMotivation[1] juju
Motivation[1] jujujuraimiomar
 
The effects of motivation in education
The effects of motivation in educationThe effects of motivation in education
The effects of motivation in educationIgorOps1
 
Motivation and Learning
Motivation and LearningMotivation and Learning
Motivation and LearningRuth Ascuna
 
Leadership & motivation
Leadership & motivationLeadership & motivation
Leadership & motivationstudent
 
Paul Lawrence Intrinsic And Extrinsic Motivation
Paul Lawrence Intrinsic And Extrinsic MotivationPaul Lawrence Intrinsic And Extrinsic Motivation
Paul Lawrence Intrinsic And Extrinsic MotivationNicole Wells
 
Motivation and theories of motivation
Motivation and theories of motivationMotivation and theories of motivation
Motivation and theories of motivationAamir Ali
 
Motivation intrinsic extrinsic pres
Motivation intrinsic extrinsic presMotivation intrinsic extrinsic pres
Motivation intrinsic extrinsic presVirtu Institute
 
2000 ryan deci_sdt-bio-psycho
2000 ryan deci_sdt-bio-psycho2000 ryan deci_sdt-bio-psycho
2000 ryan deci_sdt-bio-psychojaysoncajate1
 
Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic .docx
Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic .docxSelf-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic .docx
Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic .docxbagotjesusa
 
Authentic-Assessment-of-the-Affective-Domain.pptx
Authentic-Assessment-of-the-Affective-Domain.pptxAuthentic-Assessment-of-the-Affective-Domain.pptx
Authentic-Assessment-of-the-Affective-Domain.pptxtalitejujee95
 
Motivation at work
Motivation at workMotivation at work
Motivation at workFiraKayanti
 
Motivation at work (8)
Motivation at work (8)Motivation at work (8)
Motivation at work (8)FirdhaAprivha
 
ducational Psychology provides an intern
ducational Psychology provides an internducational Psychology provides an intern
ducational Psychology provides an internNirmala Rothinam
 
Lect motivation intrinsic extrinsic july15.pptx
Lect motivation intrinsic extrinsic july15.pptxLect motivation intrinsic extrinsic july15.pptx
Lect motivation intrinsic extrinsic july15.pptxVirtu Institute
 

Ähnlich wie Extrinsec & intrinsec (20)

cognition and social neuroscience (pp. 350-367). New York, ;.docx
cognition and social neuroscience (pp. 350-367). New York, ;.docxcognition and social neuroscience (pp. 350-367). New York, ;.docx
cognition and social neuroscience (pp. 350-367). New York, ;.docx
 
Motivation.pptx
Motivation.pptxMotivation.pptx
Motivation.pptx
 
The_effects_of_motivation_in_education.pdf
The_effects_of_motivation_in_education.pdfThe_effects_of_motivation_in_education.pdf
The_effects_of_motivation_in_education.pdf
 
Assessing academic intrinsic motivation
Assessing academic intrinsic motivationAssessing academic intrinsic motivation
Assessing academic intrinsic motivation
 
Motivation[1] juju
Motivation[1] jujuMotivation[1] juju
Motivation[1] juju
 
The effects of motivation in education
The effects of motivation in educationThe effects of motivation in education
The effects of motivation in education
 
Motivation and Learning
Motivation and LearningMotivation and Learning
Motivation and Learning
 
Leadership & motivation
Leadership & motivationLeadership & motivation
Leadership & motivation
 
Paul Lawrence Intrinsic And Extrinsic Motivation
Paul Lawrence Intrinsic And Extrinsic MotivationPaul Lawrence Intrinsic And Extrinsic Motivation
Paul Lawrence Intrinsic And Extrinsic Motivation
 
Motivation and theories of motivation
Motivation and theories of motivationMotivation and theories of motivation
Motivation and theories of motivation
 
Motivation intrinsic extrinsic pres
Motivation intrinsic extrinsic presMotivation intrinsic extrinsic pres
Motivation intrinsic extrinsic pres
 
2000 ryan deci_sdt-bio-psycho
2000 ryan deci_sdt-bio-psycho2000 ryan deci_sdt-bio-psycho
2000 ryan deci_sdt-bio-psycho
 
Motivation
MotivationMotivation
Motivation
 
Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic .docx
Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic .docxSelf-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic .docx
Self-Determination Theory and the Facilitation of Intrinsic .docx
 
Authentic-Assessment-of-the-Affective-Domain.pptx
Authentic-Assessment-of-the-Affective-Domain.pptxAuthentic-Assessment-of-the-Affective-Domain.pptx
Authentic-Assessment-of-the-Affective-Domain.pptx
 
12 chapter 2
12 chapter 212 chapter 2
12 chapter 2
 
Motivation at work
Motivation at workMotivation at work
Motivation at work
 
Motivation at work (8)
Motivation at work (8)Motivation at work (8)
Motivation at work (8)
 
ducational Psychology provides an intern
ducational Psychology provides an internducational Psychology provides an intern
ducational Psychology provides an intern
 
Lect motivation intrinsic extrinsic july15.pptx
Lect motivation intrinsic extrinsic july15.pptxLect motivation intrinsic extrinsic july15.pptx
Lect motivation intrinsic extrinsic july15.pptx
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen

Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Mark Reed
 
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptxMusic 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptxleah joy valeriano
 
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYKayeClaireEstoconing
 
Integumentary System SMP B. Pharm Sem I.ppt
Integumentary System SMP B. Pharm Sem I.pptIntegumentary System SMP B. Pharm Sem I.ppt
Integumentary System SMP B. Pharm Sem I.pptshraddhaparab530
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONHumphrey A Beña
 
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdfVirtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdfErwinPantujan2
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
ANG SEKTOR NG agrikultura.pptx QUARTER 4
ANG SEKTOR NG agrikultura.pptx QUARTER 4ANG SEKTOR NG agrikultura.pptx QUARTER 4
ANG SEKTOR NG agrikultura.pptx QUARTER 4MiaBumagat1
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parentsnavabharathschool99
 
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptxBarangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptxCarlos105
 
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Seán Kennedy
 
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)lakshayb543
 
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...Postal Advocate Inc.
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxAnupkumar Sharma
 
ROLES IN A STAGE PRODUCTION in arts.pptx
ROLES IN A STAGE PRODUCTION in arts.pptxROLES IN A STAGE PRODUCTION in arts.pptx
ROLES IN A STAGE PRODUCTION in arts.pptxVanesaIglesias10
 

Kürzlich hochgeladen (20)

Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
 
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptxMusic 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
Music 9 - 4th quarter - Vocal Music of the Romantic Period.pptx
 
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxFINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
FINALS_OF_LEFT_ON_C'N_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITYISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
ISYU TUNGKOL SA SEKSWLADIDA (ISSUE ABOUT SEXUALITY
 
Integumentary System SMP B. Pharm Sem I.ppt
Integumentary System SMP B. Pharm Sem I.pptIntegumentary System SMP B. Pharm Sem I.ppt
Integumentary System SMP B. Pharm Sem I.ppt
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
 
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATIONTHEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
THEORIES OF ORGANIZATION-PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION
 
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdfVirtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
Virtual-Orientation-on-the-Administration-of-NATG12-NATG6-and-ELLNA.pdf
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
ANG SEKTOR NG agrikultura.pptx QUARTER 4
ANG SEKTOR NG agrikultura.pptx QUARTER 4ANG SEKTOR NG agrikultura.pptx QUARTER 4
ANG SEKTOR NG agrikultura.pptx QUARTER 4
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
 
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for ParentsChoosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
Choosing the Right CBSE School A Comprehensive Guide for Parents
 
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptxBarangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
Barangay Council for the Protection of Children (BCPC) Orientation.pptx
 
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
Student Profile Sample - We help schools to connect the data they have, with ...
 
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
How to Add Barcode on PDF Report in Odoo 17
 
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
Visit to a blind student's school🧑‍🦯🧑‍🦯(community medicine)
 
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptxYOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
YOUVE_GOT_EMAIL_PRELIMS_EL_DORADO_2024.pptx
 
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
USPS® Forced Meter Migration - How to Know if Your Postage Meter Will Soon be...
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
 
ROLES IN A STAGE PRODUCTION in arts.pptx
ROLES IN A STAGE PRODUCTION in arts.pptxROLES IN A STAGE PRODUCTION in arts.pptx
ROLES IN A STAGE PRODUCTION in arts.pptx
 

Extrinsec & intrinsec

  • 1. Contemporary Educational Psychology 25, 54–67 (2000) doi:10.1006/ceps.1999.1020, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivations: Classic Definitions and New Directions Richard M. Ryan and Edward L. Deci University of Rochester Intrinsic and extrinsic types of motivation have been widely studied, and the distinction between them has shed important light on both developmental and educa- tional practices. In this review we revisit the classic definitions of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in light of contemporary research and theory. Intrinsic motiva- tion remains an important construct, reflecting the natural human propensity to learn and assimilate. However, extrinsic motivation is argued to vary considerably in its relative autonomy and thus can either reflect external control or true self-regulation. The relations of both classes of motives to basic human needs for autonomy, compe- tence and relatedness are discussed. © 2000 Academic Press To be motivated means to be moved to do something. A person who feels no impetus or inspiration to act is thus characterized as unmotivated, whereas someone who is energized or activated toward an end is considered moti- vated. Most everyone who works or plays with others is, accordingly, con- cerned with motivation, facing the question of how much motivation those others, or oneself, has for a task, and practitioners of all types face the peren- nial task of fostering more versus less motivation in those around them. Most theories of motivation reflect these concerns by viewing motivation as a uni- tary phenomenon, one that varies from very little motivation to act to a great deal of it. Yet, even brief reflection suggests that motivation is hardly a unitary phe- nomenon. People have not only different amounts, but also different kinds of motivation. That is, they vary not only in level of motivation (i.e., how much motivation), but also in the orientation of that motivation (i.e., what type of motivation). Orientation of motivation concerns the underlying atti- tudes and goals that give rise to action—that is, it concerns the why of ac- tions. As an example, a student can be highly motivated to do homework out of curiosity and interest or, alternatively, because he or she wants to procure the approval of a teacher or parent. A student could be motivated Address correspondence and reprint requests to Richard Ryan, University of Rochester, Meliora 492, Rochester, NY 14627. 54 0361-476X/00 $35.00 Copyright © 2000 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
  • 2. INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 55 to learn a new set of skills because he or she understands their potential utility or value or because learning the skills will yield a good grade and the privileges a good grade affords. In these examples the amount of motiva- tion does not necessarily vary, but the nature and focus of the motivation being evidenced certainly does. In Self-Determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 1985) we distinguish between different types of motivation based on the different reasons or goals that give rise to an action. The most basic distinction is between intrinsic motivation, which refers to doing something because it is inherently interest- ing or enjoyable, and extrinsic motivation, which refers to doing something because it leads to a separable outcome. Over three decades of research has shown that the quality of experience and performance can be very different when one is behaving for intrinsic versus extrinsic reasons. One purpose of this review is to revisit this classic distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and to summarize the functional differences of these two general types of motivation. Intrinsic motivation has emerged as an important phenomena for educa- tors—a natural wellspring of learning and achievement that can be systemati- cally catalyzed or undermined by parent and teacher practices (Ryan & Stiller, 1991). Because intrinsic motivation results in high-quality learning and creativity, it is especially important to detail the factors and forces that engender versus undermine it. However, equally important in the current review is the explication of the very different types of motivation that fall into the category of extrinsic motivation. In the classic literature, extrinsic motivation has typically been characterized as a pale and impoverished (even if powerful) form of motiva- tion that contrasts with intrinsic motivation (e.g., deCharms, 1968). How- ever, SDT proposes that there are varied types of extrinsic motivation, some of which do, indeed, represent impoverished forms of motivation and some of which represent active, agentic states. Students can perform extrinsically motivated actions with resentment, re- sistance, and disinterest or, alternatively, with an attitude of willingness that reflects an inner acceptance of the value or utility of a task. In the former case—the classic case of extrinsic motivation—one feels externally pro- pelled into action; in the later case, the extrinsic goal is self-endorsed and thus adopted with a sense of volition. Understanding these different types of extrinsic motivation, and what fosters each of them, is an important issue for educators who cannot always rely on intrinsic motivation to foster learn- ing. Frankly speaking, because many of the tasks that educators want their students to perform are not inherently interesting or enjoyable, knowing how to promote more active and volitional (versus passive and controlling) forms of extrinsic motivation becomes an essential strategy for successful teaching.
  • 3. 56 RYAN AND DECI We detail in this article not only the different types of motivational orienta- tion that exist within the global extrinsic category, but moreover, their differ- ential antecedents and consequences. In sum, our aim in this article is to revisit the classic distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation and detail the conditions that fosters each. Second, we describe a model of differing types of extrinsic motivation. Our concern here is with how teachers, parents and other socializers can lead students to internalize the responsibility and sense of value for extrinsic goals or, alternatively, how they can foster the more typically depicted ‘‘alienated’’ type of extrinsic motivation that is associated with low student persistence, interest, and involvement. INTRINSIC MOTIVATION Intrinsic motivation is defined as the doing of an activity for its inherent satisfactions rather than for some separable consequence. When intrinsically motivated a person is moved to act for the fun or challenge entailed rather than because of external prods, pressures, or rewards. The phenomenon of intrinsic motivation was first acknowledged within experimental studies of animal behavior, where it was discovered that many organisms engage in exploratory, playful, and curiosity-driven behaviors even in the absence of reinforcement or reward (White, 1959). These spontaneous behaviors, al- though clearly bestowing adaptive benefits on the organism, appear not to be done for any such instrumental reason, but rather for the positive experiences associated with exercising and extending ones capacities. In humans, intrinsic motivation is not the only form of motivation, or even of volitional activity, but it is a pervasive and important one. From birth onward, humans, in their healthiest states, are active, inquisitive, curious, and playful creatures, displaying a ubiquitous readiness to learn and explore, and they do not require extraneous incentives to do so. This natural motiva- tional tendency is a critical element in cognitive, social, and physical devel- opment because it is through acting on one’s inherent interests that one grows in knowledge and skills. The inclinations to take interest in novelty, to ac- tively assimilate, and to creatively apply our skills is not limited to childhood, but is a significant feature of human nature that affects performance, persis- tence, and well-being across life’s epochs (Ryan & LaGuardia, in press). Although, in one sense, intrinsic motivation exists within individuals, in another sense intrinsic motivation exists in the relation between individuals and activities. People are intrinsically motivated for some activities and not others, and not everyone is intrinsically motivated for any particular task. Because intrinsic motivation exists in the nexus between a person and a task, some authors have defined intrinsic motivation in terms of the task being interesting while others have defined it in terms of the satisfactions a person gains from intrinsically motivated task engagement. In part, these
  • 4. INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 57 different definitions derive from the fact that the concept of intrinsic motiva- tion was proposed as a critical reaction to the two behavioral theories that were dominant in empirical psychology from the 1940s to the 1960s. Specifically, because operant theory (Skinner, 1953) maintained that all behaviors are motivated by rewards (i.e., by separable consequence such as food or money), intrinsically motivated activities were said to be ones for which the reward was in the activity itself. Thus, researchers investigated what task characteristics make an activity interesting. In contrast, because learning theory (Hull, 1943) asserted that all behaviors are motivated by physiological drives (and their derivatives), intrinsically motivated activities were said to be ones that provided satisfaction of innate psychological needs. Thus, researchers explored what basic needs are satisfied by intrinsically motivated behaviors. Our own approach focuses primarily on psychological needs—namely, the innate needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness—but we of course recognize that basic need satisfaction accrues in part from engaging in interesting activities. Thus, we do sometimes speak of intrinsically inter- esting activities, but when we do so we are really only talking about tasks that, on average, many people find to be intrinsically interesting. There is considerable practical utility in focusing on task properties and their potential intrinsic interest, as it leads toward improved task design or selection to enhance motivation. Operational Definitions Intrinsic motivation has been operationally defined in various ways, al- though there have been two measures that have been most often used. Basic experimental research (e.g., Deci, 1971) has rested primarily on a behavioral measure of intrinsic motivation called the ‘‘free choice’’ measure. In experi- ments using this measure participants are exposed to a task under varying conditions (e.g., getting a reward or not). Following this period, the experi- menter tells participants they will not be asked to work with the target task any further, and they are then left alone in the experimental room with the target task as well as various distractor activities. They thus have a period of ‘‘free choice’’ about whether to return to the activity, and it is assumed that, if there is no extrinsic reason to do the task (e.g., no reward and no approval), then the more time they spend with the target task, the more intrin- sically motivated they are for that task. This measure has been the mainstay through which the dynamics of intrinsic motivation have been experimen- tally studied. The other common approach to the measurement of intrinsic motivation is the use of self-reports of interest and enjoyment of the activity per se. Experimental studies typically rely on task-specific measures (e.g. Ryan, 1982; Harackiewicz, 1979). Most field studies have instead used more gen-
  • 5. 58 RYAN AND DECI eral, ‘‘domain’’ focused measures, such as one’s intrinsic motivation for school (e.g., Harter, 1981). Facilitating versus Undermining Intrinsic Motivation Despite the observable evidence that humans are liberally endowed with intrinsic motivational tendencies, this propensity appears to be expressed only under specifiable conditions. Research into intrinsic motivation has thus placed much emphasis on those conditions that elicit, sustain, and enhance this special type of motivation versus those that subdue or diminish it. Self- Determination Theory is specifically framed in terms of social and environ- mental factors that facilitate versus undermine intrinsic motivation. This lan- guage reflects the assumption that intrinsic motivation, being an inherent organismic propensity, is catalyzed (rather than caused) when individuals are in conditions that conduce toward its expression. Cognitive Evaluation Theory (CET) was presented by Deci and Ryan (1985) to specify the factors in social contexts that produce variability in intrinsic motivation. CET, which is considered a subtheory of self-determina- tion theory, argues that interpersonal events and structures (e.g., rewards, communications, feedback) that conduce toward feelings of competence dur- ing action can enhance intrinsic motivation for that action because they allow satisfaction of the basic psychological need for competence. Accordingly, for example, optimal challenges, effectance promoting feedback, and free- dom from demeaning evaluations are all predicted to facilitate intrinsic moti- vation. CET further specifies that feelings of competence will not enhance intrin- sic motivation unless they are accompanied by a sense of autonomy or, in attributional terms, by an internal perceived locus of causality (IPLOC; de- Charms, 1968). Thus, people must not only experience perceived compe- tence (or self-efficacy), they must also experience their behavior to be self- determined if intrinsic motivation is to be maintained or enhanced. Stated differently, for a high level of intrinsic motivation people must experience satisfaction of the needs both for competence and autonomy. Much of the research has focused on the effects of immediate contextual conditions that either support or thwart the needs for competence and autonomy, but some has recognized that the supports can, to some extent, come from individuals’ abiding inner resources that support their ongoing feelings of competence and autonomy. The tenets of CET, with their primary focus on the needs for competence and autonomy, were formulated to integrate a set of results from initial stud- ies of the effects of rewards, feedback, and other external events on intrinsic motivation. Subsequently, they have been confirmed in both laboratory ex- periments and applied field studies, many of which have been done in class- rooms.
  • 6. INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 59 Several early studies showed that positive performance feedback enhanced intrinsic motivation (e.g., Deci, 1971; Harackiewicz, 1979), whereas nega- tive performance feedback diminished it (e.g., Deci & Cascio, 1972). Others (e.g., Vallerand & Reid, 1984) showed that perceived competence mediated these effects, and still others supported the hypothesis that increases in per- ceived competence must be accompanied by a sense of autonomy in order for the enhanced feelings of competence to result in increased intrinsic moti- vation (Ryan, 1982). In fact, the majority of the research on the effects of environmental events on intrinsic motivation has focused on the issue of autonomy versus control rather than that of competence. And this issue has been considerably more controversial. The research began with the demonstration that extrinsic re- wards can undermine intrinsic motivation (Deci, 1971; Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973), which we interpret in terms of the reward shifting people from a more internal to external perceived locus of causality. Although the issue of rewards has been hotly debated, a recent meta-analysis (Deci, Koes- tner, & Ryan, in press) confirms that virtually every type of expected tangible reward made contingent on task performance does, in fact, undermine intrin- sic motivation. Furthermore, not only tangible rewards, but also threats (Deci & Cascio, 1972), deadlines (Amabile, DeJong, & Lepper, 1976), direc- tives (Koestner, Ryan, Bernieri, & Holt, 1984), and competition pressure (Reeve & Deci, 1996) diminish intrinsic motivation because, according to CET, people experience them as controllers of their behavior. On the other hand, choice and the opportunity for self-direction (e.g., Zuckerman, Porac, Lathin, Smith, & Deci, 1978) appear to enhance intrinsic motivation, as they afford a greater sense of autonomy. The significance of autonomy versus control for the maintenance of intrin- sic motivation has been clearly observed in studies of classroom learning. For example, several studies have shown that autonomy-supportive (in con- trast to controlling) teachers catalyze in their students greater intrinsic moti- vation, curiosity, and the desire for challenge (e.g., Deci, Nezlek, & Shein- man, 1981; Ryan & Grolnick, 1986). Students who are overly controlled not only lose initiative but also learn less well, especially when learning is com- plex or requires conceptual, creative processing (Benware & Deci, 1984; Grolnick & Ryan, 1987). Similarly, studies show children of parents who are more autonomy supportive to be more mastery oriented—more likely to spontaneously explore and extend themselves—than children of parents who are more controlling (Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, 1997). To summarize, the CET aspect of SDT suggests that classroom and home environments can facilitate or forestall intrinsic motivation by supporting versus thwarting the needs for autonomy and competence. However, it is critical to remember that intrinsic motivation will occur only for activities that hold intrinsic interest for an individual—those that have the appeal of
  • 7. 60 RYAN AND DECI novelty, challenge, or aesthetic value for that individual. For activities that do not hold such appeal, the principles of CET do not apply. To understand the motivation for activities that are not experienced as inherently interesting, we need to look more deeply into the nature and dynamics of extrinsic moti- vation. EXTRINSIC MOTIVATION Although intrinsic motivation is clearly an important type of motivation, most of the activities people do are not, strictly speaking, intrinsically moti- vated. This is especially the case after early childhood, as the freedom to be intrinsically motivated becomes increasingly curtailed by social demands and roles that require individuals to assume responsibility for nonintrinsically interesting tasks. In schools, for example, it appears that intrinsic motivation becomes weaker with each advancing grade. Extrinsic motivation is a construct that pertains whenever an activity is done in order to attain some separable outcome. Extrinsic motivation thus contrasts with intrinsic motivation, which refers to doing an activity simply for the enjoyment of the activity itself, rather than its instrumental value. However, unlike some perspectives that view extrinsically motivated behav- ior as invariantly nonautonomous, SDT proposes that extrinsic motivation can vary greatly in the degree to which it is autonomous. For example, a student who does his homework only because he fears parental sanctions for not doing it is extrinsically motivated because he is doing the work in order to attain the separable outcome of avoiding sanctions. Similarly, a student who does the work because she personally believes it is valuable for her chosen career is also extrinsically motivated because she too is doing it for its instrumental value rather than because she finds it interesting. Both examples involve instrumentalities, yet the latter case entails personal endorsement and a feeling of choice, whereas the former involves mere compliance with an external control. Both represent intentional behavior, but the two types of extrinsic motivation vary in their relative autonomy. Given that many of the educational activities prescribed in schools are not designed to be intrinsically interesting, a central question concerns how to motivate students to value and self-regulate such activities, and without ex- ternal pressure, to carry them out on their own. This problem is described within SDT in terms of fostering the internalization and integration of values and behavioral regulations (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Internalization is the pro- cess of taking in a value or regulation, and integration is the process by which individuals more fully transform the regulation into their own so that it will emanate from their sense of self. Thought of as a continuum, the concept of internalization describes how one’s motivation for behavior can range from amotivation or unwillingness, to passive compliance, to active personal commitment. With increasing internalization (and its associated
  • 8. INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 61 FIG. 1. A taxonomy of human motivation. sense of personal commitment) come greater persistence, more positive self- perceptions, and better quality of engagement. Within SDT a second subtheory, referred to as Organismic Integration Theory (OIT), was introduced to detail the different forms of extrinsic moti- vation and the contextual factors that either promote or hinder internalization and integration of the regulation for these behaviors (Deci & Ryan, 1985). Figure 1 illustrates the OIT taxonomy of types of motivation, arranged from left to right in terms of the extent to which the motivation for one’s behavior emanates from one’s self. At the far left is amotivation, which is the state of lacking an intention to act. When amotivated, a person’s behavior lacks intentionality and a sense of personal causation. Amotivation results from not valuing an activity (Ryan, 1995), not feeling competent to do it (Deci, 1975), or not believing it will yield a desired outcome (Seligman, 1975). Theorists who have treated motivation as a unitary concept (e.g., Bandura, 1986) have been concerned only with the distinction between what we call amotivation and motivation. However, one can see from Fig. 1 that to the right of amotivation are various types of motivation that we have organized to reflect their differing degrees of autonomy or self-determination. Just to the right of amotivation, is a category that represents the least au- tonomous forms of extrinsic motivation, a category we label external regula- tion. Such behaviors are performed to satisfy an external demand or obtain an externally imposed reward contingency. Individuals typically experience externally regulated behavior as controlled or alienated, and their actions
  • 9. 62 RYAN AND DECI have an external perceived locus of causality (EPLOC; deCharms, 1968). External regulation is the only kind of motivation recognized by operant theorists (e.g., Skinner, 1953), and it is this type of extrinsic motivation that was typically contrasted with intrinsic motivation in early lab studies and discussions. A second type of extrinsic motivation is introjected regulation. Introjec- tion describes a type of internal regulation that is still quite controlling be- cause people perform such actions with the feeling of pressure in order to avoid guilt or anxiety or to attain ego-enhancements or pride. Put differently, introjection represents regulation by contingent self-esteem. A classic form of introjection is ego involvement (Nicholls, 1984; Ryan, 1982), in which a person performs an act in order to enhance or maintain self-esteem and the feeling of worth. Although the regulation is internal to the person, introjected behaviors are not experienced as fully part of the self and thus still have an EPLOC. A more autonomous, or self-determined, form of extrinsic motivation is regulation through identification. Here, the person has identified with the personal importance of a behavior and has thus accepted its regulation as his or her own. A boy who memorizes spelling lists because he sees it as relevant to writing, which he values as a life goal, has identified with the value of this learning activity. Finally, the most autonomous form of extrinsic motivation is integrated regulation. Integration occurs when identified regulations have been fully assimilated to the self. This occurs through self-examination and bringing new regulations into congruence with one’s other values and needs. The more one internalizes the reasons for an action and assimilates them to the self, the more one’s extrinsically motivated actions become self-determined. Integrated forms of motivation share many qualities with intrinsic motiva- tion, being both autonomous and unconflicted. However, they are still extrin- sic because behavior motivated by integrated regulation is done for its pre- sumed instrumental value with respect to some outcome that is separate from the behavior, even though it is volitional and valued by the self. At the far right hand end of the figure is intrinsic motivation. This place- ment emphasizes that intrinsic motivation is a prototype of self-determined activity. Yet, as implied above, this does not mean that as extrinsic regula- tions become more internalized they are transformed into intrinsic motiva- tion. The process of internalization is developmentally important, as social val- ues and regulations are continually being internalized over the life span. Still, we do not suggest that the continuum underlying types of extrinsic motiva- tion is a developmental continuum, per se. One does not have to progress through each stage of internalization with respect to a particular regulation; indeed, one can initially adopt a new behavioral regulation at any point along
  • 10. INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 63 this continuum depending upon prior experiences and situational factors (Ryan, 1995). Some behaviors could begin as introjects, others as identifica- tions. A person might originally get exposed to an activity because of an external regulation (e.g., a reward), and (if the reward is not perceived as too controlling) such exposure might allow the person to experience the ac- tivity’s intrinsically interesting properties, resulting in an orientation shift. Or a person who has identified with the value of an activity might lose that sense of value under a controlling mentor and move ‘‘backward’’ into an external regulatory mode. Thus, while there are predictable reasons for movement between orientations, there is no necessary ‘‘sequence.’’ Devel- opmental issues are, however, evident in two ways: (1) the types of behaviors and values that can be assimilated to the self increase with growing cognitive and ego capacities and (2) it appears that people’s general regulatory style does, on average, tend to become more ‘‘internal’’ over time (e.g., Chan- dler & Connell, 1987), in accord with the general organismic tendencies toward autonomy and self-regulation (Ryan, 1995). Ryan and Connell (1989) tested the formulation that these different types of motivation do indeed lie along a continuum of relative autonomy. They investigated achievement behaviors (e.g., doing homework) among elemen- tary school children, assessing external, introjected, identified, and intrinsic reasons for engaging in these behaviors. They found that the four types of regulation were intercorrelated according to a quasi-simplex (ordered corre- lation) pattern, thus providing evidence for an underlying continuum of au- tonomy. Differences in attitudes and adjustment were also associated with the different types of extrinsic motivation. For example, the more students were externally regulated the less they showed interest, value, or effort, and the more they indicated a tendency to blame others, such as the teacher, for negative outcomes. Introjected regulation was positively related to ex- pending effort, but was also related to more anxiety and to poorer coping with failures. Identified regulation was associated with greater enjoyment of school and more positive coping styles. And intrinsic motivation was corre- lated with interest, enjoyment, felt competence, and positive coping. Subsequent studies have extended these findings concerning types of ex- trinsic motivation, showing for example that more autonomous extrinsic mo- tivation is associated with greater engagement (Connell & Wellborn, 1990), better performance (Miserandino, 1996), less dropping out (Vallerand & Bis- sonnette, 1992), higher quality learning (Grolnick & Ryan, 1987), and greater psychological well-being (Sheldon & Kasser, 1995), among other outcomes. Additionally, there appears to be cross-cultural generalizability to the model as presented in Fig. 1 (e.g., Hayamizu, 1997). Greater internalization appears, then, to yield manifold adaptive advan- tages (Ryan, Kuhl, & Deci, 1997), including more behavioral effectiveness (due to lessened conflict and greater access to personal resources) and greater
  • 11. 64 RYAN AND DECI experienced well-being. Given the clear significance of internalization for both personal experience and behavioral and performance outcomes, the crit- ical applied issue concerns how to promote the autonomous regulation of extrinsically motivated behaviors. Because extrinsically motivated behaviors are not inherently interesting and thus must initially be externally prompted, the primary reason people are likely to be willing to do the behaviors is that they are valued by signifi- cant others to whom they feel (or would like to feel) connected, whether that be a family, a peer group, or a society. This suggests that the groundwork for facilitating internalization is providing a sense of belongingness and con- nectedness to the persons, group, or culture disseminating a goal, or what in SDT we call a sense of relatedness. In classrooms this means that students’ feeling respected and cared for by the teacher is essential for their willingness to accept the proffered classroom values. In support of this, Ryan, Stiller, and Lynch (1994) found that relatedness to teachers (and parents) was associ- ated with greater internalization of school-related behavioral regulations. A second issue concerns perceived competence. Adopting as one’s own an extrinsic goal requires that one feel efficacious with respect to it. Students will more likely adopt and internalize a goal if they understand it and have the relevant skills to succeed at it. Thus, we theorize that supports for compe- tence (e.g., offering optimal challenges and effectance-relevant feedback) facilitate internalization. According to the SDT approach, a regulation that has been internalized may be only introjected, and that type of regulation could well leave people feeling satisfaction of their needs for competence and relatedness. However, to only introject a regulation and thus to be controlled by it will not leave the people feeling self-determined. We therefore suggest that autonomy sup- port also facilitates internalization; in fact, it is the critical element for a regulation being integrated rather than just introjected. Controlling contexts may yield introjected regulation if they support competence and relatedness, but only autonomy supportive contexts will yield integrated self-regulation. To fully internalize a regulation, and thus to become autonomous with re- spect to it, people must inwardly grasp its meaning and worth. It is these meanings that become internalized and integrated in environments that pro- vide supports for the needs for competence, relatedness, and autonomy. Again, research has supported this reasoning. Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, and Leone (1994) experimentally demonstrated that providing a meaningful ra- tionale for an uninteresting behavior, along with supports for autonomy and relatedness, promoted internalization and integration. Controlling contexts yielded less overall internalization, but even more interesting, the internaliza- tion that did occur in controlling contexts tended to be only introjected. In a study involving parent interviews, Grolnick and Ryan (1989) found higher levels of internalization and integration of school-related values among chil-
  • 12. INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 65 dren whose parents were more supportive of autonomy and relatedness. Wil- liams and Deci (1996) used a longitudinal design to show greater internaliza- tion among medical students whose instructors were more autonomy and competence supportive. These are a few of the findings in this area that sug- gest how supports for relatedness and competence facilitate internalization and how support for autonomy additionally facilitates the integration of be- havioral regulations. When that occurs, people not only feel competent and related, but also self-determined, as they carry out extrinsically valued activi- ties. CONCLUSIONS We have briefly presented self-determination theory in order to make the critical distinction between behaviors that are volitional and accompanied by the experience of freedom and autonomy—those that emanate from one’s sense of self—and those that are accompanied by the experience of pressure and control and are not representative of one’s self. Intrinsically motivated behaviors, which are performed out of interest and satisfy the innate psycho- logical needs for competence and autonomy are the prototype of self-deter- mined behavior. Extrinsically motivated behaviors—those that are executed because they are instrumental to some separable consequence—can vary in the extent to which they represent self-determination. Internalization and in- tegration are the processes through which extrinsically motivated behaviors become more self-determined. We reviewed studies that have specified the social contextual conditions that support intrinsic motivation and facilitate internalization and integration of extrinsically motivated tasks. The studies have been interpreted in terms of the basic psychological needs. That is, we saw that social contextual con- ditions that support one’s feelings of competence, autonomy, and relatedness are the basis for one maintaining intrinsic motivation and becoming more self-determined with respect to extrinsic motivation. We pointed out that in schools, the facilitation of more self-determined learning requires classroom conditions that allow satisfaction of these three basic human needs—that is that support the innate needs to feel connected, effective, and agentic as one is exposed to new ideas and exercises new skills. REFERENCES Amabile, T. M., DeJong, W., & Lepper, M. R. (1976). Effects of externally imposed deadlines on subsequent intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 92–98. Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Engle- wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice–Hall. Benware, C., & Deci, E. L. (1984). Quality of learning with an active versus passive motiva- tional set. American Educational Research Journal, 21, 755–765.
  • 13. 66 RYAN AND DECI Chandler, C. L., & Connell, J. P. (1987). Children’s intrinsic, extrinsic and internalized motiva- tion: A developmental study of children’s reasons for liked and disliked behaviours. Brit- ish Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 357–365. Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1990). Competence, autonomy and relatedness: A motiva- tional analysis of self-system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), The Minnesota symposium on child psychology (Vol. 22, (pp. 43–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic Press. Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 105–115. Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum. Deci, E. L., & Cascio, W. F. (1972, April). Changes in intrinsic motivation as a function of negative feedback and threats. Presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston. Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994). Facilitating internalization: The self-determination theory perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119–142. Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1998). Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation: Clear and reliable effects. Unpublished manuscript, University of Rochester. Deci, E. L., Nezlek, J., & Sheinman, L. (1981). Characteristics of the rewarder and intrinsic motivation of the rewardee. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 1–10. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum. Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997). Internalization within the family: The selfdetermination perspective. In J. E. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and children’s internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary theory (pp. 135–161). New York: Wiley. Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy in children’s learning: An experimental and individual difference investigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 890–898. Harackiewicz, J. (1979). The effects of reward contingency and performance feedback on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1352–1363. Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the class- room: Motivational and informational components. Developmental Psychology, 17, 300– 312. Hayamizu, T. (1997). Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for academic study based on the theory of internalization. Japanese Psychological Research, 39, 98–108. Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts. Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. (1984). Setting limits on children’s behav- ior: The differential effects of controlling versus informational styles on intrinsic motiva- tion and creativity. Journal of Personality, 52, 233–248. Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the ‘‘overjustification’’ hypothesis. Journal of Personal- ity and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137. Miserandino, M. (1996). Children who do well in school: Individual differences in perceived competence and autonomy in above-average children. Journal of Educational Psychol- ogy, 88, 203–214.
  • 14. INTRINSIC AND EXTRINSIC MOTIVATIONS 67 Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Psychological Review, 91, 328–346. Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Elements of the competitive situation that affect intrinsic motivation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 24–33. Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450–461. Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal of Personality, 63, 397–427. Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of causality and internalization: Examin- ing reasons for acting in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 749–761. Ryan, R. M., & Grolnick, W. S. (1986). Origins and pawns in the classroom: Self-report and projective assessments of individual differences in children’s perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 550–558. Ryan, R. M., Kuhl, J., & Deci, E. L. (1997). Nature and autonomy: Organizational view of social and neurobiological aspects of self-regulation in behavior and development. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 701–728. Ryan, R. M., & Stiller, J. (1991). The social contexts of internalization: Parent and teacher influences on autonomy, motivation and learning. In P. R. Pintrich & M. L. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 115–149). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Ryan, R. M., Stiller, J., & Lynch, J. H. (1994). Representations of relationships to teachers, parents, and friends as predictors of academic motivation and self-esteem. Journal of Early Adolescence,, 14, 226–249. Seligman, M. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1995). Coherence and congruence: Two aspects of personality integration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 531–543. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan. Vallerand, R. J., & Bissonnette, R. (1992). Intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivational styles as predictors of behavior: A prospective study. Journal of Personality, 60, 599–620. Vallerand, R. J., & Reid, G. (1984). On the causal effects of perceived competence on intrinsic motivation: A test of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Sport Psychology, 6, 94– 102. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333. Williams, G. C., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Internalization of biopsychosocial values by medical students: A test of self-determination theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 70, 767–779. Zuckerman, M., Porac, J., Lathin, D., Smith, R., & Deci, E. L. (1978). On the importance of self-determination for intrinsically motivated behavior. Personality and Social Psychol- ogy Bulletin, 4, 443–446.