SlideShare ist ein Scribd-Unternehmen logo
1 von 31
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 1



Running Head: A COMPARISON OF PROMPTING STRATEGIES




                  A Comparison of Prompting Strategies in an

                Early Childhood Developmental Delay Classroom

                            Kristen Lynn Gaisford

                         Western Michigan University
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 2


                                             Abstract

       The design of the current study was to compare two prompting strategies; most to least

prompting (MTL) and least to most prompting (LTM). These two strategies were compared

using a multi-element design, assessing the performance of three children, ranging from 2 to 4

years of age. These children were selected from a classroom that provides services to children

with Early Childhood Developmental Delays (ECDD). In order to compare the MTL and LTM

strategies, the three children were taught identical three-dimensional matching. There was no

demonstrated difference between strategies in terms of effectiveness or efficiency. While it was

not the focus of the study, generalized matching data are also presented for all three children.

All three children acquired a generalized identical-object matching repertoire. Additionally, two

of the children had a history of emotional responding prior to this study, yet displayed almost no

emotional responding during the study, suggesting that the reinforcement of prompted responses

was the reason for the absence of emotional responding. The study took place at Croyden

Avenue School, in Kalamazoo, Michigan.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 3


A Comparison of Prompting Strategies in an Early Childhood Developmental Delay Classroom

       When working with clients with developmental disabilities the main goal is to help them

acquire a functional repertoire. Unfortunately, many people with developmental disabilities often

fail to respond to environmental cues in the same way as typical learners (MacDuff, Krantz, &

McClannahan, 2001). Therefore supplemental stimuli must be used to train skills. These stimuli

are referred to as prompts, stimuli designed to increase the likelihood that a correct response will

occur (Malott, 2008). Therefore added prompts (response prompts) are often used when training

individuals with developmental disabilities.

Response Prompts

       Response prompts are typically presented in a hierarchy, either most-to-least or least-to-

most. Most-to-least prompting (MTL) starts with the most assistance necessary in order to evoke

the desired response to an instruction or the stimulus. Over time, the prompt level is faded until

the individual responds independently to the spoken discriminative stimulus (SD) or to the

naturally occurring stimulus. In most-to-least prompting, the typical hierarchy is as follows: full

physical prompt, partial physical prompt, gestural prompt, and verbal prompt (McConville,

Hantula, & Axelrod, 1998). A full physical prompt consists of providing hand over hand

guidance to the learner in order to make the correct response. A partial physical prompt consists

of gently guiding the learner by the forearm. A gestural prompt consists of the instructor

pointing to the correct stimulus. For example, to begin a training trial, the instructor would

deliver a verbal SD (usually some form of instruction, e.g. “match”) and then immediately

provide a full physical prompt in order for the learner to make a correct response of matching the

correct card. As the fading criterion is met, the prompts are reduced in intensity by using prompts
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 4


lower on the prompt hierarchy until the learner responds independently. The least-to-most

prompting strategy is carried out in the reverse order.

       The least-to-most prompting strategy (LTM) starts by allowing the learner to respond

independently to the instruction or the naturally occurring stimulus. If the learner responds

incorrectly, the instructor then provides a sequence of prompts beginning at the level of least

assistance, escalating to the level of most assistance in order to achieve a correct response. The

least-to-most prompt hierarchy starts with a verbal prompt, then a gestural prompt, partial

physical prompt and finally a full physical prompt (McConville et al., 1998). For example, if the

learner does not respond to the verbal SD within a set period of time (typically 3 to 5 seconds),

then the instructor would deliver a verbal prompt to enable the learner to make a correct

response. The instructor would continue to increase the level of prompt until a correct response is

made. For example, if a learner has the ability to identify colors, but not objects, a least-to-most

training trial would look like this: the instructor would deliver the SD, “touch car” (in this case

the car is blue), if the learner did not respond or responded incorrectly by touching a different

object, the instructor would provide a verbal prompt. Given that the learner can identify colors,

the instructor would say, “the blue one”, as a verbal prompt to touch the blue car. If the learner’s

response was still incorrect, the instructor would continue through the hierarchy as follows:

pointing to the car, followed by gently nudging the learner’s arm toward the car, and finally, if

necessary, providing hand-over-hand assistance to help the learner touch the car when given the

instruction, “touch car”.

       The rationale for this study’s focus on response prompts is due to the heavy use of

response prompts in applied settings. Response prompts are often used instead of stimulus
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 5


prompts because response prompts are easy to train to instructors, easy to implement and do not

require the creation of materials for each target skill (MacDuff et al., 2001).

                        Response Prompting Strategies in Applied Studies

       Response prompting has been in the literature since the 1980s (e.g., Bennett, Gast,

Wolery, & Schuster, 1986; McDonnell, 1987; McDonnell & Ferguson, 1989), however, there is

no general agreement as to which response prompting strategy is the most effective and therefore

should be considered best practice. Two papers have reviewed prompting and fading methods

(Demchak, 1990; MacDuff, Krantz, & McClannahan, 2001). Although Demchak (1990)

provided comparisons of least-to-most and most-to-least response prompting strategies, there

was not enough evidence to make recommendations on which prompting strategies should be

used for learners to acquire skills most effectively and efficiently. The MacDuff et al. (2001)

review provides a summary of different prompting strategies; however; it does not make any

comparisons or provide recommendations as to which prompting strategy should be used.

       Strong support for most-to-least prompting is due to the fact that learners do not make

numerous errors, which allows the learner to engage in stable rates of correct responding

(Luyben et al., 1986; as cited by MacDuff et al., 2001) and it prevents the individual from

making multiple incorrect responses (McConville, Hantula, & Axelrod, 1998). Demchak has

argued that error rate should be taken into consideration in terms of skill acquisition because

error trials take up valuable instructional time and that some learners exhibit problem behavior

following the commitment of errors (1990). However, the use of errorless training runs the risk

of prompts not being faded in a timely manner. This can create prompt dependency, where

learners will not respond until the prompt is presented (MacDuff et al., 2001). Despite possible

drawbacks, the most-to-least prompting strategy has been successful with different disabilities
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 6


and for teaching a variety of skills (Berkowitz, 1990; Hourcade, 1988; McConville, Hantula, &

Axelrod, 1998).

       Unlike MTL, LTM does allow the learner to make errors. The least-to-most prompting

strategy produces more errors than the most-to-least prompting strategy (Libby et al., 2008;

MacDuff et al., 2001; Rubin et al., 2006), but it also allows the learner to respond independently

to instruction. With the opportunity to respond independently, the learner is not given more

assistance than necessary to make a correct response (MacDuff et al., 2001). By not providing a

prompt unless necessary, the instructor does not have to fade assistance as is the case with the

most-to-least prompting strategy (Demchak, 1990). While instructors do not have to fade

prompts in the same way as in most-to-least prompting, prompt dependence can still occur with

least-to-most prompting because a learner may not respond until a prompt is delivered (MacDuff,

et al., 2001). Least-to-most prompting is an effective strategy to teach a variety of skills across

disabilities (Murzynski & Bourret, 2007; Sigafoos, O’Reilly, Seely-York, & Edrisinha, 2004).

                          Comparison of Response Prompting Strategies

Prompt Sequencing: Least-to-Most vs. Most-to Least

       Comparing the effectiveness and efficiency of LTM to MTL was assessed by teaching

four tasks (McConville, Hantula, & Axelrod, 1998). Matching the prompting strategy to the type

of stimuli in the environment resulted in more efficient and effective acquisition of the skill than

when the prompting strategy was unmatched (McConville et al., 1998). For example, when

making a bagged lunch and playing a matching game, the physical properties of the task should

occasion the next response in the behavior chain. When MTL was used (considered matched)

there was not an extra stimulus added to the physical property of the task, thus the relevant

physical properties of the task controlled behavior instead of an extra stimulus prompt. When
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 7


using LTM with these two tasks, the prompt controlled behavior because the verbal prompt

provided occasioned the response instead of the physical properties of the task. Failure in

transfer of stimulus control from the prompt to the stimuli occurring in the natural environment

can lead to prompt dependency. However, when teaching ordering food and asking/answering

questions using LTM (considered matched), in which case the verbal behavior of another person

should occasion a response, the SD in the natural environment is similar to the programmed

prompt (verbal prompt) and thus better stimulus control was established in comparison to using

MTL. This suggests that instead of one prompting strategy being superior to the other, as both

methods did produce skill acquisition; the stimuli in the natural environment that should be

occasioning the response should guide the design of the intervention and programming and a

choice of prompting strategy should be made based on those conditions.

       Two studies (Libby et al., 2008; Rubin, et al., 2006) also compared response prompting

strategies to determine efficiency and effectiveness. Libby et al. (2008) conducted two

experiments in which learners were taught to assemble structures out of Legos®. Study 1

compared MTL to LTM, where three of the five learners acquired the skill faster with LTM,

while two of the five learners only acquired the skill with the MTL condition. In the follow up

experiment, study 2, which compared three conditions (1) MTL, (2) LTM, and (3) to most-to-

least with time delay (MTLD), all three learners acquired the skill faster with LTM; however,

acquisition using the MTLD strategy was nearly as fast. Both MTL and MTLD produced fewer

errors than LTM. In other words MTLD was essentially as efficient as LTM but had fewer

errors. Additionally, all learners made more errors with LTM in both studies.

       The study carried out by Rubin et al. (2006) had similar findings, as three of the learners

learned the skill faster with LTM, while two of the learners not only learned the skill faster with
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 8


MTL, they were unsuccessful in learning the skill with LTM. These three comparison studies did

not warrant the recommending of one prompting strategy being superior to another in terms of

effectiveness or efficiency.

Discussion of Results

       In general, these limited data show that both strategies worked for at least some learners,

although LTM did not work with all learners on all tasks; however, MTL was always effective.

Even when both strategies were effective, there was sometimes a difference in terms of

efficiency. While LTM was consistently more efficient it also produced more errors. The

superiority of one method over the other varied across learners and/or tasks. Additionally, there

was not a consistent pattern for which strategy worked best with a particular type of task. The

majority of procedures included in this review were considered efficacious, with the exception of

Libby and colleagues (2008) and Rubin and colleagues (2006) when least-to-most prompting

was not effective in teaching the skill to some of the learners in the study. Given that almost all

the studies showed that the selected prompting strategies were effective, further analysis is

warranted to determine the superior strategy, which is why studies included some measure of

efficiency. When two different response prompts were compared to one another, both were

equally efficient, even when matched to task type (Table C.1.). Thus, with such inconsistent

findings there is not enough evidence to make any best practice recommendations.

       Most -to-least and least-to-most prompting strategies both have drawbacks as described

previously. The methodology of this study addresses many of those drawbacks for both MTL

and LTM that other studies have not considered. Least-to-most prompting is often criticized for

producing errors and that high error rates can lead to emotional responding (Demchak, 1990). In

an attempt to reduce the probability of emotional responding, both unprompted and prompted
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 9


correct responses were reinforced in LTM as well as MTL. One negative aspect reported about

MTL is that a learner can become prompt dependent if prompts are not faded in a timely and

systematic manner. Additionally, since prompts are faded out from most to least assistance,

learners are sometimes provided with more assistance than necessary to make a correct response.

In order to address both concerns associated with MTL, each session was started with a probe

trial to assess the level of assistance necessary for the learner to make a correct response. This

increases the probability that the learner is not receiving more assistance than necessary and

provides a systematic way to fade prompts. This present set of studies compared, least-to-most

prompting, most-to-least prompting, and most-to-least with a delay strategies in an attempt to

determine the most effective and efficient prompting strategy for skill acquisition. With the first

learner, least-to-most prompting was compared to most-to-least prompting. With the second

learner, least-to-most prompting was compared to most-to-least prompting, which was later

switched to most-to-least prompting with a delay. Finally, with the third learner, least-to-most

prompting was compared to most-to-least with a delay.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 10


                                               Methods

Participants

          Three students, one female and two males, enrolled in an Early Childhood

Developmental Delay (ECDD) classroom at Croyden Avenue School, participated in this study.

The ages of the children at the start of the study were 2 years and 7 months, 3 years and 9

months, and 4 years and 8 months. To participate, the children had to have the prerequisite skills

for matching objects. All three children received discrete trial instruction three hours a day, five

days a week and had been enrolled in the classroom for a range of three to nine months (with an

average of six months) at the start of the study.

Setting

          The author conducted all the sessions in each child’s study carrel, which was

approximately 3.0 x 2.0 m. The carrels contained a desk and two chairs, with the experimenter

sitting perpendicular to the child.

Interobserver Agreement

          Two graduate students collected interobserver data on data sheets identical to the ones

used by the experimenter. Interobserver agreement was taken for twenty-five percent of the

sessions. The percentage of agreement was calculated by dividing the number of agreements by

the number of agreements and disagreements and multiplying by 100. The average percentage of

agreement was 97.62 percent with a range of 86.11 percent to 100 percent.

Materials

          There were two different sets of training materials; one set of materials for each

prompting strategy, LTM and MTL or MTLD. The first set of objects included identical pairs of
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 11


green plastic plates, pink socks, and yellow wooden blocks; the second set included identical

pairs of red plastic cups, blue plastic spoons, and white shoes.

Experimental Design

        A multielement design was used in which LTM and MTL or MTLD conditions were

alternated across days. Because of the heterogeneity of the children in this study, a within-

subject design was chosen so that each child could serve as his or her own control.

Identification of Reinforcers

        Twice a week as part of the regular classroom procedures, children selected eight tangible

items from two large bins of tangibles. Following this selection, the eight tangibles along with

three to four edibles were arranged on the desk in front of the child.   Then a brief multiple-

stimulus assessment without replacement (MSWO) was conducted to identify and rank the top

five of the eleven to twelve preferred items (Higbee, Carr, and Harrison, 2000). At the beginning

of each session the experimenter offered the child the two top-ranked items and used the first one

the child selected as a reinforcer for that session. However, if during the session that item lost its

reinforcing value (e.g. the child pushed it away, did not engage with the tangible or did not eat

the edible) this process was repeated with the two top-ranked items from the remainder of the

array of five.

                                             Procedures

General Procedure

        Sessions were run five days a week, with both procedures being used once a day and at

least twenty minutes between sessions. The order of the most-to-least (MTL) and the least-to-

most (LTM) were alternated across days. Prior to each session of both conditions, LTM probe

trials were conducted.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 12


Probe trials

       In order to ensure that the child did not receive more assistance than needed, a probe trial

was conducted to determine the appropriate prompt level. At the beginning of each session the

experimenter began the probe trial by stating, “Match same,” and allowed the child two to three

seconds to respond independently. If the child did not respond or made an incorrect response,

the experimenter provided the next prompt in the least-to-most prompt hierarchy and again

allowed the child two to three seconds to respond. The experimenter continued up the prompt

hierarchy until the child made a correct response. The prompt hierarchy was a gestural prompt,

partial light physical prompt, partial firm physical prompt, and full physical prompt. The

experimenter reinforced all correct responses, both prompted and unprompted. This process was

repeated for each of the three stimuli in the array before the session began, for both training

strategies, LTM and MTL.

Most-to-Least Prompting

       Most-to-least prompting (MTL). Following the probe trial for each of the three items in

the array, the prompt levels that were determined for each stimulus were used for the remainder

of the session. In order to avoid extinction, a reinforcer was provided for both prompted and

unprompted responses, both in the probe trials and training trials. Even as prompts were faded

over successive sessions, a reinforcer was still provided if a prompt level that was higher than the

probe session was required for a correct response. This was done to address the possibility that

the child could have made a correct independent response due to chance

       The instruction, “match same” was repeated for each prompt. The child was given two to

three seconds to respond before a higher level prompt was used. If the child made an incorrect

response, the object was handed back to him or her while the experimenter simultaneously
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 13


provided a prompt and repeated the instruction until a correct response was made. If the child

did not respond to the instruction, after waiting two to three seconds, the experimenter proceeded

to provide a higher level prompt and repeated the instruction until a correct response was made.

This procedure was the original design; however, some modifications were made along the way;

exceptions to this procedure will be discussed.

Least-to-Most Prompting

       Least-to-most prompting (LTM). A probe trial was conducted for the LTM condition so

that each condition would have the same number of trials. The experimenter began the trial by

stating the instruction, “match same” and allowed the child two to three seconds to respond

independently. The experimenter then proceeded through the least-to-most prompt hierarchy

until the child made a correct response. The experimenter repeated the instruction, “match

same” with every prompt and allowed the child two to three seconds to respond after each

prompt. A reinforcer (the most preferred based on a preference assessment that was conducted

immediately prior to the session) was delivered contingent upon the correct response regardless

of the prompt level.

Instruction, Data Collection, and Mastery Criterion

       After conducting the probe trial for each of the three stimuli in a given set, the training

session began with that set of stimuli using the prompting strategy assigned to that set. Three

additional trials for each stimulus in the set were conducted in random order, again for a total of

twelve trials per session, including the three probe trials. This same method was repeated for the

other set of stimuli using the other prompting strategy, again with a total of twelve trials per

session. The session order for MTL and LTM was alternated across days.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 14


        The data collected for each trial included: (a) the prompt level needed for a correct

response, (b) whether a correct response, incorrect response, or no response was made, and (c)

whether or not any emotional responding occurred. The data collected for each session included:

(a) percentage of errors and (b) total amount of instructional time needed to complete the session.

       The mastery criterion was two consecutive sessions at 89 percent or better or three

consecutive sessions at 75 percent or better; however due to the logistics of the experiment, a

few sessions were run past the mastery criteria for all three children. Finally, the number of

sessions for acquisition was compared between conditions.




                                                                                            Figure

                                                                                            1.




Sample data sheet



                                       Results for Dawson

Experiment 1: Dawson

       For Dawson there was essentially no difference in the rate of acquisition between the

most-to-least and least-to-most prompting procedures. Using the most-to-least prompting

strategy, Dawson met the mastery criterion by session nine (Fig. A.1) and met mastery criterion

by session ten when using least-to-most prompting (Fig. A.2).
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 15


Experiment 2: Stephen

       At the start of the experiment, both the MTL prompting strategy and the LTM prompting

strategy were conducted as described in the general procedures. However, by session 24 Stephen

had not acquired the matching skill using either prompting strategy. Because the classroom’s

policy required that if a child had not acquired a skill within 20 sessions, the procedure would be

modified in some way, therefore the most-to-least prompting strategy was changed to a most-to-

least with delay (MTLD) (Libby et al., 2008).

        The MTLD condition also started with a probe trial for each stimulus and the appropriate

prompt level was determined as described before. Following the probe trials, the experimenter

held up the stimulus and said, “Match same” and gave Stephen two to three seconds to respond

independently. The two to three seconds to respond independently served as the delay in this

procedure. If Stephen did not respond, or responded incorrectly the experimenter provided the

predetermined prompt. All responses were reinforced whether they were independent or

prompted.

                                       Results for Stephen

       Using the least-to-most prompting strategy, Stephen met mastery criterion on session 34

(Fig. A.4). He failed to meet mastery criterion within twenty sessions using MTL, but once the

procedure was switched to MTLD, he met mastery criterion with only 8 more sessions—32

sessions total, suggesting that the addition of the delay to the MTL condition aided in the

acquisition of the matching skill (Fig A.3.). Direct session by session comparisons cannot be

made between the LTM and MTL conditions because the MTL condition was altered during the

experiment. It cannot be determined with certainty whether or not MTLD would have lead to

faster acquisition if it was used from the start of the experiment. It can be argued that MTLD was
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 16


not a catalyst for change but instead the prior training under the basic MTL procedure led to the

skill acquisition. The results suggest that MTLD may be more effective, but the change during

the experiment creates a confound.

Experiment 3: Kiarra

       The least-to-most prompting condition for Kiarra was the same as with the other children.

However, the most-to-least prompting was with a delay (MTLD) as it had been during the last 13

sessions with Stephen because MTLD may have been more effective than simple MTL with

Stephen, The delaying of prompts allowed Kiarra to respond independently thereby eliminating

unnecessary prompts with the goal of preventing prompt dependency.

                                         Results for Kiarra

       While data were taken on whether the children failed to respond or made an incorrect

response for all children in the study, those data are only presented for Kiarra because there was

very little non-responding for the other two children. For the first 11 sessions, Kiarra did not

respond to the initial statement of instruction; however, she always responded correctly to the

second statement of instruction, which was always accompanied by a prompt. By the 11th

session the experimenter noticed that she always waited for a second presentation of the

instruction before responding in both the MTLD and LTM conditions. Therefore from the

twelfth session on, the experimenter presented the second instruction without an accompanying

prompt as needed. And for the next two sessions she responded one hundred percent correct to

the second instruction with no prompting, suggesting that she already mastered the matching

skill under both MTLD and LTM conditions (Fig. A.5 and Fig. A.6.). It is possible that she had

mastered the matching skill more quickly with one procedure than the other, but this cannot be
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 17


ascertained because she always waited for the second presentation of the instruction and that

second presentation was always accompanied by a possibly unnecessary prompt.

Experiment 4: Generalized Matching

       Matching (matching to sample) is a common skill taught to children with developmental

delays in early intensive behavioral interventions. However, mastery of that skill will be of little

value to the child if that child has not acquired a generalized matching repertoire, so that he or

she can use that skill in a variety of educational and practical contexts where the task involves of

novel or untrained stimuli. Therefore, following the main experiments, the children’s

generalized matching repertoires were assessed with regard to different types of novel objects

and two-dimensional stimuli.

       Generalized Identical-Object Matching. When a child met a mastery criterion for object

matching, the child was tested for a generalized identical-object matching repertoire. To assess

this generalized repertoire, a child was presented with a set of three novel comparison objects,

then handed a sample object that matched one of the comparison objects and told, “Match same.”

This was done three times with each of the three comparison objects, for a total of nine trials.

Then the procedure was repeated with three different, novel objects. This testing was done in

extinction. All three children performed above the mastery criterion (80 percent correct) when

presented with the six novel identical objects, indicating that all three had acquired a generalized

identical-object-matching repertoire (Fig B.1.).

       Generalized Similar-Object-Matching. Then the same testing procedure was used with

non-identical, but similar, objects. Both Stephen and Kiarra performed above mastery criterion

when presented with similar non-identical objects indicating a generalized similar-object-
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 18


matching repertoire. While Dawson did not meet the mastery criterion, he still demonstrated

better than chance performance, suggesting some generalization.

       Generalized Simple Picture Matching. Then the same testing procedure was used with

identical, simple, two-dimensional pictures (pictures consisting of a single large shape). The

cards were 4 inches wide by six inches high. For logistical reasons, only Kiarra was tested, and

she met mastery criterion.

       Generalized Complex Picture Matching. Finally, the same testing procedure was used

with Kiarra, with identical, complex, two-dimensional pictures (pictures consisting of four or

five smaller shapes). The cards were 4 inches wide by six inches high. Again, Kiarra met

mastery criterion (Fig B.1).

       While Stephen and Dawson were not tested for a generalized two-dimensional picture

repertoire, Stephen completed the simple and complex matching procedures prior to leaving the

classroom and did so with no difficulty during either procedure. Dawson completed the simple

and complex matching procedures and three additional two-dimensional matching procedures as

part of his curriculum and did so without any difficulty. For all participants, the acquisition of

six identical objects lead to a generalized identical-matching repertoire and a generalized two-

dimensional matching repertoire for one of the participants.

The Reinforcement of Prompted Responses and Elimination of Emotional Responding

       Prompted responses were reinforced in both experimental conditions. This component

was done because of the observation of emotional responding by children in the classroom when

those children made several errors in a row and thus experienced low rates of reinforcers.

Furthermore, one child, Dawson, had a history of emotional responding prior to the start of this

study. Emotional responding included, crying, whining, swiping instructional materials and
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 19


slapping his face. After this study, he was in the classroom for one year and nine months and

had had over ten procedures that had either been removed from his curriculum or had to be

modified because of emotional responding that occurred within those procedures. However, in

the present experiment, the reinforcement of prompted responses meant that he always received a

high rate of reinforcers; and in this experiment, he emitted almost no emotional responding. In

addition, reinforcing prompted responses during other procedures outside of this experiment was

also associated with the elimination of emotional responding with those procedures. Stephen also

demonstrated emotional responding prior to this study when experiencing low rates of

reinforcement. Stephen had near zero levels of emotional responding during this study. While

Kiarra did not demonstrate high rates of emotional responding prior to this study, she did not

exhibit any emotional responding during the study. This suggests that tutors should reinforce

prompted responses if a child has a history of emotional responding during instruction.

                                        General Discussion

       This research demonstrated no differences between the most-to-least and least-to-most

prompting procedures for any of the three children. In Dawson’s case there clearly was no

difference. However, for Stephen it is not clear what would have happened, if most-to-least-with-

delay had been used from the first session. And for Kiarra, it is not clear whether differences

would have been obtained between most-to-least-with-delay and least-to-most if no prompt had

been used with the second instruction on each trial and instead, the prompt had been saved for a

third instruction on each trial as needed. But, for Kiarra, the number of sessions to mastery

would have been small in either case.

       The failure to demonstrate a difference between the two prompting procedures may be

due to acquisition of generalized matching. In other words, the learning of matching with one
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 20


prompting procedure may have generalized to and facilitated the learning of matching with the

other procedure, with the result that the children learned matching more rapidly with the less

effective procedure than they would have if they had not been acquiring generalized matching

with the more effective procedure. Or, in fact, there may be no real differences in the

effectiveness of the two prompting procedures; only further research will tell.

         Even though it was not part of the original research design, the experimenter assessed

what level of generalized matching was obtained after the children mastered the six original

objects. All three children demonstrated a generalized three-dimensional matching repertoire

and Kiarra also demonstrated a generalized two-dimensional repertoire following training on the

six original objects (Fig. B.1.) Due to this potential interaction, future research should target

skills that do not allow for a generalized skill set that would transfer from one condition to the

other. Additionally, all children had near zero rates of emotional responding during training

sessions, suggesting that reinforcing prompted responses may have reduced emotional

responding in comparison to what has been observed in the classroom where the study took

place.

                                            Conclusions

         The least-to-most and least to most-to-least prompting strategies were equally effective in

teaching the identical matching; and there was no demonstrated difference between the two

strategies in terms of efficiency and the lack of difference might have been due to an interaction

between the two conditions in this multielement design. However, this study did demonstrate

that generalized, identical, three-dimensional matching can reliably result from matching training

with as few as six three-dimensional objects; and, at least in some cases, such training can

produce generalized matching between similar, non-identical objects and both simple and
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 21


complex identical pictures. Furthermore, this study strongly suggested that reinforcing prompted

responses can essentially eliminated emotional responding for children who tend to respond

emotionally when prompted responses are not reinforced.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 22



                                               References


Berkowitz, S. (1990). A comparison of two methods of prompting in training discrimination of

       communication book pictures by autistic students. Journal of Autism and Developmental

       Disorders, 20(2), 255-262.

Demchak, M. (1990). Response prompting and fading methods: A review. American Journal on Mental

       Retardation, 94(6), 603-615.

Doyle, P. M., Wolery, M., Gast, D. C., Ault, M. J., & Wiley, K. (1990). Comparison of constant time

       delay and the system of least prompts in teaching preschoolers with developmental delays.

       Research in Developmental Disabilities, 11(1), 1-22.

Higbee, T.S., Carr, J.E., Harrison, C.D. (2000). Further evaluation of the multiple-stimulus preference

       assessment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 61-73.

Libby, M.E., Weiss, J.S., Bancroft, S., & Ahearn, W. H. (2008). A comparison of most-to-least and

       least-to-most prompting on the acquisition of solitary play skills. Behavior Analysis in Practice,

       1, 37-43.

MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (2001). Prompts and prompt-fading strategies for

       people with autism. In C. Maurice, G. Green, & R. M. Foxx (Eds.), Making a difference:

       Behavioral intervention for autism (37- 50). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

Malott, R.W. (2008). Principles of Behavior (5th ed. ). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

McConville, M. L., Hantula, D. A., & Axelrod, S. (1998). Matching training procedures to outcomes: A

       behavioral and quantitative analysis. Behavior Modification, 22(3), 391-414.

Murzynski, N. T., & Bourret, J. C. (2007). Combining video modeling and least-to-most prompting for

       establishing response chains. Behavioral Interventions, 22, 147-152.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 23


Rubin, S., Fuqua, W., Johnson, D. A., & Miguel, C. F. (2006). Evaluating increasing and decreasing

       prompt hierarchies with developmentally disable adults. Unpublished Manuscript, Western

       Michigan University.

Sigafoos, J., O'Reilly, M., Seely-York, S., & Edrisinha, C. (2004). Teaching students with

       developmental disabilities to locate their AAC device. Research in Developmental Disabilities,

       25, 371-383.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 24


                                                                                    Appendix A.

                                                       Most-to-Least and Least-to-Most Prompting for all Children


                                 100

                                  90

                                  80
            Percentage Correct




                                  70

                                  60

                                  50

                                  40

                                  30

                                  20

                                  10

                                       0
                                               1   2    3   4   5   6       7     8      9    10     11     12    13    14    15    16
                                                                                  Session

                                                                        Percent correct out of 9 trials




Figure A.1. This graph depicts Dawson’s performance when using most-to-least
        prompting.


                                 100

                                 90

                                 80

                                 70
   Percentage Correct




                                 60

                                 50

                                 40

                                 30

                                 20

                                 10

                                  0
                                           1       2    3   4   5   6       7      8     9     10     11     12    13    14    15    16
                                                                                  Session
                                                                     Percent correct out of 12 trials




Figure A.2. This graph depicts Dawson’s performance when using least-to-most
        prompting.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 25


                                      Appendix A. Continued

Dawson:

       When interpreting the graphs it is important to note that in the most-to-least condition,

Dawson’s percentage correct was calculated by the number of correct independent responses out

of the number of opportunities to independently respond. If Dawson required a prompt during

the probe trial for a particular object, then every subsequent trial for that object was presented

simultaneously with a prompt, thus there was not an opportunity for independent responding.

Because data were analyzed in this way, the percentage correct for most-to-least prompting

cannot be directly compared session by session to the least-to-most prompting strategy; however,

the number of sessions to criterion can be compared. During Dawson’s six post-mastery sessions

the percentage correct generally remained above criterion for both conditions. There were a few

sessions under both the LTM and MTL condition that the percentage correct dropped below

mastery criterion level. A molecular analysis of the data showed that Dawson had a few trials

where he did not respond until a prompt was presented, but did not respond incorrectly.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 26


                                                               Appendix A. Continued




Figure A.3. This graph depicts Stephen’s performance when using most-to-least
        prompting.




                     100

                     90

                     80

                     70
   Percent Correct




                     60

                     50

                     40

                     30

                     20

                     10

                      0
                           1   3   5   7   9   11   13    15    17    19   21   23    25    27   29   31   33   35   37
                                                                     Session

                                                         Percent correct out of 12 trials




Figure A.4. This graph depicts Sam’s performance when using least-to-most
        prompting.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 27

                                                         Appendix A. Continued

Stephen:
                     In the most-to-least condition, sessions 1 to 24, the percentage correct was calculated

by the number of correct independent responses out of the number of opportunities to

independently respond as with Dawson. Therefore a session by session comparison cannot be

made until session 25. Comparisons can be made from session 25 on when most-to-least with a

delay was used because each trial, because each trial always started with the opportunity to

respond independently regardless of the prompt level needed during the probe.



                  100%


                  90%


                  80%


                  70%
Percent correct




                  60%


                  50%


                  40%


                  30%


                  20%


                  10%


                   0%
                         1      2     3      4      5     6     7      8      9       10   11    12    13       14   15
                                                                    Session
                             Percent correct out of 12 trials                     Percentage of nonresponding




Figure A.5. This graph depicts Kiarra’s performance when using most-to-least with delayed prompting.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 28


                                                         Appendix A. Continued


                     100%

                      90%

                      80%

                      70%
   Percent Correct




                      60%

                      50%

                      40%

                      30%

                      20%

                      10%

                       0%
                            1     2     3      4     5     6   7   8    9      10   11   12   13    14      15   16
                                                                   Session

                            Percent correct out of 12 trials                  Percentage of nonresponding



Figure A.6. This graph depicts Kiarra’s performance when using least-to-most prompting.

Kiarra:

                     The most-to-least with delay prompting condition always started with the child having an

opportunity to respond independently. The percentage correct was calculated by the number of

correct independent responses out of the number of opportunities to respond independently. This

method of calculation allowed the most-to-least-with-delay condition to be directly compared to

the least-to-most condition in terms of a session to session comparison as well as total number of

sessions to criterion.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 29


                                              Appendix B.


                                     Generalized Matching Testing




       Figure B.1. This graph depicts all of the children’s performance when tested for a generalized
matching repertoire.
A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 30
Appendix C.

                                                Summary of Response Prompting Comparison Studies


Table C.1.
Comparison Studies Involving Response Prompting Strategies
                    Treatment
Author(s) (year)    Comparisons                     Skills taught                  Participants                 Effectiveness*         Efficiency
                                                                                   Study 1: 5 participants
                                                                                   from private residential     Study 1: 3 participants acquire the skills
                                                                                   school for children with     faster with LTM, 2 faster with MTL (did
                                                                                   autism or related            not make the structure with LTM). All
                                                                                   disability.                  participants made more errors with LTM.
                                                                                   Study 2: 3 participants      Study 2: LTM was faster for all, but MTLD
                    Study 1: MTL vs. LTM.                                          from the same private        was nearly as rapid. MTL and MTLD had
Libby, Weiss,                                                                      residential school for       fewer errors.
Bancroft, &         Study 2: MTL vs. LTM vs.        Lego structure                 children with autism or      MTLD was just as efficient and produced
Ahern (2008)        MTLD                                                           related disability.          fewer errors.
                                                    (a) bagged lunch
                    LTM vs. MTL (following          (b) playing a matching game
                    the fading of full physical a   (c) asking/answering social                                                        LTM when matched
McConville,         5 second time delay was         questions with peers           3 females, 15-18yrs. old                            to task type, MTL
Hantula, &          introduced                      (d) ordering food at a         with mild to moderate                               when matched to task
Axelrod (1998)                                      restaurant                     retardation                  Equal                  type
                                                                                                                For two
                                                                                                                participants LTM
                                                                                                                was not effective.     For two participants
                                                                                                                For three              MTL was more
                                                                                                                participants LTM       efficient.
Rubin, Fuqua,                                                                                                   was effective.         For three participants
Johnson, &                                                                         5 males with                 MTL was effective      LTM was more
Miguel (2006)       MTL vs. LTM                     Matching 2D pictures           developmental disabilities   for all participants   efficient.

*The prompting strategy was considered effective if the learner acquired the skill.

Weitere ähnliche Inhalte

Was ist angesagt?

Is it True? Always? Supporting Reasoning and Proof Focused Collaboration amon...
Is it True? Always? Supporting Reasoning and Proof Focused Collaboration amon...Is it True? Always? Supporting Reasoning and Proof Focused Collaboration amon...
Is it True? Always? Supporting Reasoning and Proof Focused Collaboration amon...Nicole Rigelman
 
An Experimental Study of Collaborative Instructional Strategy (CIS) for Teac...
An Experimental Study of Collaborative Instructional Strategy (CIS) for  Teac...An Experimental Study of Collaborative Instructional Strategy (CIS) for  Teac...
An Experimental Study of Collaborative Instructional Strategy (CIS) for Teac...Dr.Nasir Ahmad
 
Research on the effect of applying
Research on the effect of applyingResearch on the effect of applying
Research on the effect of applyingijcsit
 
PEShare.co.uk Shared Resource
PEShare.co.uk Shared ResourcePEShare.co.uk Shared Resource
PEShare.co.uk Shared Resourcepeshare.co.uk
 
Recommendation Report
Recommendation ReportRecommendation Report
Recommendation Reportjhollon
 
Transfer of Learning
Transfer of LearningTransfer of Learning
Transfer of LearningEzr Acelar
 
Power Point Presentation Gagne's hierarchy of learning
Power Point Presentation Gagne's hierarchy of learningPower Point Presentation Gagne's hierarchy of learning
Power Point Presentation Gagne's hierarchy of learningmumthazmaharoof
 
TRANSFER OF LEARNING by Lorraine Anoran
TRANSFER OF LEARNING by Lorraine AnoranTRANSFER OF LEARNING by Lorraine Anoran
TRANSFER OF LEARNING by Lorraine AnoranLorraine Mae Anoran
 
The effectiveness of programmed instruction
The effectiveness of programmed instructionThe effectiveness of programmed instruction
The effectiveness of programmed instructionpubali khan
 
Gestalt, Guthrie, and Tolman Theory to Education
Gestalt, Guthrie, and Tolman Theory to EducationGestalt, Guthrie, and Tolman Theory to Education
Gestalt, Guthrie, and Tolman Theory to Educationrentao16
 

Was ist angesagt? (20)

Transfer of learning
Transfer of learningTransfer of learning
Transfer of learning
 
Gagne's Conditions of Learning
Gagne's Conditions of LearningGagne's Conditions of Learning
Gagne's Conditions of Learning
 
Is it True? Always? Supporting Reasoning and Proof Focused Collaboration amon...
Is it True? Always? Supporting Reasoning and Proof Focused Collaboration amon...Is it True? Always? Supporting Reasoning and Proof Focused Collaboration amon...
Is it True? Always? Supporting Reasoning and Proof Focused Collaboration amon...
 
An Experimental Study of Collaborative Instructional Strategy (CIS) for Teac...
An Experimental Study of Collaborative Instructional Strategy (CIS) for  Teac...An Experimental Study of Collaborative Instructional Strategy (CIS) for  Teac...
An Experimental Study of Collaborative Instructional Strategy (CIS) for Teac...
 
Transfer of Learning
Transfer of LearningTransfer of Learning
Transfer of Learning
 
Research on the effect of applying
Research on the effect of applyingResearch on the effect of applying
Research on the effect of applying
 
PEShare.co.uk Shared Resource
PEShare.co.uk Shared ResourcePEShare.co.uk Shared Resource
PEShare.co.uk Shared Resource
 
Recommendation Report
Recommendation ReportRecommendation Report
Recommendation Report
 
Gagne
GagneGagne
Gagne
 
Developmental Psychology 2do Bimestre
Developmental Psychology 2do BimestreDevelopmental Psychology 2do Bimestre
Developmental Psychology 2do Bimestre
 
Classroom Behaviour Problems:
Classroom Behaviour Problems:Classroom Behaviour Problems:
Classroom Behaviour Problems:
 
programmed learning
programmed learningprogrammed learning
programmed learning
 
Transfer of Learning
Transfer of LearningTransfer of Learning
Transfer of Learning
 
Power Point Presentation Gagne's hierarchy of learning
Power Point Presentation Gagne's hierarchy of learningPower Point Presentation Gagne's hierarchy of learning
Power Point Presentation Gagne's hierarchy of learning
 
TRANSFER OF LEARNING by Lorraine Anoran
TRANSFER OF LEARNING by Lorraine AnoranTRANSFER OF LEARNING by Lorraine Anoran
TRANSFER OF LEARNING by Lorraine Anoran
 
Abstract presentation
Abstract presentationAbstract presentation
Abstract presentation
 
The effectiveness of programmed instruction
The effectiveness of programmed instructionThe effectiveness of programmed instruction
The effectiveness of programmed instruction
 
Gagnes levels of learning
Gagnes levels of learningGagnes levels of learning
Gagnes levels of learning
 
Cumulative learning theory
Cumulative learning theoryCumulative learning theory
Cumulative learning theory
 
Gestalt, Guthrie, and Tolman Theory to Education
Gestalt, Guthrie, and Tolman Theory to EducationGestalt, Guthrie, and Tolman Theory to Education
Gestalt, Guthrie, and Tolman Theory to Education
 

Andere mochten auch

Andere mochten auch (20)

Sped 614 Assistive Tech
Sped 614 Assistive TechSped 614 Assistive Tech
Sped 614 Assistive Tech
 
Trabajo de gerencia de riesgos
Trabajo de gerencia de riesgosTrabajo de gerencia de riesgos
Trabajo de gerencia de riesgos
 
Sistema oseo
Sistema oseoSistema oseo
Sistema oseo
 
Conceptos básicos de software
Conceptos básicos de softwareConceptos básicos de software
Conceptos básicos de software
 
Lozano j gabriel_act2
Lozano j gabriel_act2Lozano j gabriel_act2
Lozano j gabriel_act2
 
Pause Legacy: Chapter 1
Pause Legacy: Chapter 1Pause Legacy: Chapter 1
Pause Legacy: Chapter 1
 
Delportvervestvanheck Emj April 2004
Delportvervestvanheck   Emj April 2004Delportvervestvanheck   Emj April 2004
Delportvervestvanheck Emj April 2004
 
Maximilian Kolbe
Maximilian KolbeMaximilian Kolbe
Maximilian Kolbe
 
Informática secretaria da saúde
Informática    secretaria da saúdeInformática    secretaria da saúde
Informática secretaria da saúde
 
Software
SoftwareSoftware
Software
 
Deporte.!
Deporte.!Deporte.!
Deporte.!
 
Luz marina alvarez_ud_cierocci
Luz marina alvarez_ud_cierocciLuz marina alvarez_ud_cierocci
Luz marina alvarez_ud_cierocci
 
Caderno de orientações didáticas matemática
Caderno de orientações didáticas   matemáticaCaderno de orientações didáticas   matemática
Caderno de orientações didáticas matemática
 
Qué aporta la ere a los estudiantes
Qué aporta la ere a los estudiantesQué aporta la ere a los estudiantes
Qué aporta la ere a los estudiantes
 
R000000000005 uj
R000000000005 ujR000000000005 uj
R000000000005 uj
 
Relatorio3
Relatorio3Relatorio3
Relatorio3
 
Leprosy
LeprosyLeprosy
Leprosy
 
Busiess1
Busiess1Busiess1
Busiess1
 
Colombia pair pobre
Colombia pair pobreColombia pair pobre
Colombia pair pobre
 
Mental Focus and Spirituality for Healing
Mental Focus and Spirituality for HealingMental Focus and Spirituality for Healing
Mental Focus and Spirituality for Healing
 

Ähnlich wie Kristen.gaisford.thesis.complete.version (1)

response to intervention.ppt
response to intervention.pptresponse to intervention.ppt
response to intervention.pptJanet Van Heck
 
Running head DIRECT INSTRUCTION12DIRECT INSTRUCTION.docx
Running head DIRECT INSTRUCTION12DIRECT INSTRUCTION.docxRunning head DIRECT INSTRUCTION12DIRECT INSTRUCTION.docx
Running head DIRECT INSTRUCTION12DIRECT INSTRUCTION.docxsusanschei
 
Teaching metacognition
Teaching metacognitionTeaching metacognition
Teaching metacognitionmsksteel
 
Response 1Compare the use of Response to Intervention (RTI)
Response 1Compare the use of Response to Intervention (RTI) Response 1Compare the use of Response to Intervention (RTI)
Response 1Compare the use of Response to Intervention (RTI) mickietanger
 
Embry & Biglan "Evidence Based Kernels" Review paper 2008
Embry & Biglan "Evidence Based Kernels" Review paper 2008Embry & Biglan "Evidence Based Kernels" Review paper 2008
Embry & Biglan "Evidence Based Kernels" Review paper 2008Dennis Embry
 
A Summary Of Nine Key Studies Multi-Tier Intervention And Response To Interv...
A Summary Of Nine Key Studies  Multi-Tier Intervention And Response To Interv...A Summary Of Nine Key Studies  Multi-Tier Intervention And Response To Interv...
A Summary Of Nine Key Studies Multi-Tier Intervention And Response To Interv...Lori Moore
 
School Based Mindfulness Interventions Within a Response to Intervention Fram...
School Based Mindfulness Interventions Within a Response to Intervention Fram...School Based Mindfulness Interventions Within a Response to Intervention Fram...
School Based Mindfulness Interventions Within a Response to Intervention Fram...Innovations2Solutions
 
IntroductionIt is the purpose of this manual to teach instruction.docx
IntroductionIt is the purpose of this manual to teach instruction.docxIntroductionIt is the purpose of this manual to teach instruction.docx
IntroductionIt is the purpose of this manual to teach instruction.docxmariuse18nolet
 
LEE 215 - Term Paper - Guided Reading
LEE 215 - Term Paper - Guided ReadingLEE 215 - Term Paper - Guided Reading
LEE 215 - Term Paper - Guided ReadingRichard Santos
 
CEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
CEN Launch, Oliver HulmeCEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
CEN Launch, Oliver HulmeYishay Mor
 
CEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
CEN Launch, Oliver HulmeCEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
CEN Launch, Oliver HulmeYishay Mor
 
· Write in paragraph format (no lists, bullets, or numbers).· .docx
· Write in paragraph format (no lists, bullets, or numbers).· .docx· Write in paragraph format (no lists, bullets, or numbers).· .docx
· Write in paragraph format (no lists, bullets, or numbers).· .docxLynellBull52
 
Maestro en servicio revision de videos
Maestro en servicio revision de videosMaestro en servicio revision de videos
Maestro en servicio revision de videosSisercom SAC
 
Improving Students’ Learning With Effective Learning Techniques: Promising ...
Improving Students’ Learning With  Effective Learning Techniques: Promising  ...Improving Students’ Learning With  Effective Learning Techniques: Promising  ...
Improving Students’ Learning With Effective Learning Techniques: Promising ...Fran Maciel
 
A Comparison Of The Mystery Motivator And The Get Em On Task Interventions F...
A Comparison Of The Mystery Motivator And The Get  Em On Task Interventions F...A Comparison Of The Mystery Motivator And The Get  Em On Task Interventions F...
A Comparison Of The Mystery Motivator And The Get Em On Task Interventions F...Addison Coleman
 
Identifying Effective Math Interventions for Early Elementary Students.docx
Identifying Effective Math Interventions for Early Elementary Students.docxIdentifying Effective Math Interventions for Early Elementary Students.docx
Identifying Effective Math Interventions for Early Elementary Students.docxwrite4
 
httpsdoi.org10.11771053451219837634Intervention in Sc
httpsdoi.org10.11771053451219837634Intervention in Schttpsdoi.org10.11771053451219837634Intervention in Sc
httpsdoi.org10.11771053451219837634Intervention in ScPazSilviapm
 
Teaching Math To Sld Ld Guide
Teaching Math To Sld Ld GuideTeaching Math To Sld Ld Guide
Teaching Math To Sld Ld Guidedmloch
 

Ähnlich wie Kristen.gaisford.thesis.complete.version (1) (20)

response to intervention.ppt
response to intervention.pptresponse to intervention.ppt
response to intervention.ppt
 
Running head DIRECT INSTRUCTION12DIRECT INSTRUCTION.docx
Running head DIRECT INSTRUCTION12DIRECT INSTRUCTION.docxRunning head DIRECT INSTRUCTION12DIRECT INSTRUCTION.docx
Running head DIRECT INSTRUCTION12DIRECT INSTRUCTION.docx
 
Teaching metacognition
Teaching metacognitionTeaching metacognition
Teaching metacognition
 
Response 1Compare the use of Response to Intervention (RTI)
Response 1Compare the use of Response to Intervention (RTI) Response 1Compare the use of Response to Intervention (RTI)
Response 1Compare the use of Response to Intervention (RTI)
 
Embry & Biglan "Evidence Based Kernels" Review paper 2008
Embry & Biglan "Evidence Based Kernels" Review paper 2008Embry & Biglan "Evidence Based Kernels" Review paper 2008
Embry & Biglan "Evidence Based Kernels" Review paper 2008
 
A Summary Of Nine Key Studies Multi-Tier Intervention And Response To Interv...
A Summary Of Nine Key Studies  Multi-Tier Intervention And Response To Interv...A Summary Of Nine Key Studies  Multi-Tier Intervention And Response To Interv...
A Summary Of Nine Key Studies Multi-Tier Intervention And Response To Interv...
 
WEEK 5 ULOb.docx
WEEK 5 ULOb.docxWEEK 5 ULOb.docx
WEEK 5 ULOb.docx
 
School Based Mindfulness Interventions Within a Response to Intervention Fram...
School Based Mindfulness Interventions Within a Response to Intervention Fram...School Based Mindfulness Interventions Within a Response to Intervention Fram...
School Based Mindfulness Interventions Within a Response to Intervention Fram...
 
IntroductionIt is the purpose of this manual to teach instruction.docx
IntroductionIt is the purpose of this manual to teach instruction.docxIntroductionIt is the purpose of this manual to teach instruction.docx
IntroductionIt is the purpose of this manual to teach instruction.docx
 
LEE 215 - Term Paper - Guided Reading
LEE 215 - Term Paper - Guided ReadingLEE 215 - Term Paper - Guided Reading
LEE 215 - Term Paper - Guided Reading
 
CEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
CEN Launch, Oliver HulmeCEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
CEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
 
CEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
CEN Launch, Oliver HulmeCEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
CEN Launch, Oliver Hulme
 
· Write in paragraph format (no lists, bullets, or numbers).· .docx
· Write in paragraph format (no lists, bullets, or numbers).· .docx· Write in paragraph format (no lists, bullets, or numbers).· .docx
· Write in paragraph format (no lists, bullets, or numbers).· .docx
 
Maestro en servicio revision de videos
Maestro en servicio revision de videosMaestro en servicio revision de videos
Maestro en servicio revision de videos
 
Improving Students’ Learning With Effective Learning Techniques: Promising ...
Improving Students’ Learning With  Effective Learning Techniques: Promising  ...Improving Students’ Learning With  Effective Learning Techniques: Promising  ...
Improving Students’ Learning With Effective Learning Techniques: Promising ...
 
A Comparison Of The Mystery Motivator And The Get Em On Task Interventions F...
A Comparison Of The Mystery Motivator And The Get  Em On Task Interventions F...A Comparison Of The Mystery Motivator And The Get  Em On Task Interventions F...
A Comparison Of The Mystery Motivator And The Get Em On Task Interventions F...
 
Identifying Effective Math Interventions for Early Elementary Students.docx
Identifying Effective Math Interventions for Early Elementary Students.docxIdentifying Effective Math Interventions for Early Elementary Students.docx
Identifying Effective Math Interventions for Early Elementary Students.docx
 
httpsdoi.org10.11771053451219837634Intervention in Sc
httpsdoi.org10.11771053451219837634Intervention in Schttpsdoi.org10.11771053451219837634Intervention in Sc
httpsdoi.org10.11771053451219837634Intervention in Sc
 
teaching habit
teaching habitteaching habit
teaching habit
 
Teaching Math To Sld Ld Guide
Teaching Math To Sld Ld GuideTeaching Math To Sld Ld Guide
Teaching Math To Sld Ld Guide
 

Mehr von Behavior Analysis Training System, WMU

Mehr von Behavior Analysis Training System, WMU (20)

Final fiesta
Final fiesta Final fiesta
Final fiesta
 
Techy Tots - Final Fiesta
Techy Tots - Final FiestaTechy Tots - Final Fiesta
Techy Tots - Final Fiesta
 
Final fiesta
Final fiestaFinal fiesta
Final fiesta
 
Stella's Surrender
Stella's SurrenderStella's Surrender
Stella's Surrender
 
Stranger Things: A Behavioral Analysis
Stranger Things: A Behavioral AnalysisStranger Things: A Behavioral Analysis
Stranger Things: A Behavioral Analysis
 
Contingencies of Kingdom Hearts
Contingencies of Kingdom HeartsContingencies of Kingdom Hearts
Contingencies of Kingdom Hearts
 
Squidward's Contingencies
Squidward's ContingenciesSquidward's Contingencies
Squidward's Contingencies
 
Behavior Analysis with Lily the Beagle
Behavior Analysis with Lily the BeagleBehavior Analysis with Lily the Beagle
Behavior Analysis with Lily the Beagle
 
How To Get Your Boyfriend To Clean
How To Get Your Boyfriend To CleanHow To Get Your Boyfriend To Clean
How To Get Your Boyfriend To Clean
 
Online Professionalism and Monitoring ppt
Online Professionalism and Monitoring pptOnline Professionalism and Monitoring ppt
Online Professionalism and Monitoring ppt
 
Dead Man Test Blues
Dead Man Test BluesDead Man Test Blues
Dead Man Test Blues
 
Structured Play
Structured PlayStructured Play
Structured Play
 
Discrimination
DiscriminationDiscrimination
Discrimination
 
Icon Exchange Assignment #2: Distance and Persistence
Icon Exchange Assignment #2: Distance and PersistenceIcon Exchange Assignment #2: Distance and Persistence
Icon Exchange Assignment #2: Distance and Persistence
 
Icon Exchange Assignment #1: Enticing, Prompting, and Motivation
Icon Exchange Assignment #1: Enticing, Prompting, and MotivationIcon Exchange Assignment #1: Enticing, Prompting, and Motivation
Icon Exchange Assignment #1: Enticing, Prompting, and Motivation
 
Motivating Operations
Motivating OperationsMotivating Operations
Motivating Operations
 
Fall 2014 syllabus
Fall 2014 syllabusFall 2014 syllabus
Fall 2014 syllabus
 
Icon Exchange The Basics Phase 2-5
Icon Exchange The Basics Phase 2-5Icon Exchange The Basics Phase 2-5
Icon Exchange The Basics Phase 2-5
 
Consequences
ConsequencesConsequences
Consequences
 
ELOs
ELOsELOs
ELOs
 

Kristen.gaisford.thesis.complete.version (1)

  • 1. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 1 Running Head: A COMPARISON OF PROMPTING STRATEGIES A Comparison of Prompting Strategies in an Early Childhood Developmental Delay Classroom Kristen Lynn Gaisford Western Michigan University
  • 2. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 2 Abstract The design of the current study was to compare two prompting strategies; most to least prompting (MTL) and least to most prompting (LTM). These two strategies were compared using a multi-element design, assessing the performance of three children, ranging from 2 to 4 years of age. These children were selected from a classroom that provides services to children with Early Childhood Developmental Delays (ECDD). In order to compare the MTL and LTM strategies, the three children were taught identical three-dimensional matching. There was no demonstrated difference between strategies in terms of effectiveness or efficiency. While it was not the focus of the study, generalized matching data are also presented for all three children. All three children acquired a generalized identical-object matching repertoire. Additionally, two of the children had a history of emotional responding prior to this study, yet displayed almost no emotional responding during the study, suggesting that the reinforcement of prompted responses was the reason for the absence of emotional responding. The study took place at Croyden Avenue School, in Kalamazoo, Michigan.
  • 3. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 3 A Comparison of Prompting Strategies in an Early Childhood Developmental Delay Classroom When working with clients with developmental disabilities the main goal is to help them acquire a functional repertoire. Unfortunately, many people with developmental disabilities often fail to respond to environmental cues in the same way as typical learners (MacDuff, Krantz, & McClannahan, 2001). Therefore supplemental stimuli must be used to train skills. These stimuli are referred to as prompts, stimuli designed to increase the likelihood that a correct response will occur (Malott, 2008). Therefore added prompts (response prompts) are often used when training individuals with developmental disabilities. Response Prompts Response prompts are typically presented in a hierarchy, either most-to-least or least-to- most. Most-to-least prompting (MTL) starts with the most assistance necessary in order to evoke the desired response to an instruction or the stimulus. Over time, the prompt level is faded until the individual responds independently to the spoken discriminative stimulus (SD) or to the naturally occurring stimulus. In most-to-least prompting, the typical hierarchy is as follows: full physical prompt, partial physical prompt, gestural prompt, and verbal prompt (McConville, Hantula, & Axelrod, 1998). A full physical prompt consists of providing hand over hand guidance to the learner in order to make the correct response. A partial physical prompt consists of gently guiding the learner by the forearm. A gestural prompt consists of the instructor pointing to the correct stimulus. For example, to begin a training trial, the instructor would deliver a verbal SD (usually some form of instruction, e.g. “match”) and then immediately provide a full physical prompt in order for the learner to make a correct response of matching the correct card. As the fading criterion is met, the prompts are reduced in intensity by using prompts
  • 4. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 4 lower on the prompt hierarchy until the learner responds independently. The least-to-most prompting strategy is carried out in the reverse order. The least-to-most prompting strategy (LTM) starts by allowing the learner to respond independently to the instruction or the naturally occurring stimulus. If the learner responds incorrectly, the instructor then provides a sequence of prompts beginning at the level of least assistance, escalating to the level of most assistance in order to achieve a correct response. The least-to-most prompt hierarchy starts with a verbal prompt, then a gestural prompt, partial physical prompt and finally a full physical prompt (McConville et al., 1998). For example, if the learner does not respond to the verbal SD within a set period of time (typically 3 to 5 seconds), then the instructor would deliver a verbal prompt to enable the learner to make a correct response. The instructor would continue to increase the level of prompt until a correct response is made. For example, if a learner has the ability to identify colors, but not objects, a least-to-most training trial would look like this: the instructor would deliver the SD, “touch car” (in this case the car is blue), if the learner did not respond or responded incorrectly by touching a different object, the instructor would provide a verbal prompt. Given that the learner can identify colors, the instructor would say, “the blue one”, as a verbal prompt to touch the blue car. If the learner’s response was still incorrect, the instructor would continue through the hierarchy as follows: pointing to the car, followed by gently nudging the learner’s arm toward the car, and finally, if necessary, providing hand-over-hand assistance to help the learner touch the car when given the instruction, “touch car”. The rationale for this study’s focus on response prompts is due to the heavy use of response prompts in applied settings. Response prompts are often used instead of stimulus
  • 5. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 5 prompts because response prompts are easy to train to instructors, easy to implement and do not require the creation of materials for each target skill (MacDuff et al., 2001). Response Prompting Strategies in Applied Studies Response prompting has been in the literature since the 1980s (e.g., Bennett, Gast, Wolery, & Schuster, 1986; McDonnell, 1987; McDonnell & Ferguson, 1989), however, there is no general agreement as to which response prompting strategy is the most effective and therefore should be considered best practice. Two papers have reviewed prompting and fading methods (Demchak, 1990; MacDuff, Krantz, & McClannahan, 2001). Although Demchak (1990) provided comparisons of least-to-most and most-to-least response prompting strategies, there was not enough evidence to make recommendations on which prompting strategies should be used for learners to acquire skills most effectively and efficiently. The MacDuff et al. (2001) review provides a summary of different prompting strategies; however; it does not make any comparisons or provide recommendations as to which prompting strategy should be used. Strong support for most-to-least prompting is due to the fact that learners do not make numerous errors, which allows the learner to engage in stable rates of correct responding (Luyben et al., 1986; as cited by MacDuff et al., 2001) and it prevents the individual from making multiple incorrect responses (McConville, Hantula, & Axelrod, 1998). Demchak has argued that error rate should be taken into consideration in terms of skill acquisition because error trials take up valuable instructional time and that some learners exhibit problem behavior following the commitment of errors (1990). However, the use of errorless training runs the risk of prompts not being faded in a timely manner. This can create prompt dependency, where learners will not respond until the prompt is presented (MacDuff et al., 2001). Despite possible drawbacks, the most-to-least prompting strategy has been successful with different disabilities
  • 6. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 6 and for teaching a variety of skills (Berkowitz, 1990; Hourcade, 1988; McConville, Hantula, & Axelrod, 1998). Unlike MTL, LTM does allow the learner to make errors. The least-to-most prompting strategy produces more errors than the most-to-least prompting strategy (Libby et al., 2008; MacDuff et al., 2001; Rubin et al., 2006), but it also allows the learner to respond independently to instruction. With the opportunity to respond independently, the learner is not given more assistance than necessary to make a correct response (MacDuff et al., 2001). By not providing a prompt unless necessary, the instructor does not have to fade assistance as is the case with the most-to-least prompting strategy (Demchak, 1990). While instructors do not have to fade prompts in the same way as in most-to-least prompting, prompt dependence can still occur with least-to-most prompting because a learner may not respond until a prompt is delivered (MacDuff, et al., 2001). Least-to-most prompting is an effective strategy to teach a variety of skills across disabilities (Murzynski & Bourret, 2007; Sigafoos, O’Reilly, Seely-York, & Edrisinha, 2004). Comparison of Response Prompting Strategies Prompt Sequencing: Least-to-Most vs. Most-to Least Comparing the effectiveness and efficiency of LTM to MTL was assessed by teaching four tasks (McConville, Hantula, & Axelrod, 1998). Matching the prompting strategy to the type of stimuli in the environment resulted in more efficient and effective acquisition of the skill than when the prompting strategy was unmatched (McConville et al., 1998). For example, when making a bagged lunch and playing a matching game, the physical properties of the task should occasion the next response in the behavior chain. When MTL was used (considered matched) there was not an extra stimulus added to the physical property of the task, thus the relevant physical properties of the task controlled behavior instead of an extra stimulus prompt. When
  • 7. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 7 using LTM with these two tasks, the prompt controlled behavior because the verbal prompt provided occasioned the response instead of the physical properties of the task. Failure in transfer of stimulus control from the prompt to the stimuli occurring in the natural environment can lead to prompt dependency. However, when teaching ordering food and asking/answering questions using LTM (considered matched), in which case the verbal behavior of another person should occasion a response, the SD in the natural environment is similar to the programmed prompt (verbal prompt) and thus better stimulus control was established in comparison to using MTL. This suggests that instead of one prompting strategy being superior to the other, as both methods did produce skill acquisition; the stimuli in the natural environment that should be occasioning the response should guide the design of the intervention and programming and a choice of prompting strategy should be made based on those conditions. Two studies (Libby et al., 2008; Rubin, et al., 2006) also compared response prompting strategies to determine efficiency and effectiveness. Libby et al. (2008) conducted two experiments in which learners were taught to assemble structures out of Legos®. Study 1 compared MTL to LTM, where three of the five learners acquired the skill faster with LTM, while two of the five learners only acquired the skill with the MTL condition. In the follow up experiment, study 2, which compared three conditions (1) MTL, (2) LTM, and (3) to most-to- least with time delay (MTLD), all three learners acquired the skill faster with LTM; however, acquisition using the MTLD strategy was nearly as fast. Both MTL and MTLD produced fewer errors than LTM. In other words MTLD was essentially as efficient as LTM but had fewer errors. Additionally, all learners made more errors with LTM in both studies. The study carried out by Rubin et al. (2006) had similar findings, as three of the learners learned the skill faster with LTM, while two of the learners not only learned the skill faster with
  • 8. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 8 MTL, they were unsuccessful in learning the skill with LTM. These three comparison studies did not warrant the recommending of one prompting strategy being superior to another in terms of effectiveness or efficiency. Discussion of Results In general, these limited data show that both strategies worked for at least some learners, although LTM did not work with all learners on all tasks; however, MTL was always effective. Even when both strategies were effective, there was sometimes a difference in terms of efficiency. While LTM was consistently more efficient it also produced more errors. The superiority of one method over the other varied across learners and/or tasks. Additionally, there was not a consistent pattern for which strategy worked best with a particular type of task. The majority of procedures included in this review were considered efficacious, with the exception of Libby and colleagues (2008) and Rubin and colleagues (2006) when least-to-most prompting was not effective in teaching the skill to some of the learners in the study. Given that almost all the studies showed that the selected prompting strategies were effective, further analysis is warranted to determine the superior strategy, which is why studies included some measure of efficiency. When two different response prompts were compared to one another, both were equally efficient, even when matched to task type (Table C.1.). Thus, with such inconsistent findings there is not enough evidence to make any best practice recommendations. Most -to-least and least-to-most prompting strategies both have drawbacks as described previously. The methodology of this study addresses many of those drawbacks for both MTL and LTM that other studies have not considered. Least-to-most prompting is often criticized for producing errors and that high error rates can lead to emotional responding (Demchak, 1990). In an attempt to reduce the probability of emotional responding, both unprompted and prompted
  • 9. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 9 correct responses were reinforced in LTM as well as MTL. One negative aspect reported about MTL is that a learner can become prompt dependent if prompts are not faded in a timely and systematic manner. Additionally, since prompts are faded out from most to least assistance, learners are sometimes provided with more assistance than necessary to make a correct response. In order to address both concerns associated with MTL, each session was started with a probe trial to assess the level of assistance necessary for the learner to make a correct response. This increases the probability that the learner is not receiving more assistance than necessary and provides a systematic way to fade prompts. This present set of studies compared, least-to-most prompting, most-to-least prompting, and most-to-least with a delay strategies in an attempt to determine the most effective and efficient prompting strategy for skill acquisition. With the first learner, least-to-most prompting was compared to most-to-least prompting. With the second learner, least-to-most prompting was compared to most-to-least prompting, which was later switched to most-to-least prompting with a delay. Finally, with the third learner, least-to-most prompting was compared to most-to-least with a delay.
  • 10. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 10 Methods Participants Three students, one female and two males, enrolled in an Early Childhood Developmental Delay (ECDD) classroom at Croyden Avenue School, participated in this study. The ages of the children at the start of the study were 2 years and 7 months, 3 years and 9 months, and 4 years and 8 months. To participate, the children had to have the prerequisite skills for matching objects. All three children received discrete trial instruction three hours a day, five days a week and had been enrolled in the classroom for a range of three to nine months (with an average of six months) at the start of the study. Setting The author conducted all the sessions in each child’s study carrel, which was approximately 3.0 x 2.0 m. The carrels contained a desk and two chairs, with the experimenter sitting perpendicular to the child. Interobserver Agreement Two graduate students collected interobserver data on data sheets identical to the ones used by the experimenter. Interobserver agreement was taken for twenty-five percent of the sessions. The percentage of agreement was calculated by dividing the number of agreements by the number of agreements and disagreements and multiplying by 100. The average percentage of agreement was 97.62 percent with a range of 86.11 percent to 100 percent. Materials There were two different sets of training materials; one set of materials for each prompting strategy, LTM and MTL or MTLD. The first set of objects included identical pairs of
  • 11. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 11 green plastic plates, pink socks, and yellow wooden blocks; the second set included identical pairs of red plastic cups, blue plastic spoons, and white shoes. Experimental Design A multielement design was used in which LTM and MTL or MTLD conditions were alternated across days. Because of the heterogeneity of the children in this study, a within- subject design was chosen so that each child could serve as his or her own control. Identification of Reinforcers Twice a week as part of the regular classroom procedures, children selected eight tangible items from two large bins of tangibles. Following this selection, the eight tangibles along with three to four edibles were arranged on the desk in front of the child. Then a brief multiple- stimulus assessment without replacement (MSWO) was conducted to identify and rank the top five of the eleven to twelve preferred items (Higbee, Carr, and Harrison, 2000). At the beginning of each session the experimenter offered the child the two top-ranked items and used the first one the child selected as a reinforcer for that session. However, if during the session that item lost its reinforcing value (e.g. the child pushed it away, did not engage with the tangible or did not eat the edible) this process was repeated with the two top-ranked items from the remainder of the array of five. Procedures General Procedure Sessions were run five days a week, with both procedures being used once a day and at least twenty minutes between sessions. The order of the most-to-least (MTL) and the least-to- most (LTM) were alternated across days. Prior to each session of both conditions, LTM probe trials were conducted.
  • 12. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 12 Probe trials In order to ensure that the child did not receive more assistance than needed, a probe trial was conducted to determine the appropriate prompt level. At the beginning of each session the experimenter began the probe trial by stating, “Match same,” and allowed the child two to three seconds to respond independently. If the child did not respond or made an incorrect response, the experimenter provided the next prompt in the least-to-most prompt hierarchy and again allowed the child two to three seconds to respond. The experimenter continued up the prompt hierarchy until the child made a correct response. The prompt hierarchy was a gestural prompt, partial light physical prompt, partial firm physical prompt, and full physical prompt. The experimenter reinforced all correct responses, both prompted and unprompted. This process was repeated for each of the three stimuli in the array before the session began, for both training strategies, LTM and MTL. Most-to-Least Prompting Most-to-least prompting (MTL). Following the probe trial for each of the three items in the array, the prompt levels that were determined for each stimulus were used for the remainder of the session. In order to avoid extinction, a reinforcer was provided for both prompted and unprompted responses, both in the probe trials and training trials. Even as prompts were faded over successive sessions, a reinforcer was still provided if a prompt level that was higher than the probe session was required for a correct response. This was done to address the possibility that the child could have made a correct independent response due to chance The instruction, “match same” was repeated for each prompt. The child was given two to three seconds to respond before a higher level prompt was used. If the child made an incorrect response, the object was handed back to him or her while the experimenter simultaneously
  • 13. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 13 provided a prompt and repeated the instruction until a correct response was made. If the child did not respond to the instruction, after waiting two to three seconds, the experimenter proceeded to provide a higher level prompt and repeated the instruction until a correct response was made. This procedure was the original design; however, some modifications were made along the way; exceptions to this procedure will be discussed. Least-to-Most Prompting Least-to-most prompting (LTM). A probe trial was conducted for the LTM condition so that each condition would have the same number of trials. The experimenter began the trial by stating the instruction, “match same” and allowed the child two to three seconds to respond independently. The experimenter then proceeded through the least-to-most prompt hierarchy until the child made a correct response. The experimenter repeated the instruction, “match same” with every prompt and allowed the child two to three seconds to respond after each prompt. A reinforcer (the most preferred based on a preference assessment that was conducted immediately prior to the session) was delivered contingent upon the correct response regardless of the prompt level. Instruction, Data Collection, and Mastery Criterion After conducting the probe trial for each of the three stimuli in a given set, the training session began with that set of stimuli using the prompting strategy assigned to that set. Three additional trials for each stimulus in the set were conducted in random order, again for a total of twelve trials per session, including the three probe trials. This same method was repeated for the other set of stimuli using the other prompting strategy, again with a total of twelve trials per session. The session order for MTL and LTM was alternated across days.
  • 14. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 14 The data collected for each trial included: (a) the prompt level needed for a correct response, (b) whether a correct response, incorrect response, or no response was made, and (c) whether or not any emotional responding occurred. The data collected for each session included: (a) percentage of errors and (b) total amount of instructional time needed to complete the session. The mastery criterion was two consecutive sessions at 89 percent or better or three consecutive sessions at 75 percent or better; however due to the logistics of the experiment, a few sessions were run past the mastery criteria for all three children. Finally, the number of sessions for acquisition was compared between conditions. Figure 1. Sample data sheet Results for Dawson Experiment 1: Dawson For Dawson there was essentially no difference in the rate of acquisition between the most-to-least and least-to-most prompting procedures. Using the most-to-least prompting strategy, Dawson met the mastery criterion by session nine (Fig. A.1) and met mastery criterion by session ten when using least-to-most prompting (Fig. A.2).
  • 15. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 15 Experiment 2: Stephen At the start of the experiment, both the MTL prompting strategy and the LTM prompting strategy were conducted as described in the general procedures. However, by session 24 Stephen had not acquired the matching skill using either prompting strategy. Because the classroom’s policy required that if a child had not acquired a skill within 20 sessions, the procedure would be modified in some way, therefore the most-to-least prompting strategy was changed to a most-to- least with delay (MTLD) (Libby et al., 2008). The MTLD condition also started with a probe trial for each stimulus and the appropriate prompt level was determined as described before. Following the probe trials, the experimenter held up the stimulus and said, “Match same” and gave Stephen two to three seconds to respond independently. The two to three seconds to respond independently served as the delay in this procedure. If Stephen did not respond, or responded incorrectly the experimenter provided the predetermined prompt. All responses were reinforced whether they were independent or prompted. Results for Stephen Using the least-to-most prompting strategy, Stephen met mastery criterion on session 34 (Fig. A.4). He failed to meet mastery criterion within twenty sessions using MTL, but once the procedure was switched to MTLD, he met mastery criterion with only 8 more sessions—32 sessions total, suggesting that the addition of the delay to the MTL condition aided in the acquisition of the matching skill (Fig A.3.). Direct session by session comparisons cannot be made between the LTM and MTL conditions because the MTL condition was altered during the experiment. It cannot be determined with certainty whether or not MTLD would have lead to faster acquisition if it was used from the start of the experiment. It can be argued that MTLD was
  • 16. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 16 not a catalyst for change but instead the prior training under the basic MTL procedure led to the skill acquisition. The results suggest that MTLD may be more effective, but the change during the experiment creates a confound. Experiment 3: Kiarra The least-to-most prompting condition for Kiarra was the same as with the other children. However, the most-to-least prompting was with a delay (MTLD) as it had been during the last 13 sessions with Stephen because MTLD may have been more effective than simple MTL with Stephen, The delaying of prompts allowed Kiarra to respond independently thereby eliminating unnecessary prompts with the goal of preventing prompt dependency. Results for Kiarra While data were taken on whether the children failed to respond or made an incorrect response for all children in the study, those data are only presented for Kiarra because there was very little non-responding for the other two children. For the first 11 sessions, Kiarra did not respond to the initial statement of instruction; however, she always responded correctly to the second statement of instruction, which was always accompanied by a prompt. By the 11th session the experimenter noticed that she always waited for a second presentation of the instruction before responding in both the MTLD and LTM conditions. Therefore from the twelfth session on, the experimenter presented the second instruction without an accompanying prompt as needed. And for the next two sessions she responded one hundred percent correct to the second instruction with no prompting, suggesting that she already mastered the matching skill under both MTLD and LTM conditions (Fig. A.5 and Fig. A.6.). It is possible that she had mastered the matching skill more quickly with one procedure than the other, but this cannot be
  • 17. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 17 ascertained because she always waited for the second presentation of the instruction and that second presentation was always accompanied by a possibly unnecessary prompt. Experiment 4: Generalized Matching Matching (matching to sample) is a common skill taught to children with developmental delays in early intensive behavioral interventions. However, mastery of that skill will be of little value to the child if that child has not acquired a generalized matching repertoire, so that he or she can use that skill in a variety of educational and practical contexts where the task involves of novel or untrained stimuli. Therefore, following the main experiments, the children’s generalized matching repertoires were assessed with regard to different types of novel objects and two-dimensional stimuli. Generalized Identical-Object Matching. When a child met a mastery criterion for object matching, the child was tested for a generalized identical-object matching repertoire. To assess this generalized repertoire, a child was presented with a set of three novel comparison objects, then handed a sample object that matched one of the comparison objects and told, “Match same.” This was done three times with each of the three comparison objects, for a total of nine trials. Then the procedure was repeated with three different, novel objects. This testing was done in extinction. All three children performed above the mastery criterion (80 percent correct) when presented with the six novel identical objects, indicating that all three had acquired a generalized identical-object-matching repertoire (Fig B.1.). Generalized Similar-Object-Matching. Then the same testing procedure was used with non-identical, but similar, objects. Both Stephen and Kiarra performed above mastery criterion when presented with similar non-identical objects indicating a generalized similar-object-
  • 18. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 18 matching repertoire. While Dawson did not meet the mastery criterion, he still demonstrated better than chance performance, suggesting some generalization. Generalized Simple Picture Matching. Then the same testing procedure was used with identical, simple, two-dimensional pictures (pictures consisting of a single large shape). The cards were 4 inches wide by six inches high. For logistical reasons, only Kiarra was tested, and she met mastery criterion. Generalized Complex Picture Matching. Finally, the same testing procedure was used with Kiarra, with identical, complex, two-dimensional pictures (pictures consisting of four or five smaller shapes). The cards were 4 inches wide by six inches high. Again, Kiarra met mastery criterion (Fig B.1). While Stephen and Dawson were not tested for a generalized two-dimensional picture repertoire, Stephen completed the simple and complex matching procedures prior to leaving the classroom and did so with no difficulty during either procedure. Dawson completed the simple and complex matching procedures and three additional two-dimensional matching procedures as part of his curriculum and did so without any difficulty. For all participants, the acquisition of six identical objects lead to a generalized identical-matching repertoire and a generalized two- dimensional matching repertoire for one of the participants. The Reinforcement of Prompted Responses and Elimination of Emotional Responding Prompted responses were reinforced in both experimental conditions. This component was done because of the observation of emotional responding by children in the classroom when those children made several errors in a row and thus experienced low rates of reinforcers. Furthermore, one child, Dawson, had a history of emotional responding prior to the start of this study. Emotional responding included, crying, whining, swiping instructional materials and
  • 19. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 19 slapping his face. After this study, he was in the classroom for one year and nine months and had had over ten procedures that had either been removed from his curriculum or had to be modified because of emotional responding that occurred within those procedures. However, in the present experiment, the reinforcement of prompted responses meant that he always received a high rate of reinforcers; and in this experiment, he emitted almost no emotional responding. In addition, reinforcing prompted responses during other procedures outside of this experiment was also associated with the elimination of emotional responding with those procedures. Stephen also demonstrated emotional responding prior to this study when experiencing low rates of reinforcement. Stephen had near zero levels of emotional responding during this study. While Kiarra did not demonstrate high rates of emotional responding prior to this study, she did not exhibit any emotional responding during the study. This suggests that tutors should reinforce prompted responses if a child has a history of emotional responding during instruction. General Discussion This research demonstrated no differences between the most-to-least and least-to-most prompting procedures for any of the three children. In Dawson’s case there clearly was no difference. However, for Stephen it is not clear what would have happened, if most-to-least-with- delay had been used from the first session. And for Kiarra, it is not clear whether differences would have been obtained between most-to-least-with-delay and least-to-most if no prompt had been used with the second instruction on each trial and instead, the prompt had been saved for a third instruction on each trial as needed. But, for Kiarra, the number of sessions to mastery would have been small in either case. The failure to demonstrate a difference between the two prompting procedures may be due to acquisition of generalized matching. In other words, the learning of matching with one
  • 20. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 20 prompting procedure may have generalized to and facilitated the learning of matching with the other procedure, with the result that the children learned matching more rapidly with the less effective procedure than they would have if they had not been acquiring generalized matching with the more effective procedure. Or, in fact, there may be no real differences in the effectiveness of the two prompting procedures; only further research will tell. Even though it was not part of the original research design, the experimenter assessed what level of generalized matching was obtained after the children mastered the six original objects. All three children demonstrated a generalized three-dimensional matching repertoire and Kiarra also demonstrated a generalized two-dimensional repertoire following training on the six original objects (Fig. B.1.) Due to this potential interaction, future research should target skills that do not allow for a generalized skill set that would transfer from one condition to the other. Additionally, all children had near zero rates of emotional responding during training sessions, suggesting that reinforcing prompted responses may have reduced emotional responding in comparison to what has been observed in the classroom where the study took place. Conclusions The least-to-most and least to most-to-least prompting strategies were equally effective in teaching the identical matching; and there was no demonstrated difference between the two strategies in terms of efficiency and the lack of difference might have been due to an interaction between the two conditions in this multielement design. However, this study did demonstrate that generalized, identical, three-dimensional matching can reliably result from matching training with as few as six three-dimensional objects; and, at least in some cases, such training can produce generalized matching between similar, non-identical objects and both simple and
  • 21. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 21 complex identical pictures. Furthermore, this study strongly suggested that reinforcing prompted responses can essentially eliminated emotional responding for children who tend to respond emotionally when prompted responses are not reinforced.
  • 22. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 22 References Berkowitz, S. (1990). A comparison of two methods of prompting in training discrimination of communication book pictures by autistic students. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(2), 255-262. Demchak, M. (1990). Response prompting and fading methods: A review. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 94(6), 603-615. Doyle, P. M., Wolery, M., Gast, D. C., Ault, M. J., & Wiley, K. (1990). Comparison of constant time delay and the system of least prompts in teaching preschoolers with developmental delays. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 11(1), 1-22. Higbee, T.S., Carr, J.E., Harrison, C.D. (2000). Further evaluation of the multiple-stimulus preference assessment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 61-73. Libby, M.E., Weiss, J.S., Bancroft, S., & Ahearn, W. H. (2008). A comparison of most-to-least and least-to-most prompting on the acquisition of solitary play skills. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 37-43. MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (2001). Prompts and prompt-fading strategies for people with autism. In C. Maurice, G. Green, & R. M. Foxx (Eds.), Making a difference: Behavioral intervention for autism (37- 50). Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Malott, R.W. (2008). Principles of Behavior (5th ed. ). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. McConville, M. L., Hantula, D. A., & Axelrod, S. (1998). Matching training procedures to outcomes: A behavioral and quantitative analysis. Behavior Modification, 22(3), 391-414. Murzynski, N. T., & Bourret, J. C. (2007). Combining video modeling and least-to-most prompting for establishing response chains. Behavioral Interventions, 22, 147-152.
  • 23. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 23 Rubin, S., Fuqua, W., Johnson, D. A., & Miguel, C. F. (2006). Evaluating increasing and decreasing prompt hierarchies with developmentally disable adults. Unpublished Manuscript, Western Michigan University. Sigafoos, J., O'Reilly, M., Seely-York, S., & Edrisinha, C. (2004). Teaching students with developmental disabilities to locate their AAC device. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25, 371-383.
  • 24. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 24 Appendix A. Most-to-Least and Least-to-Most Prompting for all Children 100 90 80 Percentage Correct 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Session Percent correct out of 9 trials Figure A.1. This graph depicts Dawson’s performance when using most-to-least prompting. 100 90 80 70 Percentage Correct 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Session Percent correct out of 12 trials Figure A.2. This graph depicts Dawson’s performance when using least-to-most prompting.
  • 25. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 25 Appendix A. Continued Dawson: When interpreting the graphs it is important to note that in the most-to-least condition, Dawson’s percentage correct was calculated by the number of correct independent responses out of the number of opportunities to independently respond. If Dawson required a prompt during the probe trial for a particular object, then every subsequent trial for that object was presented simultaneously with a prompt, thus there was not an opportunity for independent responding. Because data were analyzed in this way, the percentage correct for most-to-least prompting cannot be directly compared session by session to the least-to-most prompting strategy; however, the number of sessions to criterion can be compared. During Dawson’s six post-mastery sessions the percentage correct generally remained above criterion for both conditions. There were a few sessions under both the LTM and MTL condition that the percentage correct dropped below mastery criterion level. A molecular analysis of the data showed that Dawson had a few trials where he did not respond until a prompt was presented, but did not respond incorrectly.
  • 26. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 26 Appendix A. Continued Figure A.3. This graph depicts Stephen’s performance when using most-to-least prompting. 100 90 80 70 Percent Correct 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 29 31 33 35 37 Session Percent correct out of 12 trials Figure A.4. This graph depicts Sam’s performance when using least-to-most prompting.
  • 27. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 27 Appendix A. Continued Stephen: In the most-to-least condition, sessions 1 to 24, the percentage correct was calculated by the number of correct independent responses out of the number of opportunities to independently respond as with Dawson. Therefore a session by session comparison cannot be made until session 25. Comparisons can be made from session 25 on when most-to-least with a delay was used because each trial, because each trial always started with the opportunity to respond independently regardless of the prompt level needed during the probe. 100% 90% 80% 70% Percent correct 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Session Percent correct out of 12 trials Percentage of nonresponding Figure A.5. This graph depicts Kiarra’s performance when using most-to-least with delayed prompting.
  • 28. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 28 Appendix A. Continued 100% 90% 80% 70% Percent Correct 60% 50% 40% 30% 20% 10% 0% 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Session Percent correct out of 12 trials Percentage of nonresponding Figure A.6. This graph depicts Kiarra’s performance when using least-to-most prompting. Kiarra: The most-to-least with delay prompting condition always started with the child having an opportunity to respond independently. The percentage correct was calculated by the number of correct independent responses out of the number of opportunities to respond independently. This method of calculation allowed the most-to-least-with-delay condition to be directly compared to the least-to-most condition in terms of a session to session comparison as well as total number of sessions to criterion.
  • 29. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 29 Appendix B. Generalized Matching Testing Figure B.1. This graph depicts all of the children’s performance when tested for a generalized matching repertoire.
  • 30. A Comparison of Prompting Strategies 30
  • 31. Appendix C. Summary of Response Prompting Comparison Studies Table C.1. Comparison Studies Involving Response Prompting Strategies Treatment Author(s) (year) Comparisons Skills taught Participants Effectiveness* Efficiency Study 1: 5 participants from private residential Study 1: 3 participants acquire the skills school for children with faster with LTM, 2 faster with MTL (did autism or related not make the structure with LTM). All disability. participants made more errors with LTM. Study 2: 3 participants Study 2: LTM was faster for all, but MTLD Study 1: MTL vs. LTM. from the same private was nearly as rapid. MTL and MTLD had Libby, Weiss, residential school for fewer errors. Bancroft, & Study 2: MTL vs. LTM vs. Lego structure children with autism or MTLD was just as efficient and produced Ahern (2008) MTLD related disability. fewer errors. (a) bagged lunch LTM vs. MTL (following (b) playing a matching game the fading of full physical a (c) asking/answering social LTM when matched McConville, 5 second time delay was questions with peers 3 females, 15-18yrs. old to task type, MTL Hantula, & introduced (d) ordering food at a with mild to moderate when matched to task Axelrod (1998) restaurant retardation Equal type For two participants LTM was not effective. For two participants For three MTL was more participants LTM efficient. Rubin, Fuqua, was effective. For three participants Johnson, & 5 males with MTL was effective LTM was more Miguel (2006) MTL vs. LTM Matching 2D pictures developmental disabilities for all participants efficient. *The prompting strategy was considered effective if the learner acquired the skill.